Authors:
Shan-Shan Ma Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Shan-Shan Ma in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Chiang-Shan R. Li Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Department of Neuroscience, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA

Search for other papers by Chiang-Shan R. Li in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Sheng Zhang Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA

Search for other papers by Sheng Zhang in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Patrick D. Worhunsky Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA

Search for other papers by Patrick D. Worhunsky in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Nan Zhou Faculty of Education, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Nan Zhou in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jin-Tao Zhang State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Beijing Key Lab of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Jin-Tao Zhang in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4807-1196
,
Lu Liu Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
German Institute of Human Nutrition Potsdam-Rehbruecke, 14558 Nuthetal, Germany

Search for other papers by Lu Liu in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Yuan-Wei Yao State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
Einstein Center for Neurosciences Berlin, Charitéplatz 1, 10117 Berlin, Germany

Search for other papers by Yuan-Wei Yao in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Xiao-Yi Fang Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Xiao-Yi Fang in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9494-0997
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Deficits in cognitive control represent a core feature of addiction. Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) offers an ideal model to study the mechanisms underlying cognitive control deficits in addiction, eliminating the confounding effects of substance use. Studies have reported behavioral and neural deficits in reactive control in IGD, but it remains unclear whether individuals with IGD are compromised in proactive control or behavioral adjustment by learning from the changing contexts.

Methods

Here, fMRI data of 21 male young adults with IGD and 21 matched healthy controls (HC) were collected during a stop-signal task. We employed group independent component analysis to investigate group differences in temporally coherent, large-scale functional network activities during post-error slowing, the typical type of behavioral adjustments. We also employed a Bayesian belief model to quantify the trial-by-trial learning of the likelihood of stop signal – P(Stop) – a broader process underlying behavioral adjustment, and identified the alterations in functional network responses to P(Stop).

Results

The results showed diminished engagement of the fronto-parietal network during post-error slowing, and weaker activity in the ventral attention and anterior default mode network in response to P(Stop) in IGD relative to HC.

Discussion and conclusions

These results add to the literatures by suggesting deficits in updating and anticipating conflicts as well as in behavioral adjustment according to contextual information in individuals with IGD.

Abstract

Background and aims

Deficits in cognitive control represent a core feature of addiction. Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) offers an ideal model to study the mechanisms underlying cognitive control deficits in addiction, eliminating the confounding effects of substance use. Studies have reported behavioral and neural deficits in reactive control in IGD, but it remains unclear whether individuals with IGD are compromised in proactive control or behavioral adjustment by learning from the changing contexts.

Methods

Here, fMRI data of 21 male young adults with IGD and 21 matched healthy controls (HC) were collected during a stop-signal task. We employed group independent component analysis to investigate group differences in temporally coherent, large-scale functional network activities during post-error slowing, the typical type of behavioral adjustments. We also employed a Bayesian belief model to quantify the trial-by-trial learning of the likelihood of stop signal – P(Stop) – a broader process underlying behavioral adjustment, and identified the alterations in functional network responses to P(Stop).

Results

The results showed diminished engagement of the fronto-parietal network during post-error slowing, and weaker activity in the ventral attention and anterior default mode network in response to P(Stop) in IGD relative to HC.

Discussion and conclusions

These results add to the literatures by suggesting deficits in updating and anticipating conflicts as well as in behavioral adjustment according to contextual information in individuals with IGD.

Introduction

Internet gaming disorder (IGD) represents a putative behavioral addiction that has been included as a condition deserving further studies by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Recently, gaming disorder was also included in 11th Edition of International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) (World Health Organization, 2020). Relatively free from the confounding effects of substance use, IGD offers an ideal model to study the neural mechanisms of addiction (Cho et al., 2014). Deficits in cognitive control are central to etiological processes of addiction, including IGD. Cognitive control has been studied with a myriad of behavioral paradigms and comprises both reactive and proactive processes (Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2016; Wang et al., 2018). Extant research has largely focused on the behavioral and neural bases of reactive processes, such as response inhibition (Argyriou, Davison, & Lee, 2017; Chen et al., 2015), and few studies investigated whether contextual learning and behavioral adjustment, a core process of proactive control (Reed, Vile, Osborne, Romano, & Truzoli, 2015; Verbruggen, Stevens, & Chambers, 2014), may be compromised in IGD.

In experimental psychology, the stop signal task (SST) is one of the most widely used paradigms to study behavioral adjustment. In the SST, participants make trial-by-trial adjustment in reaction time (RT) in order to negotiate the conflicting needs to respond to go signal quickly and to avoid making a response in stop trials (Ide, Shenoy, Yu, & Li, 2013). Participants typically slow down in RT in the go trials following a stop error as compared to those following another go trial, and this RT adjustment has been called post-error slowing (PES) (Li et al., 2008). PES reflects behavioral adjustment to the immediate experience of an error, anticipation of a higher likelihood of a stop signal, and consequently a more cautious approach in executing a motor response.

Many studies have focused on the effect of drug use on behavioral and neural response to PES. For example, diminished PES was found after acute injections of alcohol (Bombeke, Schouppe, Duthoo, & Notebaert, 2013) and in opioid (Liao et al., 2014) and cocaine (Li, Milivojevic, Kemp, Hong, & Sinha, 2006) dependent individuals. By comparing post-error go trials with increased RT (pSEi) and those that did not increase in RT (pSEni), Li and colleagues demonstrated decreased activation in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during PES in alcohol dependent as compared to control participants (Li, Luo, Yan, Bergquist, & Sinha, 2009). Other studies showed that long-term alcohol consumption compromised frontal-striatal circuit function, leading to poor post-error behavior adjustment (Lawrence, Luty, Bogdan, Sahakian, & Clark, 2009). Thus, PES may represent a robust behavioral marker of addiction, and it is of importance to examine whether PES is altered in individuals with IGD.

A Bayes-optimal decision-making model has been proposed to incorporate learning in examining behavioral adjustment in the SST. In this model, participants choose to go or stop by combining sensory evidence in a trial and prior belief about the likelihood of a stop trial (Shenoy & Yu, 2011). Specifically, each trial contributes to one’s belief about the likelihood of encountering a subsequent stop trial, P(stop), and one strategically modulates RT accordingly: an increase in P(Stop) predicts a prolonged go trial RT – a sequential effect (Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2015; Ide et al., 2013; Ide, Hu, Zhang, Yu, & Li, 2015; Shenoy & Yu, 2011). It is likely that deficits in behavioral adjustment reflect a specific instance of impaired ability to use contextual information to anticipate stop signals and adjust behavior accordingly. Indeed, an earlier work showed that PES was less correlated with the Bayesian account of sequential effect in cocaine dependent relative to control subjects (Ide et al., 2015). Previous imaging studies have found that bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule and anterior pre-supplementary motor area are modulated by the probability of stop trials (Chikazoe et al., 2009; Hu, Ide, Zhang, Sinha, & Li, 2015). Neural deficits have also been shown in the left insula, bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, and inferior parietal lobule in response to P(Stop) in occasional stimulant including methylphenidate users and in alcohol dependent individuals performing the SST (Harle et al., 2014; Hu, Ide, Zhang, Sinha, et al., 2015). Thus, examining Bayesian learning would advance our understanding of the psychological processes of behavioral adjustment in IGD.

