View More View Less
  • 1 MTA Természettudományi Kutatóközpont, Kognitív Idegtudományi és Pszichológiai Intézet
  • | 2 Szegedi Tudományegyetem
Restricted access

Purchase article

USD  $25.00

1 year subscription (Individual Only)

USD  $204.00

Bár a legalapvetőbb hangjellemzők észlelt felbontása fiatal csecsemőknél messze elmarad a felnőttek hasonló képességeitől, a fejlettebb hangfeldolgozási funkciókat tekintve minőségileg nem különböznek tőlük. A csecsemők kompetens észlelők a hangok világában, hallási tárgyreprezentációkat alkotnak, és képesek kiemelni a beérkező hangok bonyolult belső összefüggéseit. Áttekintve a hangfeldolgozási képességek mintázatát fiatal csecsemőknél, a cikk amellett érvel, hogy e képességek jelentős része a hangokkal történő kommunikáció, azon belül is a dialógusok felépítésének és fenntartásának szolgálatában áll. Az emberi fejlődésben a szociális kommunikációnak kiemelt jelentősége van, mivel lehetővé teszi, hogy az élet kezdetén rövid idő alatt igen nagy mennyiségű kész tudást vehessünk át. Ez indokolja, hogy a fiatal csecsemők hallórendszere az első pillanattól kezdve készen áll az emberi tanulás e leghatékonyabb formájának támogatására.

  • ABERCROMBIE, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Chicago: Aldine.

  • ALAIN, C., ARNOTT, S. R., & PICTON, T. W. (2001). Bottom-up and top-down influences on auditory scene analysis: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 10721089.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ALAIN, C., & WINKLER, I. (2012). Recording event-related brain potentials: application to study auditory perception. In D. POEPPEL, T. OVERATH, A. N. POPPER, & R. R. FAY (Eds.), The Human Auditory Cortex. Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, Vol. 43 (6996). New York: Springer.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ALEXANDER, A. L., LEE, J. E., LAZAR, M., & FIELD, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics, 4(3), 316329.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BABARCZY A. , LUKÁCS Á. és PLÉH CS. (2014). A nyelvelsajátítás elméleti modelljei. In LUKÁCS Á. és PLÉH CS. (szerk.), Pszicholingvisztika 1–2. Magyar pszicholingvisztikai kézikönyv (463499). Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BARUCH, C., & DRAKE, C. (1997). Tempo discrimination in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 573577.

  • BASIRAT, A., DEHAENE, S., & DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G. (2014). A hierarchy of cortical responses to sequence violations in three-month-old infants. Cognition, 132(2), 137150.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BENDIXEN, A., HÁDEN, G. P., NÉMETH, R., FARKAS, D., TÖRÖK, M., & WINKLER, I. (2015). Newborn infants detect cues of concurrent sound segregation. Developmental Neuroscience, 37(2), 172181.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BENDIXEN, A., SCHRÖGER, E., & WINKLER, I. (2009). I heard that coming: event-related potential evidence for stimulus-driven prediction in the auditory system. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(26), 84478451.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BERGESON, T. R., & TREHUB, S. E. (2006). Infants’ perception of rhythmic patterns. Music Perception, 23(4), 345360.

  • BREGMAN, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • BREGMAN, A. S., & RUDNICKY, A. I. (1975). Auditory segregation: Stream or streams. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 263267.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BULL, D., EILERS, R. E., & OLLER, D. K. (1984). Infants’ discrimination of intensity variation in mulitsyllabic stimuli. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 1317.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CARRAL, V., HUOTILAINEN, M., RUUSUVIRTA, T., FELLMAN, V., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., & ESCERA, C. (2005). A kind of auditory ‘primitive intelligence’ already present at birth. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 32013204.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CHANG, H-W., & TREHUB, S. E. (1977). Infants’ perception of temporal grouping in auditory patterns. Child Development, 48, 16661670.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CHRISTOPHE, A., DUPOUX, E., BERTONCINI, J., & MEHLER, J. (1994). Do infants perceive word boundaries? An empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 95(3), 15701580.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CHRISTOPHE, A., MEHLER, J., & SEBASTIÁN-GALLÉS, N. (2001). Perception of prosodic boundary correlates by newborn infants. Infancy, 2, 385394.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CHRISTOPHE, A., MILLOTTE, S., BERNAL, S., & LIDZ, J. (2008). Bootstrapping lexical and syntactic acquisition. Language and Speech, 51(1–2), 6175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CIOCCA, V. (2008). The auditory organization of complex sounds. Frontiers in Bioscience, 13, 148169.

  • CLARKSON, M. G., & CLIFTON, R. K. (1985). Infant pitch perception: evidence for responding to the pitch of the missing fundamental. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 77, 15211528.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CLARKSON, M. G., CLIFTON, R. K., & PERRIS, E. E. (1988). Infant timbre perception: Discrimination of spectral envelopes. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 1520.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CLIFTON, R. K. (1992). The development of spatial hearing in human infants. In L. A. WERNER, & E. W. RUBEL (Eds.), Developmental Psychoacoustics (135157). Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • COOPER, R. P., & ASLIN, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 15841595.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • COWAN, N. (1984). On short and long auditory stores. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 341370.

