View More View Less
  • 1 Pécsi Tudományegyetem Pszichológia Intézet Pécs
Restricted access

Purchase article

USD  $25.00

1 year subscription (Individual Only)

USD  $204.00

A közös figyelem kialakulása jelentős mérföldkő a csecsemő társas kapcsolatainak világában. Az utóbbi évtized intenzív kutatása feltárta a közös figyelem csecsemő- és kisgyermekkori fejlődési szakaszait és szerepét a később kibontakozó társas kompetencia területén. Jelen tanulmánynak kettős célja van; egyrészt összefoglalást kíván adni a közös figyelmi képesség fejlődéséről és szerepéről a társas képességek kialakulásában, másrészt áttekintjük a közös figyelem tipikustól eltérő fejlődésmenetét. A kora gyermekkorban megmutatkozó közös figyelemi zavar több, később manifesztálódó pszichopatológiai állapot előzményének tekinthető. Legpregnánsabban az autizmusspektrum-zavarban jelentkezik, de más szociális kommunikációs zavarnál és kapcsolati problémáknál is megfigyelhető az elvárttól eltérő megjelenési forma.

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interactions. Child Development, 55, 1278–1289.

  • Baldwin, D. A. (1995). Understanding the link between joint attention and language. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 131–158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.

  • Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111–127.

  • Block, J. (2005). Infant joint attention, attachment and cognitive/language outcomes in 24 month-old preschool children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL.

  • Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255– 287.

  • Capps, L., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1994). Attachment security in children with autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 29–261.

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social Cognition, Joint Attention, and Communicative Competence from 9 to 15 Month of Age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4): i–vi, 1–143.

  • Claussen, A., Mundy, P., Mallik, S., & Willoughby, J. (2002). Joint attention and disorganized attachment status in at risk infants. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 279–292.

  • Corriveau, K. H., Harris, P. L., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Arnott, B., Elliott, L. et al. (2009). Young children’s trust in their mother’s claims: Longitudinal links with attachment security in infancy. Child Development, 80, 750–761.

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.

  • Drew, A., Baird, G., Taylor, E., Milne, E., & Charman, T. (2007). The Social Communication Assessment for Toddlers with Autism (SCATA): an instrument to measure the frequency, form and function of communication in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 648–666.

  • DSM-IV: American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.), Washington, DC: APA.

  • Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 581–604.

  • Gergely Gy., Egyed K. és Király I. (2007). A természetes pedagógiáról. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle, 62(1), 107–125.

  • Goldsmith, D. F., & Rogoff, B. (1997). Mothers’ and toddlers’ coordinated joint focus of attention: Variations with maternal dysphoric symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 113–119.

  • Hámori E. (2013). Rizikófaktorok, adaptáció és reziliencia a korai fejlődésben – A koraszülöttség a fejlődési pszichopatológia modelljében. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle, 68(1), 7–22.

  • Hobson, J. A., & Hobson, R. P. (2007). Identification: The missing link between joint attention and imitation? Development and Psychopathology, 19, 411–431.

  • Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching reciprocal imitation skills to young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 488–505.

  • Jellema, T., Baker, C. I., Wicker, B., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of hand actions. Brain and Cognition, 44, 280–302.

  • Jones, E. A., Carr, E. G., & Feeley, K. M. (2006). Multiple effects of joint attention intervention for children with autism. Behavior Modification, 30, 782–834.

  • Koegel, R. L., Koegel, L. K., & Brookman L. I. (2003). Empirically supported privotal response interventions for children with autism. In A. E. Kazdin, & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychoterapies for children and adolescents (pp. 341–355). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Kroupina, M., Kuefner, D., Iverson, S., & Johnson, D. (2003). Joint attention skills of post-institutionalized children. Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Tampa, FL.

  • Krstovska-Guerrero, I., & Jones, E. A. (2013). Joint attention in autism: Teaching smiling coordinated with gaze to respond to joint attention bids. Research in Autism Disorders, 7, 93–108.

  • Leekam, S. R., Hunnisett, E., & Moore, C. (1998). Targets and cues: Gaze-following in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Applied Disciplines, 39(7), 951–962.

  • Leekam, S. R., López, B., & Moore, C., (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 261–273.

  • Legerstee, M., Varghese, J., & van Beek, Y. (2002). Effects of maintaining and redirecting infant attention on the production of referential communication in infants with and without Down syndrome. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 71–82.

  • Lord, C., Floody, H., Anderson, D., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social engagement in very young children with autism: Differences across context. Paper presented at the meeting of. the Society for Research in Child Development. Tampa, FL.

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C. et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorder, 30, 205–223.

  • Mars, A. E., Mauk, J. E., & Dowrick, P. W. (1998). Symptoms of pervasive developmental disorders as observed in prediagnostic home videos of infants and toddlers. Journal of. Pediatry, 132, 500–504.