General linear modeling (GLM) represents a major approach in identifying regional brain activations during cognitive performance and has been widely used in studies of the SST (Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2015; Li et al., 2009). In contrast, as a data-driven approach, independent component analysis (ICA) reveals “hidden” factors from a set of measurements such that the sources of the observed data are maximally independent. Requiring no prior knowledge, ICA helps in identifying functional networks rather than individual brain regions of importance to psychological processes (Zhang & Li, 2012). Group ICA (gICA) may identify functional networks that participate in multiple cognitive processes (Xu et al., 2013) that elude detection by GLM-based analyses (Xu, Calhoun, & Potenza, 2015). Previous research using gICA has revealed functional networks underlying SST performance (Zhang & Li, 2012). For instance, an earlier study demonstrated disengagement of a network comprising the inferior frontal cortex and intraparietal sulcus during PES in substance-abusing individuals (Elton et al., 2014).

In the current study, we employed gICA to examine fMRI data of the SST and compared IGD subjects and healthy controls (HCs) in neural responses during post-error behavioral adjustment. We also investigated the neural representation of probabilistic expectations of the stop signal during the SST. According to previous findings, we hypothesized that IGD subjects relative to HCs would show deficient activity in the fronto-parietal network (FPN), a circuit widely implicated in action withholding/cancellation (Zhang, Geng, & Lee, 2017), during PES. We also posited attenuated activity in the ventral attention network (VAN), a network that partakes in monitoring environmental information (Congdon et al., 2010; Zhang, Hu, Bednarski, Erdman, & Li, 2014), in response to P(Stop) in individuals with IGD.

Materials and methods

Participants and clinical assessments

Considering the higher occurrence of IGD among men and to avoid the potential confounds of gender (Ko et al., 2009), we recruited 22 male participants with IGD and 22 HC for the current study. Inclusion criteria included 18–30 years of age, right-handed, not currently taking any psychotropic medications, no contraindications for MRI, and no current or previous psychiatric or neurological illness as assessed with the Chinese version of the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Si et al., 2009). Additional inclusion criteria for the IGD group included: (i) meeting at least 5 of the 9 diagnostic criteria for IGD as proposed in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Ko et al., 2014); (ii) scores ≥50 on Young’s online Internet addiction test (IAT) (Young, 2016); (iii) engagement in Internet gaming for >20 hours per week for a minimum of 1 year; and (iv) Internet gaming as their primary online activity. Additional inclusion criteria for HC included: (i) meeting fewer than 5 of the 9 diagnostic criteria for IGD in the DSM-5; (ii) <50 on the IAT; and (iii) never engaging in Internet gaming. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck, Brown, Epstein, & Steer, 1988) and Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck, Erbaugh, Ward, Mock, & Mendelsohn, 1961) were used to assess anxiety and depressive symptoms.

Imaging sessions with head movements exceeding 3 mm in translation or 3° in rotation were removed. In addition, performance with a more than 50% rate of response in the go trials, 40–60% stop success rate was deemed compliant with instructions (Jilka et al., 2014). Imaging data of two subjects were excluded for these reasons from subsequent data analysis, and the final sample comprised 21 IGDs and 21 HCs.

Stop-signal task and behavioral data analyses

Participants performed a stop signal task (Zhang & Li, 2012). They were instructed to quickly press the button when they saw the go signal, while in 25% of these trials, a stop signal would come up after the go signal (stop trials), and they should inhibit the response to go signal when they encountered the stop signal. Go and stop trials were inter-mixed randomly in presentation. A fixation dot appeared on the screen to begin in a trial, and after a fore-period varying from 1 s to 5 s, the dot became a circle – the go signal – instructing a response. The circle disappeared at button press or after 1 s if the participant failed to respond. In approximately one quarter of trials, the circle was followed by a ‘cross’ – the stop signal – prompting participants to withhold button press. The trial terminated at button press or after 1 s if participant successfully inhibited the response. The time between the go and stop signals, the stop signal delay (SSD), started at 200 ms and varied from one stop trial to the next according to a staircase procedure, increasing or decreasing by 64 ms each after a successful or failed stop trial. Participants were trained briefly out of the scanner before imaging to ensure that they understood the task. To balance the statistic power and fatigue that may result from a lengthy task, we shorted the duration of each session (relative to (Zhang & Li, 2012)), with three 8-min sessions of the task. The total number of trials varied slightly between subjects, based on the subjects’ response rate and actual inter-stimulus-interval. The average trial numbers of each session of each individual ranged from 78 to 82 (77.77 ± 1.75).

We computed the RT difference between the go trials that followed a stop error (pSE) and those that followed another go trial (pG) to quantify the extent of PES (PES effect). We used a dynamic Bayesian model to estimate the probability of stop signal, or P(Stop). The model assumes that subjects anticipate an impending stop signal on each trial, based on prior stimulus history. They believe that stop signal frequency rk on trial k has probability α of being the same as rk−1, and probability (1 − α) of being re-sampled from a prior distribution π(rk) (Angela & Cohen, 2009). Previous studies found that individual learning rates are typically too unstable to produce robust regressors for imaging data analyses (Daw & Doya, 2006; Schonberg, Daw, Joel, & O’Doherty, 2007; Wimmer, Daw, & Shohamy, 2012). Thus, following previous work (Harle et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy & Yu, 2011), we employed the standard model-based fMRI analyses with a fixed set of parameters across all subjects, the mean of the prior distribution = 0.25, scale = 10 and a learning parameter α = 0.8, to characterize P(Stop) and regional responses to P(Stop) (Harle et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2013; Shenoy & Yu, 2011). A sequential effect, was quantified by Pearson correlation between P(Stop) and RT on go trials for individual subjects (Ide et al., 2015).

Image acquisition and preprocessing

MR imaging was conducted with a Siemens Trio 3-Tesla scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). Thirty-four 3.0 mm axial fMRI data were acquired using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with TR = 2,000 ms; TE = 30 ms; flip angle = 85°; matrix size = 64 × 64; resolution= 3 × 3 mm2; FOV = 220*220 mm2. One hundred forty-four high-resolution slices were obtained using T1-weighted sagittal 3-D magnetization prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence: TR = 2,530 ms, TE = 3.39 ms, TI = 1,100 ms, FA= 7°, FOV= 256 × 256 mm, thickness = 1.33 mm, in-plane resolution = 1 × 1 mm2.