  • CSIBRA, G., & GERGELY, GY. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. MUNAKATA, & M. H. JOHNSON (Eds.), Processes of Change in Brain and Cognitive Development. Attention and Performance, XXI. (249274). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • CSIBRA, G., & GERGELY, GY. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148153.

  • CSIBRA, G., & GERGELY, GY. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 11491157.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DECASPER, A. J., & FIFER, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 208, 11741176.

  • DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G. (2011). The architecture of the baby brain. In A. BATTRO, & S. DEHAENE (Eds.), Human Neuroplasticity and Education (185259). Vatican city: Singer.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G., DEHAENE, S., & HERTZ-PANNIER, L. (2002). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, 298, 20132015.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G., HERTZ-PANNIER, L., & DUBOIS, J. (2006). Nature and nurture in language acquisition: anatomical and functional brain-imaging studies in infants. Trends in Neurosciences, 29(7), 367373.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G., MONTAVONT, A., JOBERT, A., ALLIROL, L., DUBOIS, J., HERTZPANNIER, L., & DEHAENE, S. (2010). Language or music, mother or Mozart? Structural and environmental influences on infants’ language networks. Brain and Language, 114(2), 5365.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DEMANY, L. (1982). Auditory stream segregation in infancy. Infant Behavior & Development, 5, 261276.

  • DEMANY, L., & ARMAND, F. (1984). The perceptual reality of tone chroma in early infancy. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 5766.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DEMANY, L., MCKENZIE, B., & VURPILLOT, E. (1977). Rhythm perception in early infancy. Nature, 266, 718719.

  • DENHAM, S. L., & WINKLER, I. (2015). Auditory perceptual organization. In J. WAGEMANS (Ed.), Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization (601619). Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DOWLING, W. J., & HARWOOD, D. L. (1986). Music Cognition. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

  • DUBOIS, J., HERTZ-PANNIER, L., DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G., COINTEPAS, Y., & LE BIHAN, D. (2006). Assessment of the early organization and maturation of infants’ cerebral white matter fiber bundles: a feasibility study using quantitative diffusion tensor imaging and tractography. NeuroImage, 30, 11211132.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DUNCAN, J., & HUMPHREYS, G. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433458.

  • ELFENBEIN, J. L., SMALL, A. M., & DAVIS, M. (1993). Developmental patterns of duration discrimination. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 842849.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FASSBENDER, C. (1993). Auditory Grouping and Segregation Processes in Infancy. Norderstedt: Kaste Verlag.

  • FERRARI, M. & QUARESIMA, V. (2012). A brief review on the history of human functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) development and fields of application. Neuroimage, 63(2), 921935.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FISCHER, B., & HARTNEGG, H. K. (2004). On the development of low-level auditory discrimination and deficits indyslexia. Dyslexia, 10, 105118.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FITZGIBBONS, P. J., POLLATSEK, A., & THOMAS, I. B. (1974). Detection of temporal gaps within and between perceptual tonal groups. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 522552.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FLOCCIA, C., CHRISTOPHE, A., & BERTONCINI, J. (1997). High-amplitude sucking and newborns: the quest for underlying mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(2), 175198.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FOLLAND, N. A., BUTLER, B. E., SMITH, N. A., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2012). Processing simultaneous auditory objects: Infants’ ability to detect mistunings in harmonic complexes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131, 993997.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FOWLER, C. A., SMITH, M. R., & TASSINARY, L. G. (1986). Perception of syllable timing by prebabbling infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 79(3), 814825.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FRIEDERICI, A. D. (2005). Neurophysiological markers of early language acquisition: from syllables to sentences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 481488.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FRIEDERICI, A. D., MUELLER, J. L., & OBERECKER, R. (2011). Precursors to natural grammar learning: preliminary evidence from 4-month-old infants. PLoS One, 6(3), e17920.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FRIEDLANDER, B. Z. (1968). The effect of speaker identity, voice inflection, vocabulary, and message redundancy on infants’ selection of vocal reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Child Psycliology, 6, 443445.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FRISTON, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360, 815836.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GEFFEN, M. N., GERVAIN, J., WERKER, J. F., & MAGNASCO, M. O. (2011). Auditory perception of self-similarity in water sounds. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 15.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GERVAIN, J., BERENT, I., & WERKER, J. F. (2012). Binding at birth: the newborn brain detects identity relations and sequential position in speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(3), 564574.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GERVAIN, J., & MEHLER, J. (2010). Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 191218.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GERVAIN, J., MACAGNO, F., COGOI, S., PENA, M., & MEHLER, J. (2008). The neonate brain detects speech structure. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA., 105, 1422214227.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GERVAIN, J., WERKER, J. F., & GEFFEN, M. N. (2014). Category-specific processing of scaleinvariant sounds in infancy. PLOS ONE, 9(5), e96278.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GOMES, H., BERNSTEIN, R., RITTER, W., VAUGHAN, H. G., & MILLER, J. (1997). Storage of feature conjunctions in transient auditory memory. Psychophysiology, 34, 712716.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GORDON, I. E. (2004). Theories of Visual Perception. New York: Psychology Press.

