A koraszülöttség pszichológiai szakirodalma igen változatos eredményeket közöl a koraszülöttség fejlődési kimenetelével kapcsolatban. Az anya–gyermek interakció vizsgálata segíthet a fejlődési kimenetel heterogenitásának megértésében. Jelen vizsgálatban 29 mérsékelten rizikós koraszülött és 40 egészséges, időre született gyermek és édesanyáik vettek részt. A csecsemők 12 hónapos korában megfigyeléses adatgyűjtést végeztünk, játékhelyzetben rögzítettük általunk kialakított szempontrendszer alapján az anyai és a gyermeki viselkedéses sajátosságokat. Emellett a 12 hónapos kori Bayley fejlődési mutatókat is bevontuk az elemzésbe. Eredményeink szerint a koraszülöttek csoportjában némileg kedvezőtlenebb sajátosságok mutatkoztak. Jellemzőbb az anyai passzivitás, amire a gyermekek nagyobb fokú kezdeményezéssel válaszolnak. A koraszülött csecsemők továbbá többször váltanak játéktárgyat, és kevesebbet vokalizálnak. A koraszülött csoportban összefüggést találtunk az anyai tanítói viselkedés és a gyermek mentális fejlődési indexe között. Úgy tűnik, hogy a koraszülötteknél talált mentális fejlődésbeli hátrányhoz, amely főleg a fiúkat érinti, részben a kisebb anyai tanítói igyekezet is hozzájárul.
1. Aylward, G. P. (2003): Neonatology, prematurity, NICU, and developmental issues. In: M. C. Roberts (ed.), Handbook of Pediatric Psychology. New York, Guilford.
2. Bakeman, R., Brown, J. V. (1980): Early interaction: consequences for social and mental development at three years. Child Development, 51, 437–447.
3. Barnard, K. E., Bee, H. L., Hammond, M. A. (1984): Developmental changes in maternal interactions with term and preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 101–113.
4. Barrera, M. E., Rosenbaum, P. L., Cunningham, C. E. (1987): Corrected and uncorrected Bayley scores: longitudinal developmental patterns in low and high birthweight preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 337–346.
5. Bayley, N. (1993): Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York, Psychological Corporation.
6. Beckwith, L., Cohen, S. E. (1978): Preterm birth: Hazardous obstetrical and postnatal events as releted to cargiver-infant behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 403–411.
7. Beckwith, L., Rodning, C., Cohen, S. (1992): Preterm children at early adolescence and continuity and discontinuity in maternal responsiveness from infancy. Child Development, 63, 1198–1208.
8. Bradley, R. H., Corwyn, R. F., Burchinal, M., Pipes-McAdoo, H., Garcia-Coll, C. (2001): The home environments of children in the United States, Part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development, 72, 1868–1886.
9. Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Hooper, S., Zeisel, S. A. (2000): Cumulative risk and early cognitive development: A comparison of statistical risk models. Developmental Psychology, 36, 793–807.
10. Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A., Ahluwalia J. (2000): Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113–118.
11. Crawford, J. W. (1982): Mother-infant interaction in premature and full-term infants. Child Development, 53, 957–962.
12. Crnic, K. A., Ragozin, A. S., Greenberg, M. T., Robinson, N. M., Basham, R. B. (1983): Social interaction and developmental competence of preterm and full-term infants during the first year of life. Child Development, 54, 1199–1210.
13. Drillien, C. M., Thomson, A. S. M., Burgoyne, K. (1980): Low-birthweight children at early school age: A longitudinal study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 22, 26–47.
14. Field, T. M. (1977): Effects of early separation, interactive deficits, and experimental manipulations on infant-mother face-to-face interaction. Child Development, 48, 763–771.
15. Garner, P. W., Landry, S. H., Richardson, M. A. (1991): The development of joint attention skills in very-low-birth-weight infants across the first 2 years. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 489–495.
16. Gervai Judit (2005): A Budapesti Családvizsgálat. Alkalmazott Pszichológia, VII. 4, 5–13.
17. Goldberg, S., DiVitto, B. (2002): Parenting children born preterm. In: M. H. Bornstein (ed.), Handbook of Parenting. Vol. 3, Mahwah, N.J., Erlbaum, 339–360.
18. Goldberg, S., Lojkasek, M., Gartner, G., Corter, C. (1989): Maternal responsiveness and social development in preterm infants. In: M. H. Bornstein (ed), Maternal Responsiveness: Characteristics and Consequences. New Directions for Child Development, No. 43. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass, 89–103.
19. Greenberg, M., Crnic, K. A. (1988): Longitudinal predictors of developmental status and social interaction in premature and full-term infants at age two. Child Development, 59, 554–570.
20. Greene, J. G., Fox, N. A., Lewis, M. (1983): The relationship between neonatal characteristics and three-month mother-infant interaction in high-risk infants. Child Development, 54, 1286–1296.
21. Gustafson, G. E., Green, J. A., West, M. J. (1979): The infants changing role in mother-infant games: the growth of social skills. Infant-Behavior-and-Development, 2, 301–308.
22. Hindmarsh, G. J., O'Callaghan, M. J., Mohay, H. A., Rogers, Y. M. (2000): Gender differences in cognitive abilities at 2 years in ELBW infants. Early Human Development, 60, 115–122.
23. Jarvis, P. A., Myers, B. J., Creasey, G. L. (1989): The effects of infants’ illness on mothers’ interactions with prematures at 4 and 8 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 1, 25–35.
24. Kalmár Magda , Boronkai J. (2001): Az otthoni környezet minőségének szerepe koraszülött gyerekek hosszútávú értelmi fejlődésében. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle, 56, 387–410.