First five TRs of each session were discarded, and the images were analyzed using the standard process in DPABI version 4.2 (Yan, Wang, Zuo, & Zang, 2016), including slice timing correction, realignment, normalization to 3 × 3 × 3 mm3 MNI space, and smoothing with a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm at full width half maximum (FWHM).

Generalized linear model for post-error slowing and stop signal anticipation

Generalized linear models were conducted using SPM12 (Welcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12). In the first GLM (GLM 1, G model), to explore the neural responses to post-error slowing, go signal onsets in each trial type were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) and with the temporal derivative of the canonical HRF. Four trial outcomes were distinguished: go success (G), go error (F), stop success (SS), and stop error (SE). G trials were then divided into those that followed a G (pG), and SE (pSE) trial. The pG/pSE trials were further divided into those that increased in RT (pGi/pSEi) and those that did not increase in RT (pGni/pSEni), as compared with the mean RT of all preceding pG trials during each session (Li et al., 2008). The motion parameters were included as regressors of no interest. We also included RT of G trials, SSD of SS and SE trials as parametric modulators (Li et al., 2009).

In the second GLM (GLM 2, F model), to examine the neural correlates of stop signal anticipation, we modeled BOLD signals by convolving the onsets of the fixation point of each G, SS, and SE trial with a canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF) and the temporal derivative of the canonical HRF, with motion parameters included as regressors of no interest. The P(Stop) of G trials [G x P(Stop)], P(Stop) of SS trials [SS x P(Stop)], SSD of SS trials, P(Stop) of SE trials [SE x P(Stop)], SSD of SE trials were included as parametric modulators (Harle et al., 2014; Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2015).

Group independent component analysis and temporal sorting

The spatially processed images were analyzed with group ICA (GIFT, http://mialab.mrn.org/software/gift, version 4.0b). Briefly, data from all sessions of participants were concatenated into a single dataset and reduced in dimensionality using two stages of principal component analysis (PCA) (Zhang & Li, 2012). The data were then separated with an infomax algorithm into 29 independent components, determined by the modified minimal description length (MDL) criteria (Li, Adali, & Calhoun, 2007). Extraction was repeated 20 times using ICASSO to assess the stability of independent components (Himberg, Hyvärinen, & Esposito, 2004). The time course and spatial map for each component were back-reconstructed for each participant. We visually identified a priori networks for our hypotheses (i.e. FPN and VAN) according to previous findings (Laird et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2009).

To examine the relevance of each component to PES, we performed multiple regression temporal sorting to compare component time courses with the modeled event time courses in GLM 1. The resulting beta weights indicated the extent to which a given network was temporally associated with, or ‘engaged’ in each event. Beta weights were averaged across runs for each event for each subject.

Similarly, a multiple regression temporal sorting was performed to compare component time courses with the modeled event time courses in GLM 2. The parametric modulator of P(Stop) ([G x P(Stop)], [SS x P(Stop)], [SE x P(Stop)]) were weighted by the proportion of trial number each of G, SS, and SE trials, respectively, to examine the relevance to P(Stop) of each component ([f x P(Stop)]). A positive value for a certain functional network indicated that the activation of this functional network increased along with the likelihood that a stop signal would appear (Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2015).

Group differences of functional network activities

For PES, we examined the beta weight contrast for ‘pSEi-pSEni’ and ‘(pSEi-pSEni)- (pGi-pGni)’ of the FPNs (the left fronto-parietal network (lFPN) and right fronto-parietal network (rFPN), corresponding to the IC34 and IC60 of Allen et al. (2011) with two-sample t tests. To offer exploratory results, we performed t tests for each component and evaluated the findings with a Bonferroni-corrected threshold. Similarly, the beta weight of [f x P(Stop)] for VAN (corresponding to the IC71 of Allen et al. (2011)) was examined with a two-sample t test to investigate the group difference, and, to offer exploratory results, we performed t tests for each component and evaluated the findings at the same Bonferroni-corrected threshold.

Given the small sample size, we computed the post-hoc power of two-sample t test (two groups) using G*Power (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). In post-hoc analyses, Power (1-β) is computed as a function of α (0.05), the population effect size, and the sample size in our study.

Ethics

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University (study number: ICBIR_A_0075_002). All participants providing written informed consent prior to the study.

Results

Demographic and behavioral characteristics

The demographic characteristics of the IGD and HC are shown in Table 1. Compared with HC, IGD reported higher YIAS score and more frequent Internet gaming, as well as higher levels of anxiety and higher proportion of Alcohol use. IGD and HC both showed post-error slowing (one-t test; IGD: t(20) = 2.29, p = 0.033; HC: t(20) = 2.95, p = 0.008) and sequential effect (one-t test; IGD: t(20) = 3.73, p = 0.001; HC: t(20) = 2.96, p = 0.008), but IGD and HC showed no difference in SST performance measures.

Table 1.

Participants' characteristics and behavioral performance in the SST

IGD (n = 21)HC (n = 21)t/χ2/UP
mean ± SDmean ± SD
Clinical assessments
Age (years)22.29 ± 1.4221.82 ± 1.850.840.405
Education (years)16.14 ± 1.3216.00 ± 1.820.290.772
YIAT score69.62 ± 12.4825.19 ± 6.7414.35<0.001***
Internet gaming (h/wk)31.83 ± 9.645.59a<0.001***
Alcohol use (at least once/month)6b/0.29c13b/0.62c4.71d0.030*
Cigarette use (at least once/month)0b/0.00c0b/0.00c
BAI score4.57 ± 4.821.81 ± 2.802.270.030*
BDI score6.38 ± 7.693.38 ± 4.501.540.131
SST performance
Mean number of trials (per session)78.09 ± 1.9277.46 ± 1.551.160.252
Mean number of G (per session)52.39 ± 5.7048.35 ± 9.361.690.099
Mean number of F (per session)5.14 ± 3.918.88 ± 7.63−2.000.053
Mean number of SS (per session)11.02 ± 1.0311.21 ± 1.33−0.540.591
Mean number of SE (per session)9.54 ± 1.819.02 ± 1.071.140.261
GS %91.10 ± 6.9184.41 ± 13.622.000.054
SS %53.78 ± 4.3955.76 ± 2.32−2.710.073
Mean GoRT (ms)690.42 ± 109.94721.94 ± 77.37−1.070.289
Critical SSD473.63 ± 133.07495.89 ± 113.83−0.580.563
SSRT212.41 ± 56.12226.80 ± 61.77−0.790.434
PES effect (ms)32.92 ± 61.5447.8 ± 81.67−0.660.512
Sequential effect0.15 ± 0.190.09 ± 0.141.230.225
Mean FD0.13 ± 0.050.12 ± 0.060.380.704

Abbreviations: SD = standard deviation; IGD = Internet gaming disorder; HC = healthy control; YIAT = Young's online Internet addiction test; Internet gaming (hrs/wk) = Internet gaming hours per week; BAI = Back Anxiety Inventory; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; Mean number of trials (per session) = the average number of trials per session; Mean number of G (per session) = the average number of successful go trials per session; Mean number of F (per session) = the average number of failed go trials per session; Mean number of SS (per session) = the average number of successful stop trials per session; Mean number of SE (per session) = the average number of stop error trials per session; GS (%) and SS (%) = percentage of successful go and stop trials; Mean GoRT (ms) = the mean reaction time of successful go trials; PES effect (ms) = the RT difference between the go trials that followed a stop error (pSE) and those that followed another go trial (pG); Critical SSD = Critical stop-signal delay; SSRT = Stop-signal reaction time; Sequential effect= Pearson correlation between P(Stop) and RT on go trials for individual subjects; Mean FD = the frame-wise displacement (FD) of head position.