  • GREGORY, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 290, 181197.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GRIFFITHS, T. D., & WARREN, J. D. (2004). Opinion: What is an auditory object? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 887892.

  • HÁDEN, G. P., STEFANICS, G., VESTERGAARD, M. D., DENHAM, S. L., SZILLER, I., & WINKLER, I. (2009). Timbre-independent extraction of pitch in newborn infants. Psychophysiology, 46, 6974.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HÁDEN, G. P., NÉMETH, R., TÖRÖK, M., DRÁVUCZ, S., & WINKLER, I. (2013). Context effects on processing widely deviant sounds in newborn infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 674.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HÁDEN, G. P., HONING, H., TÖRÖK, M., & WINKLER, I. (2015a). Detecting the temporal structure of sound sequences in newborn infants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 96(1), 2328.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HÁDEN, G. P., NÉMETH, R., TÖRÖK, M., & WINKLER, I. (2015b). Predictive processing of pitch trends in newborn infants. Brain Research, 1626, 1420.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HÄMÄLÄINEN, M., HARI, R., ILMONIEMI, R. J., KNUUTILA, J., & LOUNASMAA, O. V. (1993). Magnetoencephalography-theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Reviews of Modern Physics, 65(2), 413497.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HANNON, E. E., & JOHNSON, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 354377.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HANNON, E. E., & TREHUB, S. E. (2005). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychological Science, 16, 4855.

  • HARTMANN, W. M., MCADAMS, S., & SMITH, B. K. (1990). Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88(4), 17121724.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HE, C., HOTSON, L., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2009a). Maturation of cortical mismatch responses to occasional pitch change in early infancy: Effects of presentation rate and magnitude of change. Neuropsychologia, 47, 218229.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HE, C., HOTSON, L., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2009b). Development of infant mismatch responses to auditory pattern changes between 2 and 4 months old. European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 861867.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HE, C., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2009). Finding the pitch of the missing fundamental in infants. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 77187722.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HOMAE, F., WATANABE, H., NAKANO, T., ASAKAWA, K., & TAGA, G. (2006). The right hemisphere of sleeping infant perceives sentential prosody. Neuroscience Research, 54, 276280.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HORVÁTH, J., CZIGLER, I., SUSSMAN, E., & WINKLER, I. (2001). Simultaneously active preattentive representations of local and global rules for sound sequences. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 131144.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HUETTEL, S. A., SONG, A. W., & MCCARTHY, G. (2008). Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2nd edition. Sunderland, MA.: Sinauer Associates Inc.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ISEL, F., ALTER, K., & FRIEDERICI, A. D. (2005). Influence of prosodic information on the processing of split particles: ERP evidence from spoken German. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 154167.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JACOBSEN, T., SCHRÖGER, E., WINKLER, I., & HORVÁTH, J. (2005). Familiarity affects the processing of task-irrelevant ignored sounds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(11), 17041713.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JAFFE, J., BEEBE, B., FELDSTEIN, S., CROWN, C. L., & JASNOW, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. In W. D. OVERTON (Ed.), Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 66, No. 2. Boston, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JENNI, O. G., & CARSKADON, M. A. (2000). Normal human sleep at different ages: Infants to adolescents. In Sleep research society. SRS Basics of Sleep Guide (1119). Westchester, IL: Sleep Research Society.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JENSEN, J. K., & NEFF, D. L. (1993). Development of basic auditory discrimination in preschool children. Psychological Science, 4, 104107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JONES, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323355.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JONES, M. R., MOYNIHAN, H., MACKENZIE, N., & PUENTE, J. (2002). Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychological Science, 13, 313319.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JUSCZYK, P. W. (1985). The high-amplitude procedure as a methodological tool in speech perception research. In G. GOTTLIEB, & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.), Infant Methodology (195222). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KABDEBON, C., PENA, M., BUIATTI, M., & DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G. (2015). Electrophysiological evidence of statistical learning of long-distance dependencies in 8-month-old preterm and full-term infants. Brain and Language, 148, 2536.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KÖHLER, W. (1947). Gestalt Psychology: An Introduction to New Concepts in Modern Psychology. New York: Liveright Publishing Corporation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KUHL, P. K. (1979). Speech perception in early infancy: Perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 66, 16681679.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KUHL, P. K. (2000). A new view of language acquisition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 97, 1185011857.