25. Kalmár Magda (2007): Az intelligencia alakulásának előrejelezhetősége és váratlan fordulatai. Rizikómentesen született, valamint koraszülött gyerekek követésének tanulságai. Budapest, ELTE Eötvös Kiadó.
26. Landry, S. H. (1986): Preterm infant's responses in early joint attention interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 1–14.
27. Landry, S. H., Chapieski, M. L. (1988): Visual attention during toy exploration in preterm infants: Effects of medical risk and maternal interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 187–204.
28. Landry, S. H., Garner, P. W., Swank, P. R., Baldwin, C. D. (1996): Effects of maternal scaffolding during joint toy play with preterm and full-term infants. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 42, 177–199.
29. Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Swank, P. R. (1997): Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1040–1053.
30. Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Swank, P. R. (1998): The relation of change in maternal interactive styles to the developing social competence of full-term and preterm children. Child Development, 69, 1, 105–123.
31. Lindahl, L. B., Heiman, M. (1997): Social proximity in early mother-infant interactions: Implications for gender differences? Early Development and Parenting, 6, 2, 83–88.
32. Littman, B., Parmelee, A. H. Jr. (1978): Madical correlates of infant development. Pediatrics, 61, 470–474.
33. Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P. (2002): Foetal testosterone and eye contact in 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 327–335.
34. Magill-Evans, J., Harrison, M. J., Burke, S. O. (1999): Parent-child interactions and development of toddlers born preterm. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 21, 292.
35. Malatesta, C. Z., Grigoryev, P., Lamb, C., Albin, M., Culver, C. (1986): Emotion socialization and development in preterm and full-term infants. Child Development, 57, 316–330.
36. Meadows, S. (1996): Parenting Behaviour and Children's Cognitive Development. Howe, U.K., Erlbaum Taylor and Francis.
37. Minde, K., Perotta, M., Marton, P. (1985): Maternal caretaking and play with full-term and premature infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 2, 231–244.
38. Minde, K. (1993): Prematurity and serious medical illness in infancy: Implications for development and interventions. In: C. H. Jr. Zeanah (ed.), Handbook of Infant Mental Health. New York, The Guilford Press, 87–105.
39. Neisser, U., Boodoo, D. F., Loehlink, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J., Urbins, F. (1996): Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.
40. Ney Krisztina , Szöllősi Ágnes, Fenes Ditte, Gervai Judit (2005): A szülő-csecsemő interakciók jellemzői és kódolási lehetőségei. Alkalmazott Pszichológia, 7 (4), 38–48.
41. Olson, S. L., Bates, J. E., Kaskie, B. (1992): Caregiver-infant interaction antecedents of children's school-age cognitive ability. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38, 309–330.
42. Poehlmann, J., Fiese, B. H. (2001): Parent-infant interaction as a mediator of the relation between neo-natal risk status and 12-month cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 24, 171–188.
43. Reissland, N., Shepherd, J., Stephenson, T. (1999): Maternal verbal Interaction in different situations with infants born prematurely or at term. Infant and Child Development, 8, 39–48.
44. Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., Jankowski, J. J., Van Rossem, R. (2008): A cognitive cascade in infancy: Pathways from prematurity to later mental development. Intelligence, 36, 367–378.
45. Ruske, T. M., Rifkin, L., Stewart, A. L., Townsend, J. P., Roth, S. C., Wyatt, J. S., Murray, R. M. (2001): Neuropsychological outcome at adolescence of very preterm birth and its relation to brain structure. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 226–233.
46. Saltaris, C., Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., Karp, J. A., Schwartzman, A. E., Ledingham, J. E. (2004): Nurturing cognitive competence in preschoolers: A longitudinal study of intergenerational continuity and risk. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, 105–115.
47. Samsom, J. F., Groot, L., Cranendonk, A., Bezemer, D., Lafeber H.N., Fetter W. P. F. (2002): Neuromotor function and school performance in 7-year-old children born as high-risk preterm infants. Journal of Child Neurology, 17, 5, 325–332.
48. Schneider, W., Wolke, D., Schlagmüller, M., Meyer, R. (2004): Pathways to school achievement in very preterm and full term children. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 19, 4, 385–406.
49. Smith, K. E., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Baldwin, C. D., Denson, S. E., Wildin, S. (1996): The relation of medical risk and maternal stimulation with preterm infants’ cognitive, language, and living skills. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 855–864.
50. Smith, L., Ulvund, S. E. (2003): The role of joint attention in later development among preterm children: Linkages between early and middle childhood. Social Development, 12 (2), 222–234.
51. Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F. (1989): infant-mother face-to-face interaction: Age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Development, 60, 85–92.
52. UNICEF (2007): Child poverty in perspective: An overview of child well-being in rich countries. Innocenti Report Card, 7.
53. Van Beek, Y., Hopkins, B., Hoeksma, J. B. (1994): Development of communicative behaviors in preterm infants: the effects of birthweight status and gestational age. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 107–117.
54. Van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Wijnroks, L., Jongmans, M. J. (2008): Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: A review. Infant Behavior and Development, 31 (3), 333–351.
55. Wijnroks, L. (1998): Early maternal stimulation and the development of cognitive competence and attention of preterm infants. Early Development and Parenting, 7, 19–30.
56. Yamokoski, C. A., Jaquay, C., Smith, P. H., Bernieri, F. J., Dixon, W. E. (2000): Measuring interactional synchrony: Context plays a role. Poster presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies. 2000, July, Brighton, UK.
57. Zelkowitz, P., Papageorgiou, A. (1996): Childrearing attitudes among parents of very low birthweight and normal birthweight children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 17, 84–89.