U value.

the number of participants.

the rate of alcohol and cigarette use.

χ2 value.

Group differences in neural response

The activity of two functional networks during post-error slowing and two functional networks during Bayesian learning anticipation of stop signal differed between IGD and HC. Functional networks were labelled based on the primary regions of positive spatial integration in each network according to the known intrinsic connectivity of the networks (Allen et al., 2011; Laird et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2009).

Altered functional networks in post-error slowing

The left (Fig. 1) and right fronto-parietal network (Fig. 2) encompassed the dorsolateral prefrontal cortices and posterior parietal cortex. Beta weights in these networks were lower for the ‘pSEi-pSEni’ contrast (lFPN: t(1,40) = −2.97, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = −0.92, r = −0.42, power = 0.90; rFPN: t(1,40) = −2.54, p = 0.015, Cohen’s d = −0.78, r = −0.36, power = 0.80) and for the ‘(pSEi-pSEni)- (pGi-pGni)’ contrast (lFPN: t(1,40) = −2.55, p = 0.015, Cohen’s d = −0.79, r = −0.37, power = 0.81; rFPN: t(1,40) = −2.08, p = 0.044, Cohen’s d = −0.64, r = −0.31, power = 0.65) in IGD, as compared to HC. Exploratory results showed no significant finding with Bonferroni correction.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

The left fronto-parietal network (lFPN). A: Spatial map displayed at a threshold at p = 0.00001; B: Lower engagement in lFPN during post-error slowing in IGD

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10, 1; 10.1556/2006.2021.00010

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

The right fronto-parietal network (rFPN). A: Spatial map displayed at a threshold at p = 0.00001; B: Lower engagement in rFPN during post-error slowing in IGD

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10, 1; 10.1556/2006.2021.00010

Altered functional networks responding to P(Stop)

The ventral attention network, comprising the temporo-parietal junction, insula, supplementary motor area, inferior frontal gyrus and inferior parietal lobule, showed lower beta weight for [f x P(Stop)] in IGD relative to HC (t(1,40) = −2.76, p = 0.009, Cohen’s d = −0.85, r = −0.39, power = 0.86; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.

The ventral attention network (VAN) coding of P(Stop). A: Spatial map displayed at a threshold at p = 0.00001; B: Lower engagement in ventral attention network coding of P(Stop) in IGD

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10, 1; 10.1556/2006.2021.00010

Exploratory results showed that the beta weight for [f x P(Stop)] of anterior default mode network (aDMN), corresponding to IC25 in Allen et al. (2011) and comprising the medial frontal gyrus, was decreased in IGD relative to HC (Bonferroni corrected, t(1,40) = −3.76, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = −1.16, r = −0.50, power = 0.98; Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.

The coding of P(Stop) in anterior default mode network (aDMN). A: Spatial map displayed at a threshold at p = 0.00001; B: Higher disengagement in aDMN coding of P(Stop) in IGD

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10, 1; 10.1556/2006.2021.00010

Because IGD reported higher levels of anxiety and higher proportion of alcohol use, we also examined group differences with anxiety score and alcohol use state (1= use, 0 = no use) as covariates. The results were similar with current findings (Supplementary Information Table S1).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first report to identify alterations in functional network activities for proactive control in IGD, a behavioral addiction independent of drug effects. We found, despite indistinguishable behavioral performance, lower engagement of the fronto-parietal network during post-error slowing and weaker activity in the ventral attention network and the anterior default mode network during Bayesian learning in IGD as compared to HC. These results suggested altered neural activity not only during post-error behavioral adjustment, but also during probabilistic expectations of the stop signal. We highlighted the main findings in discussion.

Altered functional networks in post-error slowing

The left and right fronto-parietal network showed lower engagement during PES in IGD. Frontal and parietal cortices respond to cued attention allocation (Luks, Simpson, Dale, & Hough, 2007) and preparatory control of response switching (Rushworth, Paus, & Sipila, 2001). An extensive body of work also provides evidence that activation of middle frontal regions reflects response adjustment strategies to balance the opposing go and stop demands (Chevrier, Noseworthy, & Schachar, 2007), following conflicts or errors (Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004). Further, fronto-parietal circuit (e.g. dorsolateral prefrontal cortices) dysfunction compromised post-signal behavioral adjustment in cocaine and alcohol addicted individuals (Chevrier et al., 2007; Hester, Simoes-Franklin, & Garavan, 2007; Li et al., 2009).

The lFPN and rFPN may support different functional dimensions of cognitive control. Previous studies have proposed that the rFPN is involved in high attentional load stop trials, hence in support of the restraint of actions, while lFPN is involved in low attentional load go trials, hence in support of the cancellation of actions, in the SST (Schachar et al., 2007; Stevens, Kiehl, Pearlson, & Calhoun, 2009; Xu, Calhoun, Pearlson, & Potenza, 2014). Indeed, consistent with the temporal organization of these functional roles, Zhang and Li (2012) also found that the activity of the lFPN significantly lagged in time behind the rFPN during the SST.

Thus, decreased activity in the fronto-parietal network in IGD may indicate that individuals with IGD are unable to utilize this network to adjust response strategies to balance the opposing demands of the go and stop trials to restrain action and to inhibit a motor response following errors.

Altered functional networks responding to P(Stop)

The ventral attention network

IGD showed attenuated activity for Bayesian learning in the ventral attention network. The VAN responds to changes in events and reorients attention to the stop signal (Congdon et al., 2010; Zhang & Li, 2012). Previous studies have implicated many areas in the VAN in updating the probability of stop signal to adjust behavior and compromised activities in these areas in addiction (Hu, Ide, Zhang, Sinha, et al., 2015; Shi et al., 2019). For example, occasional stimulant users demonstrated subtle deficits in anticipation of a stop signal in the inferior frontal gyrus (Harle et al., 2014). Schlosser et al. (2009) reported that methylphenidate attenuated inferior parietal lobule response to uncertainty when making decisions according to sensory information. Thus, the disruption of the VAN suggests lower efficiency of bottom-up signal filtering, resulting in difficulties to adapt behavioral responses to environmental needs (Collantoni et al., 2016).