  • KUJALA, T., TERVANIEMI, M., & SCHRÖGER, E. (2007). The mismatch negativity in cognitive and clinical neuroscience: theoretical and methodological considerations. Biological Psychology, 74(1), 119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KUSHNERENKO, E., CEPONIENE, R., FELLMAN, V., HUOTILAINEN, M., & WINKLER, I. (2001). Event-related potential correlates of sound duration: Similar pattern from birth to adulthood. NeuroReport, 12, 37773781.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KUSHNERENKO, E. V., VAN DEN BERGH, B. R., & WINKLER, I. (2013). Separating acoustic deviance from novelty during the first year of life: a review of event-related potential evidence. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 595.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KUSHNERENKO, E., WINKLER, I., HORVÁTH, J., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., PAVLOV, I., FELLMAN, V., & HUOTILAINEN, M. (2007). Processing acoustic change and novelty in newborn infants. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 265274.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LADINIG, O., HONING, H., HÁDEN, G., & WINKLER, I. (2009). Probing attentive and preattentive emergent meter in adult listeners without extensive music training. Music Perception, 26(4), 377386.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LANY, J., & SAFFRAN, J. R. (2013). Statistical learning mechanisms in infancy. In J. L. R. RUBENSTEIN, & P. RAKIC (Eds.), Comprehensive Developmental Neuroscience: Neural Circuit Development and Function in the Brain, Vol. 3 (231248). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LECANUET, J-P., & SCHAAL, B. (1996). Fetal sensory competencies. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 68, 123.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LERDAHL, F., & JACKENDOFF, R. (1983). A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • LEWKOWICZ, D. (1989). The role of temporal factors in infant behavior and development. In I. LEVIN, & D. ZAKAY (Eds.), Time and Human Cognition (962). North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LEWKOWICZ, D. J. (2003). Learning and discrimination of audiovisual events in human infants: The hierarchical relation between intersensory temporal synchrony and rhythmic pattern cues. Developmental Psychology, 39, 795804.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LITOVSKY, R. Y., & ASHMEAD, D. H. (1997). Development of binaural and spatial hearing in infants and children. In R. H. GILKEY, & T. R. ANDERSON (Eds.), Binarual and Spatial Hearing in Real and Virtual Environments (571592). Manwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LUCK, S. J. (2005). An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • MAREAN, G. C., WERNER, L. A., & KUHL, P. K. (1992). Vowel categorization by very young infants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 396405.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MASATAKA, N. (2006). Preference for consonance over dissonance by hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. Developmental Science, 9, 4650.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MAXON, A. B., & HOCHBERG, I. (1982). Development of psychoacoustic behavior: sensitivity and discrimination. Ear and Hearing, 3, 301308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MCADAMS, S., & BERTONCINI, J. (1997). Organization and discrimination of repeating sound sequences by newborn infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 29452953.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MEHLER, J., JUSCZYK, P., LAMBERTZ, G., HALSTED, N., BERTONCINI, J., & AMIEL-TISON, C. (1988). A precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143178.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MILLER, C. L. (1983). Developmental changes in male/female voice classification by infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 313330.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MILLS, A. W. (1972). Auditory Localization. In J. V. TOBIAS (Ed.), Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory, Vol. II. (301348). NY: Academic Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MOLNAR, M., GERVAIN, J., & CARREIRAS, M. (2013). Within-rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque-Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326337.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MOON, C., COOPER, R. P., & FIFER, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behavior & Development, 16, 495500.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MOON, C., ZERNZACH, R. C., & KUHL, P. K. (2015). Mothers say “baby” and their newborns do not choose to listen: a behavioral preference study to compare with ERP results. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 153.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MOORE, B. C. J., GLASBERG, B. R., & PETERS, R. W. (1986). Thresholds for hearing mistuned partials as separate tones in harmonic complexes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 80(2), 479483.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MOORE, J. M., & WILSON, W. (1978). Visual reinforcement audiometry (VRA) with infants. In S. E. GERBER, & G. T. MENCHER (Eds.), Early Diagnosis of Hearing Loss (177–213). New York: Grune and Stratton.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MORGAN, J. L. (1996). Prosody and the roots of parsing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 11, 69106.

  • MORRONGIELLO, B. A., FENWICK, K. D., HILLIER, L., & GRAHAM, C. (1994). Sound localization in newborn human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 27(8), 519538.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MORRONGIELLO, B. A., & TREHUB, S. E. (1987). Age-related changes in auditory temporal perception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 413426.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NÄÄTÄNEN, R., KUJALA, T., WINKLER, I. (2011). Auditory processing that leads to conscious perception: A unique window to central auditory processing opened by the mismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology, 48, 422.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NAZZI, T., BERTONCINI, J., & MEHLER, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756766.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NAZZI, T., JUSCZYK, P. W., & JOHNSON, E. K. (2000). Language discrimination by Englishlearning 5-month-olds: Effects of rhythm and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NAZZI, T., & RAMUS, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41, 233243.