  • Meindl, J. N., & Cannella-Malone, H. I. (2011). Initiating and responding to joint attention bids in children with autism: A review of the literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 1441–1454.

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, Ch., Arnott, B., Vittorini, L., Turner, M., Leekam, S., & Parkinson, K. (2011). Individual Differences in Infants’ Joint Attention Behaviors With Mother and a New Social Partner. Infancy, 16, 587–610.

  • Morales, M., Mundy, P., Crowson, M., Neal, R., & Delgado, C. (2005). Individual differences in infants attention skills, joint attention, and emotion regulation behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 259–263.

  • Mundy, P. (2003). The neural basis of social impairments in autism: The role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44, 793–809.

  • Mundy, P. C., & Acra, C. F. (2006). Joint Attention, Social Engagement, and the Development of Social Competence In P. J. Marshall, & N. A. Fox (Eds.), The Development of. Social Engagement: Neurobiological Perspectives (pp. 81–117). New York, US: Oxford University Press.

  • Mundy, P., & Newell, L. (2007). Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269–274.

  • Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. A. (2000). EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 325–338.

  • Nichols, K. E., Fox, N. A., & Mundy, P. (2005). Joint attention, self-recognition, and neurocognitive functioning. Infancy, 7, 35–51.

  • Olafsen, K. S., Ronning, J. A., Kaaresen, P. I., Ulvund, S. E., Handegard, B. H., & Dahl, L. B. (2006). Joint attention in term and preterm infants at 12 months corrected age: The significance of gender and intervention based on a randomized controlled trial. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 554–563.

  • Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. (2002). Early recognition of one year old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation: A study of first birthday party home videotapes. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–252.

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the human brain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Roach, M. A., Barratt, S., M., Miller, J. F., & Leavitt, L. A. (1998). The structure of mother–child play: Young children with Down syndrome and typically developing children. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 77–87.

  • Schertz, H. H., & Odom, S. L. (2007). Promoting joint attention in toddlers with autism: A parent-mediated developmental model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1562–1575.

  • Seibert, J. M., Hogan, A. E., & Mundy, P. C. (1982). Assessing interactional competencies: The early social-communication scales. Infant Mental Health Journal, 3(4), 244–258.

  • Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals. Current Biology, 18, 668–671.

  • Sigman, M., Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64(1), 109–113.

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 77–89.

  • Stefanik K., Győri M., Kanizsai-Nagy I., Sajó E., Várnai Zs. és Balázs A. (2007). Az autizmus spektrumzavarok diagnózisa a klinikumban és a kutatásban: az ADI-R és az ADOS eljárások magyar adaptációjának részeredményei. In Racsmány M. (szerk.), A fejlődés zavarai és vizsgálómódszerei (pp. 171–190). Budapest: Akadémia Kiadó.

  • Stern, D. (2002). A csecsemő személyközi világa. Budapest: Animula.

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore, & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

  • Torkildsen, J., Thormodsen, R., Syvensen, G., Smith, L., & Lingren, M. (2008). Brain correlates of nonverbal communicative comprehension in 20–24 month olds. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies. Vancouver, BC, Canada.

  • Toth, K., Munson, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Dawson, G. (2006). Early predictors of communication development in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Joint attention, imitation, and toy play. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 993–1005.

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and Cooperation in Early Infancy: A Description of Primary Intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication (pp. 321–348). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Vaughan, A., Mundy, P., Block, J., Burnette, C., Delgado, C., Gomez, Y., et al. (2003). Child, caregiver, and temperament contributions to infant joint attention. Infancy, 4, 603–616.

  • Vaughan, A., van Hecke, A., Mundy, P., Block, J. J., Delgado, C. E. F., Parlade, M. V., et al. (2012). Infant responding to joint attention, executive process and self-regulation in preschool children. Infant Behavior and Development, 35, 303–311.

  • Venezia, M., Messinger, D., Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (2004). Timing changes: The development of anticipatory smiling. Infancy, 6(3), 397–406.

  • Wachs, T., & Chan, A. (1986). Specificity of environmental action, as seen in environmental correlates of infants’ communication performance. Child Development, 57, 1464–1474.

  • Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

  • Whalen, C., Schreibman, L., & Ingersoll, B. (2006). The collateral effects of joint attention training on social initiations, positive affect, imitation, and spontaneous speech for young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 655–664.

  • White, P. J., O’Reilly, M., Streusand, W., Levine, A., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., et al. (2011). Best practice for teaching joint attention: systematic review of the intervention literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 5(4), 1283–1295.

  • Wimpory, D. C., Hobson, R. P., Williams, J. M. G., & Nash, S. (2000). Are infants with autism socially engaged? A controlled study of recent retrospective parental reports. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 525–536.

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23,143–152.

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jun 2020 0 4 5
Jul 2020 3 4 3
Aug 2020 1 0 0
Sep 2020 3 0 0
Oct 2020 10 1 2
Nov 2020 11 0 0
Dec 2020 0 0 0