The anterior default mode network

The anterior default mode network showed greater disengagement during stop signal anticipation. The aDMN was deactivated, consistent with earlier findings of downregulation during a cognitively demanding task (Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss, & Menon, 2003). However, aDMN is also involved in bottom-up cue-elicited attentional processes during cognitive control (Bonini et al., 2014), particularly in response to transiently presented stimuli and stimulus complexity (McKiernan, Kaufman, Kucera-Thompson, & Binder, 2003), and self-processing (Molnar-Szakacs & Uddin, 2013). As a critical area of the aDMN, the medial frontal cortices, showed increasing activation to stop signal likelihood (Hu, Ide, Zhang, & Li, 2015; Vink et al., 2005).

Altered intra-aDMN functional connectivity may be associated with impaired awareness (Blumenfeld et al., 2003), and diminished self-referential thinking may hinder behavioral regulation after unexpected feedback (Patterson & Newman, 1993). In addition, altered activity and functional connectivity of regions within this network (e.g. the ventromedial prefrontal cortex) were also found during error processing and resting-state in addictive individuals (Sutherland, McHugh, Pariyadath, & Stein, 2012; Worhunsky et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014). Therefore, the greater disengagement of aDMN found in the present study may reflect less attentional and monitoring/reflectivity processing to unexpected cues and, as a result, suboptimal readiness to withhold response, in IGD.

General discussion

The current findings are in accordance with a previous review of the SST, which implicated the fronto-parietal network in action withholding/cancellation and the ventral attention network in interference resolution (Zhang et al., 2017). Our findings were also consistent with the previous findings that substance dependent participants showed failure to use con-textual information to update the probability of the stop signal (Harle et al., 2014; Ide et al., 2015) and to adjust behavior after errors (Hester et al., 2007; Li et al., 2009). It may suggest that these impairments might be general across different types of addiction.

IGD in this study reported higher levels of anxiety and alcohol use than HC, though none met criteria for an anxiety or alcohol use disorder as assessed with the Chinese version of the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (Si et al., 2009). We controlled anxiety score and drink state in data analyses; however, the potential influence of anxiety and alcohol use on the present findings cannot be ruled out entirely. Based on problem behavior theory and previous studies, comorbidities of anxiety and alcohol use are common in IGD (Demirci, Akgönül, & Akpinar, 2015; Ho et al., 2014; Ko et al., 2008; Tonioni et al., 2012). We hope the current findings would reflect the broad populations of IGD.

The results of conventional GLM did not survive the corrected threshold. However, the main regions revealed with voxel-wise p < 0.005 (uncorrected) were consistent with the ICA findings (Supplementary Information Table S2). ICA identifies regional signals that contribute to functional networks, whereas GLM examines the aggregate signal in a particular region. It is worth noting that different functional networks may share the same brain regions, suggesting that individual brain regions may serve concurrent but different or even opposite functional roles when engaged as part of different functional networks (Xu et al., 2013). Thus, ICA is able to identify alterations in task-related, concurrent, but opposite changes in time courses in the same brain regions that may not be detected by GLM-based analyses (Xu et al., 2015). Furthermore, this highlights the complexity of regional activations and the utility of ICA in considering regional interactions to provide nuanced understanding of the function of a brain region.

Limitations of the study

Some limitations need to be considered for the current study. Firstly, we recruited only male participants in the study, which limits the generalizability of the current findings to female IGD. Secondly, IGD participants in the present study represented a sample of students enrolled in Chinese universities and thus the findings may not be generalized to a broader clinical population. Thirdly, although the results showed acceptable effect size and post-hoc power, the current findings remain to be verified in the future, considering the limited sample size and relatively small trial number of each participant. Finally, this is a cross-sectional study, and whether the alterations in functional network engagement represent a consequence of IGD or a risk factor of IGD remains to be determined.

Conclusions

The current study investigated functional network activities for behavioral adjustment and Bayesian learning from the changing contexts in IGD. We found decreased engagement in functional networks for post-error response restraint and behavioral adjustment; and weaker activity in functional networks for attentional monitoring and sensory salience evaluation during Bayesian learning for behavioral adjustment in IGD. Together, IGD showed not only deficits in trial-by-trial behavioral adjustments, but also a broader impairment to use contextual information to expect and update the adjustment demand. It remains to be seen whether these findings would be shared across drug and behavioral addictions.

Funding sources

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 31871122, 31170990, and 31700966) and Open Project grant from the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (to Sheng, Zhang).

Authors’ contribution

JTZ, and XYF were responsible for the study concept and design. SSM, LL, and YWY contributed to the data acquisition. SSM, PW, and SZ assisted with data analysis and interpretation of findings. SSM drafted the manuscript. JTZ, CSRL, NZ, and XYF provided critical revision of the manuscript. All authors critically reviewed the content and approved final version for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interest.

Supplementary material

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00010.