  • NÉMETH, R., HÁDEN, G. P., TÖRÖK, M., & WINKLER, I. (2015). Processing of horizontal sound localization cues in newborn infants. Ear and Hearing, 36(5), 550556.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NIEDERMEYER, E. & DA SILVA, F. L. (2004). Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincot Williams & Wilkins.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • NOVITSKI, N., HUOTILAINEN, M., TERVANIEMI, M., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., & FELLMAN, V. (2007). Neonatal frequency discrimination in 250–4000-Hz range: Electrophysiological evidence. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 412419.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OLSHO, L. W., KOCH, E. G., CARTER, E. A., HALPIN, C. F., & SPETNER, N. B (1988). Puretone sensitivity of human infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 84, 13161324.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OLSHO, L. W., KOCH, E. G., & HALPIN, C. F. (1987). Level and age effects in infant frequency discrimination. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 82, 454464.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OLSHO, L. W., KOCH, E. G., HALPIN, C. F., & CARTER, E. A. (1987). An observer-based psychoacoustic procedure for use with young infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 627640.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • OTTE, R. A., WINKLER, I., BRAEKEN, M. A. K. A., STEKELENBURG, J. J., VAN DER STELT, O., & VAN DEN BERGH, B. R. H. (2013). Detecting violations of temporal regularities in waking and sleeping two-month-old infants. Biological Psychology, 92(2), 315322.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PANNESE, A., HERRMANN, C. S., & SUSSMAN, E. (2015). Analyzing the auditory scene: neurophysiologic evidence of a dissociation between detection of regularity and detection of change. Brain Topography, 28(3), 411422.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PEGG, J. E., WERKER, J. F., & MCLEOD, P. J. (1992). Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 325345.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PEÑA, M., MAKI, A., KOVACIC, D., DEHAENE-LAMBERTZ, G., KOIZUMI, H., BOUQUET, F., & MEHLER, J. (2003). Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100(20), 1170211705.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PERANI, D., SACCUMAN, M. C., SCIFO, P., SPADA, D., ANDREOLLI, G., ROVELLI, R., BALDOLI, C., & KOELSCH, S. (2009). Functional specializations for music processing in the human newborn brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(10), 47586473.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PETERS, A. M. (1983). The Units of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • PHILLIPS-SILVER, J., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: Movement influences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308(5727), 1430.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PICTON, T. W. (2010). Human Auditory Evoked Potentials. San Diego: Plural Publishing.

  • PLANTINGA, J., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2005). Memory for melody: Infants use a relative pitch code. Cognition, 98, 111.

  • PURHONEN, M., KILPELÄINEN-LEES, R., VALKONEN-KORHONEN, M., KARHU, J., & LEHTONEN, J. (2005). Four-month-old infants process own mother’s voice faster than unfamiliar voices–electrical signs of sensitization in infant brain. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 627633.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RAMUS, F. (2002). Language discrimination by newborns: Teasing apart phonotactic, rhythmic, and intonational cues. Annual Review of Language Acquisition, 2, 85115.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RUUSUVIRTA, T., HUOTILAINEN, M., FELLMAN, V., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (2003). The newborn human brain binds sound features together. Neuroreport, 14(16), 21172119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RUUSUVIRTA, T., HUOTILAINEN, M., FELLMAN, V., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (2004). Newborn human brain identifies repeated auditory feature conjunctions of low sequential probability. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20(10), 28192821.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RUUSUVIRTA, T., HUOTILAINEN, M., FELLMAN, V., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (2009). Numerical discrimination in newborn infants as revealed by event-related potentials to tone sequences. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(8), 16201624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SAARINEN, J., PAAVILAINEN, P., SCHRÖGER, E., TERVANIEMI, M., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (1992). Representation of abstract attributes of auditory stimuli in the human brain. Neuroreport, 3, 11491151.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SADIA, G., RITTER, W., & SUSSMAN, E. (2013). Category effects: Is top-down control alone sufficient to elicit the mismatch negativity (MMN) component? Biological Psychology, 92(2), 191198.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SAMBETH, A., RUOHIO, K., ALKU, P., FELLMAN, V., & HUOTILAINEN, M. (2008). Sleeping newborns extract prosody from continuous speech. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(2), 332341.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SANSAVINI, A., BERTONCINI, J., & GIOVANELLI, G. (1997). Newborns discriminate the rhythm of multisyllabic stressed words. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 311.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SCHARF, B., & HOUTSMA, A. J. M. (1986). Audition II: Loudness, pitch, localization, aural distortion, pathology. In K. R. BOFF, L. KAUFMAN, & J. P. THOMAS (Eds.), Handbook of Perception and Human Performance: Vol. I, Sensory Processes and Perception (15.2–60). New York: Wiley.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SCHNEIDER, B. A., TREHUB, S. E., MORRONGIELLO, B. A., & THORPE, L. A. (1989). Developmental changes in masked thresholds. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 17331742.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SENJU, A., & CSIBRA, G. (2008). Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology, 18, 668671.

  • SHI, R., WERKER, J. F., & MORGAN, J. L. (1999). Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72(2), B1121.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SHOFNER, W. P. (2005). Comparative aspects of pitch perception. In C. J. PLACK, A. J. OXENHAM, R. R. FAY, & A. N. POPPER (Eds.), Pitch Perception: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research, Vol. 24 (56–98). New York: Springer.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SINGH, N., & THEUNISSEN, F. (2003). Modulation spectra of natural sounds and ethological theories of auditory processing. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 33943411.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SINNOTT, J. M., & ASLIN, R. N. (1985). Frequency and intensity discrimination in human infants and adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 78, 19861992.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SLUGOCKI, C., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2014). Cortical indices of sound localization mature monotonically in early infancy. European Journal of Neuroscience, 40, 36083619.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SMITH, E. C., & LEWICKI, M. S. (2006). Efficient auditory coding. Nature, 439, 978982.

  • SMITH, N. A., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2011). Auditory stream segregation improves infants’ selective attention to target tones amid distractors. Infancy, 16, 655668.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SMITH, N. A., TRAINOR, L. J., & SHORE, D. I. (2006). The development of temporal resolution: Between-channel gap detection in infants and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(5), 11041113.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SPETNER, N. B., & OLSHO, L. W. (1990). Auditory frequency resolution in human infancy. Child Development, 61, 632652.

  • STEFANICS, G., HÁDEN, G., HUOTILAINEN, M., BALÁZS, L., SZILLER, I., BEKE, A., FELLMAN, V., & WINKLER, I. (2007). Auditory temporal grouping in newborn infants. Psychophysiology, 44, 697702.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • STEFANICS, G., HÁDEN, G. P., SZILLER, I., BALÁZS, L., BEKE, A., & WINKLER, I. (2009). Newborn infants process pitch intervals. Clinical Neurophysiology, 120, 304308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • STEFANICS, G., HANGYA, B., HERNÁDI, I., WINKLER, I., LAKATOS, P., & ULBERT, I. (2010). Phase entrainment of human delta oscillations can mediate the effects of expectation on reaction speed. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(41), 1357813585.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • STOFFGREN, T. A., & BRADY, B. G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 195222.

  • SUSSMAN, E. (2007). A new view on the MMN and attention debate: Auditory context effects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3–4), 164175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SUSSMAN, E., RITTER, W., & VAUGHAN, H. G. JR. (1998). Predictability of stimulus deviance and the mismatch negativity. NeuroReport, 9, 41674170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SUSSMAN, E., WINKLER, I., HUOTILAINEN, M., RITTER, W., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (2002). Topdown effects on stimulus-driven auditory organization. Cognitive Brain Research, 13, 393405.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TAKEGATA, R., BRATTICO, E., TERVANIEMI, M., VARYAGINA, O., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., & WINKLER, I. (2005). Pre-attentive representation of feature conjunctions for simultaneous, spatially distributed auditory objects. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(1), 169179.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TEINONEN, T., FELLMAN, V., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., ALKU, P., & HUOTILAINEN, M. (2009). Statistical language learning in neonates revealed by event-related brain potentials. BMC Neuroscience, 10, 21.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TEW, S., FUJIOKA, T., HE, C., & TRAINOR, L. (2009). Neural representation of transposed melody in infants at 6 months of age. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 287290.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TSAO, F-M., LIU, H-M., & KUHL, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 75, 10671084.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • THARPE, A. M., & ASHMEAD, D. H. (2001). A longitudinal investigation of infant auditory sensitivity. American Journal of Audiology, 10, 104112.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • THERIEN, J. M., WORWA, C. T., MATTIA, F. R., & DE REGNIER, R-A. (2004). Altered pathways for auditory discrimination and recognition memory in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46, 816824.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • THORPE, L. A., & TREHUB, S. E. (1989). Duration illusion and auditory grouping in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 122127.

  • THORPE, L. A., TREHUB, S. E., MORRONGIELLO, B. A., & BULL, D. (1988). Perceptual grouping by infants and preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 484491.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TOMASELLO, M. (2003). Constructing a Language: A Usage-Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • TOMASELLO, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA/London: The MIT Press.