References

  • Allen, E. A., Erhardt, E. B., Damaraju, E., Gruner, W., Segall, J. M., Silva, R. F., et al. (2011). A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Angela, J. Y., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Sequential effects: Superstition or rational behavior? Paper presented at the advances in neural information processing systems.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Argyriou, E., Davison, C. B., & Lee, T. T. C. (2017). Response inhibition and internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 71, 5460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.02.026.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety – Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.893.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T., Erbaugh, J., Ward, C. H., Mock, J., & Mendelsohn, M. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6), 561-&.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blumenfeld, H., Westerveld, M., Ostroff, R. B., Vanderhill, S. D., Freeman, J., Necochea, A., et al. (2003). Selective frontal, parietal, and temporal networks in generalized seizures. Neuroimage, 19(4), 15561566. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00204-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bombeke, K., Schouppe, N., Duthoo, W., & Notebaert, W. (2013). The effect of alcohol and placebo on post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00003.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bonini, F., Burle, B., Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Regis, J., Chauvel, P., & Vidal, F. (2014). Action monitoring and medial frontal cortex: Leading role of supplementary motor area. Science, 343(6173), 888891. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1247412.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chen, C. Y., Huang, M. F., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., et al. (2015). Brain correlates of response inhibition in Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 69(4), 201209. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12224.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chevrier, A. D., Noseworthy, M. D., & Schachar, R. (2007). Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 28(12), 13471358. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20355.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chikazoe, J., Jimura, K., Hirose, S., Yamashita, K., Miyashita, Y., & Konishi, S. (2009). Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(50), 1587015877.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cho, H., Kwon, M., Choi, J.-H., Lee, S.-K., Choi, J. S., Choi, S.-W., et al. (2014). Development of the Internet addiction scale based on the Internet Gaming Disorder criteria suggested in DSM-5. Addictive Behaviors, 39(9), 13611366.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collantoni, E., Michelon, S., Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Titton, F., Manara, R., et al. (2016). Functional connectivity correlates of response inhibition impairment in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging, 247, 916.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Congdon, E., Mumford, J. A., Cohen, J. R., Galvan, A., Aron, A. R., Xue, G., et al. (2010). Engagement of large-scale networks is related to individual differences in inhibitory control. Neuroimage, 53(2), 653663.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Daw, N. D., & Doya, K. (2006). The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 199204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.006.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demirci, K., Akgönül, M., & Akpinar, A. (2015). Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(2), 8592.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elton, A., Young, J., Smitherman, S., Gross, R. E., Mletzko, T., & Kilts, C. D. (2014). Neural network activation during a stop-signal task discriminates cocaine-dependent from non-drug-abusing men. Addiction Biology, 19(3), 427438. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12011.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harle, K. M., Shenoy, P., Stewart, J. L., Tapert, S. F., Yu, A. J., & Paulus, M. P. (2014). Altered neural processing of the need to stop in young adults at risk for stimulant dependence. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 45674580. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.2297-13.2014.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hester, R., Simoes-Franklin, C., & Garavan, H. (2007). Post-error behavior in active cocaine users: Poor awareness of errors in the presence of intact performance adjustments. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(9), 19741984. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301326.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Himberg, J., Hyvärinen, A., & Esposito, F. (2004). Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage, 22(3), 12141222.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ho, R. C., Zhang, M. W. B., Tsang, T. Y., Toh, A. H., Pan, F., Lu, Y. X., et al. (2014). The association between internet addiction and psychiatric co-morbidity: A meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 14.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Anticipating conflict: Neural correlates of a Bayesian belief and its motor consequence. Neuroimage, 119, 286295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.032.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., & Li, C. R. (2016). The right superior frontal gyrus and individual variation in proactive control of impulsive response. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(50), 1268812696. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1175-16.2016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., Sinha, R., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Conflict anticipation in alcohol dependence – A model-based fMRI study of stop signal task. Neuroimage-Clinical, 8, 3950.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ide, J. S., Hu, S. E., Zhang, S., Yu, A. J., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Impaired Bayesian learning for cognitive control in cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 151, 220227.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ide, J. S., Shenoy, P., Yu, A. J., & Li, C. S. (2013). Bayesian prediction and evaluation in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 20392047. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2201-12.2013.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jilka, S. R., Scott, G., Ham, T., Pickering, A., Bonnelle, V., Braga, R. M., et al. (2014). Damage to the salience network and interactions with the default mode network. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(33), 1079810807. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-14.2014.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, S. H., Wang, P. W., Chen, C. S., & Yen, C. F. (2014). Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 53, 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C.-H., Yen, J.-Y., Chen, S.-H., Yang, M.-J., Lin, H.-C., & Yen, C.-F. (2009). Proposed diagnostic criteria and the screening and diagnosing tool of Internet addiction in college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(4), 378384.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., Weng, C. C., & Chen, C. C. (2008). The association between internet addiction and problematic alcohol use in adolescents: The problem behavior model. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 11(5), 571576.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laird, A. R., Fox, P. M., Eickhoff, S. B., Turner, J. A., Ray, K. L., McKay, D. R., et al. (2011). Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(12), 40224037. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00077.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology, 207(1), 163172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, Y. O., Adali, T., & Calhoun, V. D. (2007). Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance Imaging data. Human Brain Mapping, 28(11), 12511266.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S. R., Huang, C., Yan, P. S., Paliwal, P., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2008). Neural correlates of post-error slowing during a stop signal task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(6), 10211029. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20071.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C.-S. R., Luo, X., Yan, P., Bergquist, K., & Sinha, R. (2009). Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: Neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(4), 740750. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00891.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S., Milivojevic, V., Kemp, K., Hong, K., & Sinha, R. (2006). Performance monitoring and stop signal inhibition in abstinent patients with cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 85(3), 205212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.04.008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liao, D. L., Huang, C. Y., Hu, S., Fang, S. C., Wu, C. S., Chen, W. T., et al. (2014). Cognitive control in opioid dependence and methadone maintenance treatment. PloS One, 9(4), e94589. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094589.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luks, T. L., Simpson, G. V., Dale, C. L., & Hough, M. G. (2007). Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing. Neuroimage, 35(2), 949958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.041.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394408. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321593117.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Uddin, L. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: Interactions with the mirror neuron system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 571. https://doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2013.00571.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aversive events – Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100(4), 716736. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.716.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reed, P., Vile, R., Osborne, L. A., Romano, M., & Truzoli, R. (2015). Problematic internet usage and immune function. PloS One, 10(8), e0134538. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Segalowitz, S. J., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 129140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rushworth, M. F. S., Paus, T., & Sipila, P. K. (2001). Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(14), 52625271. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.21-14-05262.2001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., Robaey, P., Chen, S., Ickowicz, A., & Barr, C. (2007). Restraint and cancellation: Multiple inhibition deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(2), 229238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9075-2.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schlosser, R. G. M., Nenadic, I., Wagner, G., Zysset, S., Koch, K., & Sauer, H. (2009). Dopaminergic modulation of brain systems subserving decision making under uncertainty: A study with fMRI and methylphenidate challenge. Synapse, 63(5), 429442.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schonberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(47), 1286012867. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.2496-07.2007.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shenoy, P., & Yu, A. J. (2011). Rational decision-making in inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 48. https://doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2011.00048.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shi, L., Lou, W. T., Wong, A., Zhang, F., Abrigo, J., Chu, W. C. W., et al. (2019). Neural evidence for long-term marriage shaping the functional brain network organization between couples. Neuroimage, 199, 8792. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.058.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Si, T.-M., Shu, L., Dang, W.-M., Se, Y.-A., Chen, J.-X., Dong, W.-T., et al. (2009). Evaluation of the reliability and validity of Chinese version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview in patients with mental disorders. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 23(7), 493503.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(31), 1304013045.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens, M. C., Kiehl, K. A., Pearlson, G. D., & Calhoun, V. D. (2009). Brain network dynamics during error commission. Human Brain Mapping, 30(1), 2437. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20478.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sutherland, M. T., McHugh, M. J., Pariyadath, V., & Stein, E. A. (2012). Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: Lessons learned and a road ahead. Neuroimage, 62(4), 22812295.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tonioni, F., D’Alessandris, L., Lai, C., Martinelli, D., Corvino, S., Vasale, M., et al. (2012). Internet addiction: Hours spent online, behaviors and psychological symptoms. General Hospital Psychiatry, 34(1), 8087.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Stevens, T., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). Proactive and reactive stopping when distracted: An attentional account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 12951300. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036542.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vink, M., Kahn, R. S., Raemaekers, M., van den Heuvel, M., Boersma, M., & Ramsey, N. F. (2005). Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Human Brain Mapping, 25(3), 336344. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20111.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, W., Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhornitsky, S., Zhang, S., Yu, A. J., et al. (2018). Motor preparation disrupts proactive control in the stop signal task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 151. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00151.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wimmer, G. E., Daw, N. D., & Shohamy, D. (2012). Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 10921104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08017.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Worhunsky, P. D., Stevens, M. C., Carroll, K. M., Rounsaville, B. J., Calhoun, V. D., Pearlson, G. D., et al. (2013). Functional brain networks associated with cognitive control, cocaine dependence, and treatment outcome. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(2), 477488. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029092.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2020). ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics (ICD-11 MMS).