  • TRAINOR, L. J. (2012). Musical experience, plasticity, and maturation: issues in measuring developmental change using EEG and MEG. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252, 2536.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TRAINOR, L. J. (2015). The origins of music in auditory scene analysis and the roles of evolution and culture in musical creation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140089.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TRAINOR, L. J., LEE, K., & BOSNYAK, D. J. (2011). Cortical plasticity in 4-month-old infants: Specific effects of experience with musical timbres. Brain Topography, 24, 192203.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TRAINOR, L. J., SAMUEL, S. S., DESJARDINS, R. N., & SONNADARA, R. R. (2001). Measuring temporal resolution in infants using mismatch negativity. Neuroreport, 12, 24432448.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TRAINOR, L. J., TSANG, C. D., & CHEUNG, V. H. W. (2002). Preference for consonance in 2-and 4-month-old infants. Music Perception, 20, 187194.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TRAINOR, L. J., & UNRAU, A. J. (2012). Development of pitch and music perception. In L. WERNER, R. R. FAY, & A. N. POPPER (Eds.), Springer Handbook of Auditory Research: Human Auditory Development (223254). New York: Springer.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TREHUB, S. E., ENDMAN, M. W., & THORPE, L. A. (1990). Infants’ perception of timbre: Classification of complex tones by spectral structure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49, 300313.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TREHUB, S. E., SCHNEIDER, B. A., & ENDMAN, M. (1980). Developmental changes in infants’ sensitivity to octave-band noises. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 282293.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TREHUB, S. E., SCHNEIDER, B. A., & HENDERSON, J. (1995). Gap detection in infants, children, and adults. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 98, 25322541.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TREISMAN, A. M., & GELADE, G. A. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97136.

  • TSANG, C. D., & TRAINOR, L. J. (2002). Spectral slope discrimination in infancy: Sensitivity to socially important timbres. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 183194.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • TURK-BROWNE, N. B., SCHOLL, B. J., & CHUN, M. M. (2008). Babies and brains: Habituation in infant cognition and functional neuroimaging. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 16.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VAN DEN HEUVEL, M. I., OTTE, R. A., BRAEKEN, M. A. K. A., WINKLER, I., KUSHNERENKO, E., & VAN DEN BERGH, B. R. H. (2015). Differences between human auditory eventrelated potentials (AERP) measured at 2 and 4 months after birth. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 7583.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VAN DEN HEUVEL, M. P., & SPORNS, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 683696.

  • VAN DINTHER, R., & PATTERSON, R. D. (2006). The perception of size in musical instruments. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 21582176.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VESTERGAARD, M. D., HÁDEN, G. P., SHTYROV, Y., PATTERSON, R. D., PULVERMÜLLER, F., DENHAM, S. L., SZILLER, I., & WINKLER, I. (2009). Auditory size-deviant detection in adults and newborn infants. Biological Psychology, 82, 169175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VIRTALA, P., HUOTILAINEN, M., PARTANEN, E., FELLMAN, V., & TERVANIEMI, M. (2013). Newborn infants’ auditory system is sensitive to Western music chord categories. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 492.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • VOULOUMANOS, A., & WERKER, J. F. (2004). Tuned to the signal: the privileged status of speech for young infants. Developmental Science, 7(3), 270276.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WEBER, C., HAHNE, A., FRIEDRICH, M., & FRIEDERICI, A. D. (2004). Discrimination of word stress in early infant perception: electrophysiological evidence. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 149161.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERKER, J. F., & MCLEOD, P. J. (1989). Infant preference for both male and female infantdirected talk: A developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 43, 230246.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A. (1995). Observer-based approaches to human infant psychoacoustics. In G. M. KLUMP, R. J. DOOLING, R. R. FAY, & W. C. STEBBINS (Eds.), Methods in Comparative Psychoacoustics (135146). Boston: Birkhäuser.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A. (1997). Forward masking among infant and adult listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105(4), 24452453.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A. (2007). Human auditory development. In R. HOY, P. DALLOS, & D. OERTEL (Eds.), The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, Volume 3 –Audition (871894). St. Louis: Elsevier.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A., FOLSOM, R. C., & MANCL, L. R. (1994). The relation between auditory brainstem response latencies and behavioral thresholds in normal hearing infants and adults. Hearing Research, 77, 8898.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A., & GILLENWATER, J. M. (1990). Pure-tone sensitivity of 2-to 5-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 355375.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WERNER, L. A., MAREAN, G. C., HALPIN, C. F., SPETNER, N. B., & GILLENWATER, J. M. (1992). Infant auditory temporal acuity: Gap detection. Child Development, 63, 260272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WILLIAMS, L., & GOLENSKI, J. (1978). Infant speech sound discrimination: The effects of contingent versus noncontingent stimulus presentation. Child Development, 49, 213217.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I. (2007). Interpreting the mismatch negativity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(3–4), 147163.