  • Xu, J. S., Calhoun, V. D., Pearlson, G. D., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Opposite modulation of brain functional networks implicated at low vs. high demand of attention and working memory. PloS One, 9(1), e87078. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087078.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, J. S., Calhoun, V. D., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). The absence of task-related increases in BOLD signal does not equate to absence of task-related brain activation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 240, 125127.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, J. S., Zhang, S., Calhoun, V. D., Monterosso, J., Li, C. S. R., Worhunsky, P. D., et al. (2013). Task-related concurrent but opposite modulations of overlapping functional networks as revealed by spatial IA. Neuroimage, 79, 6271.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yan, C. G., Wang, X. D., Zuo, X. N., & Zang, Y. F. (2016). DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (Resting-State) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics, 14(3), 339351.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. (2016). Internet addiction test (IAT): Stoelting.

  • Zhang, R. B., Geng, X. J., & Lee, T. M. C. (2017). Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: An fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 222(9), 39733990.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, S., Hu, S., Bednarski, S. R., Erdman, E., & Li, C. S. R. (2014). Error-related functional connectivity of the thalamus in cocaine dependence. Neuroimage-Clinical, 4, 585592.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, S., & Li, C. S. R. (2012). Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: Independent component analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 89104. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21197.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Supplementary Materials

  • Allen, E. A., Erhardt, E. B., Damaraju, E., Gruner, W., Segall, J. M., Silva, R. F., et al. (2011). A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 5, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Angela, J. Y., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Sequential effects: Superstition or rational behavior? Paper presented at the advances in neural information processing systems.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Argyriou, E., Davison, C. B., & Lee, T. T. C. (2017). Response inhibition and internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 71, 5460. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.02.026.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T., Brown, G., Epstein, N., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety – Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.893.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T., Erbaugh, J., Ward, C. H., Mock, J., & Mendelsohn, M. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6), 561-&.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blumenfeld, H., Westerveld, M., Ostroff, R. B., Vanderhill, S. D., Freeman, J., Necochea, A., et al. (2003). Selective frontal, parietal, and temporal networks in generalized seizures. Neuroimage, 19(4), 15561566. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00204-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bombeke, K., Schouppe, N., Duthoo, W., & Notebaert, W. (2013). The effect of alcohol and placebo on post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00003.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bonini, F., Burle, B., Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Regis, J., Chauvel, P., & Vidal, F. (2014). Action monitoring and medial frontal cortex: Leading role of supplementary motor area. Science, 343(6173), 888891. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1247412.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chen, C. Y., Huang, M. F., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., et al. (2015). Brain correlates of response inhibition in Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 69(4), 201209. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12224.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chevrier, A. D., Noseworthy, M. D., & Schachar, R. (2007). Dissociation of response inhibition and performance monitoring in the stop signal task using event-related fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 28(12), 13471358. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20355.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chikazoe, J., Jimura, K., Hirose, S., Yamashita, K., Miyashita, Y., & Konishi, S. (2009). Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(50), 1587015877.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cho, H., Kwon, M., Choi, J.-H., Lee, S.-K., Choi, J. S., Choi, S.-W., et al. (2014). Development of the Internet addiction scale based on the Internet Gaming Disorder criteria suggested in DSM-5. Addictive Behaviors, 39(9), 13611366.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collantoni, E., Michelon, S., Tenconi, E., Degortes, D., Titton, F., Manara, R., et al. (2016). Functional connectivity correlates of response inhibition impairment in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research-Neuroimaging, 247, 916.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Congdon, E., Mumford, J. A., Cohen, J. R., Galvan, A., Aron, A. R., Xue, G., et al. (2010). Engagement of large-scale networks is related to individual differences in inhibitory control. Neuroimage, 53(2), 653663.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Daw, N. D., & Doya, K. (2006). The computational neurobiology of learning and reward. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 199204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.006.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demirci, K., Akgönül, M., & Akpinar, A. (2015). Relationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(2), 8592.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elton, A., Young, J., Smitherman, S., Gross, R. E., Mletzko, T., & Kilts, C. D. (2014). Neural network activation during a stop-signal task discriminates cocaine-dependent from non-drug-abusing men. Addiction Biology, 19(3), 427438. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12011.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253258. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0135058100.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harle, K. M., Shenoy, P., Stewart, J. L., Tapert, S. F., Yu, A. J., & Paulus, M. P. (2014). Altered neural processing of the need to stop in young adults at risk for stimulant dependence. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 45674580. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.2297-13.2014.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hester, R., Simoes-Franklin, C., & Garavan, H. (2007). Post-error behavior in active cocaine users: Poor awareness of errors in the presence of intact performance adjustments. Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(9), 19741984. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301326.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Himberg, J., Hyvärinen, A., & Esposito, F. (2004). Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage, 22(3), 12141222.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ho, R. C., Zhang, M. W. B., Tsang, T. Y., Toh, A. H., Pan, F., Lu, Y. X., et al. (2014). The association between internet addiction and psychiatric co-morbidity: A meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 14.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Anticipating conflict: Neural correlates of a Bayesian belief and its motor consequence. Neuroimage, 119, 286295. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.032.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., & Li, C. R. (2016). The right superior frontal gyrus and individual variation in proactive control of impulsive response. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(50), 1268812696. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1175-16.2016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhang, S., Sinha, R., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Conflict anticipation in alcohol dependence – A model-based fMRI study of stop signal task. Neuroimage-Clinical, 8, 3950.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ide, J. S., Hu, S. E., Zhang, S., Yu, A. J., & Li, C. S. R. (2015). Impaired Bayesian learning for cognitive control in cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 151, 220227.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ide, J. S., Shenoy, P., Yu, A. J., & Li, C. S. (2013). Bayesian prediction and evaluation in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 20392047. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2201-12.2013.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jilka, S. R., Scott, G., Ham, T., Pickering, A., Bonnelle, V., Braga, R. M., et al. (2014). Damage to the salience network and interactions with the default mode network. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(33), 1079810807. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0518-14.2014.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, S. H., Wang, P. W., Chen, C. S., & Yen, C. F. (2014). Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 53, 103110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.02.008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C.-H., Yen, J.-Y., Chen, S.-H., Yang, M.-J., Lin, H.-C., & Yen, C.-F. (2009). Proposed diagnostic criteria and the screening and diagnosing tool of Internet addiction in college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50(4), 378384.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., Weng, C. C., & Chen, C. C. (2008). The association between internet addiction and problematic alcohol use in adolescents: The problem behavior model. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 11(5), 571576.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laird, A. R., Fox, P. M., Eickhoff, S. B., Turner, J. A., Ray, K. L., McKay, D. R., et al. (2011). Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(12), 40224037. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00077.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., & Clark, L. (2009). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology, 207(1), 163172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-009-1645-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, Y. O., Adali, T., & Calhoun, V. D. (2007). Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance Imaging data. Human Brain Mapping, 28(11), 12511266.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S. R., Huang, C., Yan, P. S., Paliwal, P., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2008). Neural correlates of post-error slowing during a stop signal task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(6), 10211029. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20071.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C.-S. R., Luo, X., Yan, P., Bergquist, K., & Sinha, R. (2009). Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: Neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(4), 740750. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00891.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S., Milivojevic, V., Kemp, K., Hong, K., & Sinha, R. (2006). Performance monitoring and stop signal inhibition in abstinent patients with cocaine dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 85(3), 205212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.04.008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liao, D. L., Huang, C. Y., Hu, S., Fang, S. C., Wu, C. S., Chen, W. T., et al. (2014). Cognitive control in opioid dependence and methadone maintenance treatment. PloS One, 9(4), e94589. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094589.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luks, T. L., Simpson, G. V., Dale, C. L., & Hough, M. G. (2007). Preparatory allocation of attention and adjustments in conflict processing. Neuroimage, 35(2), 949958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.041.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394408. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321593117.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Uddin, L. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: Interactions with the mirror neuron system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 571. https://doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2013.00571.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Patterson, C. M., & Newman, J. P. (1993). Reflectivity and learning from aversive events – Toward a psychological mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review, 100(4), 716736. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.100.4.716.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reed, P., Vile, R., Osborne, L. A., Romano, M., & Truzoli, R. (2015). Problematic internet usage and immune function. PloS One, 10(8), e0134538. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Segalowitz, S. J., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain and Cognition, 56(2), 129140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rushworth, M. F. S., Paus, T., & Sipila, P. K. (2001). Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(14), 52625271. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.21-14-05262.2001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schachar, R., Logan, G. D., Robaey, P., Chen, S., Ickowicz, A., & Barr, C. (2007). Restraint and cancellation: Multiple inhibition deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(2), 229238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9075-2.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schlosser, R. G. M., Nenadic, I., Wagner, G., Zysset, S., Koch, K., & Sauer, H. (2009). Dopaminergic modulation of brain systems subserving decision making under uncertainty: A study with fMRI and methylphenidate challenge. Synapse, 63(5), 429442.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schonberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(47), 1286012867. https://doi.org/10.1523/Jneurosci.2496-07.2007.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shenoy, P., & Yu, A. J. (2011). Rational decision-making in inhibitory control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 48. https://doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2011.00048.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shi, L., Lou, W. T., Wong, A., Zhang, F., Abrigo, J., Chu, W. C. W., et al. (2019). Neural evidence for long-term marriage shaping the functional brain network organization between couples. Neuroimage, 199, 8792. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.058.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Si, T.-M., Shu, L., Dang, W.-M., Se, Y.-A., Chen, J.-X., Dong, W.-T., et al. (2009). Evaluation of the reliability and validity of Chinese version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview in patients with mental disorders. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 23(7), 493503.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., Miller, K. L., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. M., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(31), 1304013045.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens, M. C., Kiehl, K. A., Pearlson, G. D., & Calhoun, V. D. (2009). Brain network dynamics during error commission. Human Brain Mapping, 30(1), 2437. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20478.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sutherland, M. T., McHugh, M. J., Pariyadath, V., & Stein, E. A. (2012). Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: Lessons learned and a road ahead. Neuroimage, 62(4), 22812295.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tonioni, F., D’Alessandris, L., Lai, C., Martinelli, D., Corvino, S., Vasale, M., et al. (2012). Internet addiction: Hours spent online, behaviors and psychological symptoms. General Hospital Psychiatry, 34(1), 8087.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Stevens, T., & Chambers, C. D. (2014). Proactive and reactive stopping when distracted: An attentional account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 12951300. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036542.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vink, M., Kahn, R. S., Raemaekers, M., van den Heuvel, M., Boersma, M., & Ramsey, N. F. (2005). Function of striatum beyond inhibition and execution of motor responses. Human Brain Mapping, 25(3), 336344. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20111.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, W., Hu, S., Ide, J. S., Zhornitsky, S., Zhang, S., Yu, A. J., et al. (2018). Motor preparation disrupts proactive control in the stop signal task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 151. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00151.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wimmer, G. E., Daw, N. D., & Shohamy, D. (2012). Generalization of value in reinforcement learning by humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 10921104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08017.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Worhunsky, P. D., Stevens, M. C., Carroll, K. M., Rounsaville, B. J., Calhoun, V. D., Pearlson, G. D., et al. (2013). Functional brain networks associated with cognitive control, cocaine dependence, and treatment outcome. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(2), 477488. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029092.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2020). ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics (ICD-11 MMS).

  • Xu, J. S., Calhoun, V. D., Pearlson, G. D., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Opposite modulation of brain functional networks implicated at low vs. high demand of attention and working memory. PloS One, 9(1), e87078. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087078.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, J. S., Calhoun, V. D., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). The absence of task-related increases in BOLD signal does not equate to absence of task-related brain activation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 240, 125127.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, J. S., Zhang, S., Calhoun, V. D., Monterosso, J., Li, C. S. R., Worhunsky, P. D., et al. (2013). Task-related concurrent but opposite modulations of overlapping functional networks as revealed by spatial IA. Neuroimage, 79, 6271.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yan, C. G., Wang, X. D., Zuo, X. N., & Zang, Y. F. (2016). DPABI: Data processing & analysis for (Resting-State) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics, 14(3), 339351.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. (2016). Internet addiction test (IAT): Stoelting.

  • Zhang, R. B., Geng, X. J., & Lee, T. M. C. (2017). Large-scale functional neural network correlates of response inhibition: An fMRI meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 222(9), 39733990.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, S., Hu, S., Bednarski, S. R., Erdman, E., & Li, C. S. R. (2014). Error-related functional connectivity of the thalamus in cocaine dependence. Neuroimage-Clinical, 4, 585592.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, S., & Li, C. S. R. (2012). Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: Independent component analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 33(1), 89104. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21197.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Aug 2024 0 94 35
Sep 2024 0 82 27
Oct 2024 0 418 28
Nov 2024 0 523 45
Dec 2024 0 657 41
Jan 2025 0 210 14
Feb 2025 0 0 0