  • WINKLER, I., & CZIGLER, I. (2012). Evidence from auditory and visual event-related potential (ERP) studies of deviance detection (MMN and vMMN) linking predictive coding theories and perceptual object representations. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 132143.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., CZIGLER, I., SUSSMAN, E., HORVÁTH, J., & BALÁZS, L. (2005). Preattentive binding of auditory and visual stimulus features. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(2), 320339.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., DENHAM, S., MILL, R., BOHM, T. M., & BENDIXEN, A. (2012). Multistability in auditory stream segregation: A predictive coding view. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 10011012.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., DENHAM, S. L., & NELKEN, I. (2009). Modeling the auditory scene: predictive regularity representations and perceptual objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 532540.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., HÁDEN, G. P., LADINIG, O., SZILLER, I., & HONING, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106(7), 24682471.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., KUSHNERENKO, E., HORVÁTH, J., CEPONIENE, R., FELLMAN, V., HUOTILAINEN, M., NÄÄTÄNEN, R., & SUSSMAN, E. (2003). Newborn infants can organize the auditory world. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 100(20), 1181211815.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., & SCHRÖGER, E. (2015). Auditory perceptual objects as generative models: Setting the stage for communication by sound. Brain and Language, 148, 122.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WINKLER, I., TERVANIEMI, M., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (1997). Two separate codes for missing fundamental pitch in the auditory cortex. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102, 10721082.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • YABE, H., TERVANIEMI, M., SINKKONEN, J., HUOTILAINEN, M., ILMONIEMI, R. J., & NÄÄTÄNEN, R. (1998). The temporal window of integration of auditory information in the human brain. Psychophysiology, 35, 615619.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ZENTNER, M. R., & KAGAN, J. (1998). Infants’ perception of consonance and dissonance in music. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 483492.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

 

The author instruction is available in PDF.
Please, download the file from HERE.

 

 

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Fülöp, Márta

Chair of the Editorial Board:
Molnár, Márk

          Area Editors

  • Bereczkei Tamás (Evolutionary psychology)
  • Demetrovics Zsolt (Clinical psychology)
  • Egyed Katalin (Developmental psychology)
  • Hámori Eszter (Clinical child psychology)
  • Molnárné Kovács Judit (Social psychology)
  • Rózsa Sándor (Personality psychology and psychometrics)
  • Nguyen Luu Lan Anh (Cross-cultural psychology)
  • Pléh Csaba (Book Review)
  • Sass Judit (Industrial and organizational psychology)
  • Szabó Éva (Educational psychology)
  • Urbán Róbert (Health psychology)
  • Csákvári Judit (Psychology of special education)
  • Faragó Klára (Organizational psychology)
  • Kéri Szabolcs (Experimental and Neuropsychology)

 

        Editorial Board

  • Császár Noémi
  • Csépe Valéria
  • Dúll Andrea
  • Ehmann Bea
  • Erős Ferenc
  • Gervai Judit
  • Kiss Enikő Csilla
  • Kiss Paszkál
  • Mészáros Judit
  • Németh Dezső
  • Oláh Attila
  • Péley Bernadette
  • Perczel-Forintos Dóra
  • Révész György
  • Winkler István
  • Molnár Márk
  • Fülöp Márta

 

Secretary of the editorial board: 

  •  Saád Judit

 

Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle
ELTE PPK Pszichológiai Intézet
Address: H-1064 Budapest, Izabella u. 46.
E-mail: pszichoszemle@gmail.com

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • PsycINFO
  • Scopus

2020  
Scimago
H-index
7
Scimago
Journal Rank
0,142
Scimago
Quartile Score
Psychology (miscellaneous) Q4
Scopus
Cite Score
17/111=0,2
Scopus
Cite Score Rank
General Psychology 199/203 (Q4)
Scopus
SNIP
0,079
Scopus
Cites
53
Scopus
Documents
24
Days from submission to acceptance 116
Days from acceptance to publication 225
Acceptance
Rate
81%

 

2019  
Scimago
H-index
6
Scimago
Journal Rank
0,139
Scimago
Quartile Score
Psychology (miscellaneous) Q4
Scopus
Cite Score
24/103=0,2
Scopus
Cite Score Rank
General Psychology 192/204 (Q4)
Scopus
SNIP
0,113
Scopus
Cites
35
Scopus
Documents
14
Acceptance
Rate
58%

 

Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle
Publication Model Hybrid
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 900 EUR/article
Printed Color Illustrations 40 EUR (or 10 000 HUF) + VAT / piece
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Editorial Board / Advisory Board members: 50%
Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%
Subscription fee 2021 Online subsscription: 152 EUR / 216 USD
Print + online subscription: 184 EUR / 244 USD
Subscription fee 2022 Online subsscription: 156 EUR / 220 USD
Print + online subscription: 188 EUR / 250 USD
Subscription Information Online subscribers are entitled access to all back issues published by Akadémiai Kiadó for each title for the duration of the subscription, as well as Online First content for the subscribed content.
Purchase per Title Individual articles are sold on the displayed price.

Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle
Language Hungarian
Size B5
Year of
Foundation
1928
Publication
Programme
2021 Volume 76
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Magyar Pszichológiai Társaság 
Founder's
Address
H-1075 Budapest, Hungary Kazinczy u. 23-27. I/116. 
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 0025-0279 (Print)
ISSN 1588-2799 (Online)

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
May 2021 0 0 0
Jun 2021 3 0 0
Jul 2021 0 0 0
Aug 2021 2 1 1
Sep 2021 3 0 0
Oct 2021 7 0 0
Nov 2021 0 0 0