Authors:
Nicki A. Dowling School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC 3220, Australia
Melbourne Graduate School of Education, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC 3010, Australia

Search for other papers by Nicki A. Dowling in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8592-2407
,
Christopher J. Greenwood School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC 3220, Australia
Centre for Adolescent Health, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia

Search for other papers by Christopher J. Greenwood in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Stephanie S. Merkouris School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC 3220, Australia

Search for other papers by Stephanie S. Merkouris in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
George J. Youssef School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC 3220, Australia
Centre for Adolescent Health, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia

Search for other papers by George J. Youssef in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Matthew Browne Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, CQUniversity, Bundaberg, QLD, Australia

Search for other papers by Matthew Browne in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Matthew Rockloff Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, CQUniversity, Bundaberg, QLD, Australia

Search for other papers by Matthew Rockloff in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Paul Myers The Social Research Centre, Australian National University, Melbourne, VIC 3000, Australia

Search for other papers by Paul Myers in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Problem gambling severity and gambling-related harm are closely coupled, but conceptually distinct, constructs. The primary aim was to compare low-risk gambling limits when gambling-related harm was defined using the negative consequence items of the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI-Harm) and the Short Gambling Harms Scale items (SGHS-Harm). A secondary aim was compare low-risk limits derived using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains (e.g. financial and relationship) were endorsed with a definition of harm in which at least two harms from any domain were endorsed.

Methods

Data were collected from dual-frame computer-assisted telephone interviews of 5,000 respondents in the fourth Social and Economic Impact Study (SEIS) of Gambling in Tasmania. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analyse were conducted to identify low-risk gambling limits.

Results

PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm definitions produced similar overall limits: 30–37 times per year; AUD$510–$544 per year; expenditure comprising no more than 10.2–10.3% of gross personal income; 400–454 minutes per year; and 2 types of gambling activities per year. Acceptable limits (AUC ≥0.70) were identified for horse/dog racing, keno, and sports/other betting using the PGSI definition; and electronic gaming machines, keno, and bingo using the SGHS definition. The requirement that gamblers endorse two or more harms across different domains had a relatively negligible effect.

Discussion and conclusions

Although replications using alternative measures of harm are required, previous PGSI-based limits appear to be robust thresholds that have considerable potential utility in the prevention of gambling-related harm.

Abstract

Background and aims

Problem gambling severity and gambling-related harm are closely coupled, but conceptually distinct, constructs. The primary aim was to compare low-risk gambling limits when gambling-related harm was defined using the negative consequence items of the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI-Harm) and the Short Gambling Harms Scale items (SGHS-Harm). A secondary aim was compare low-risk limits derived using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains (e.g. financial and relationship) were endorsed with a definition of harm in which at least two harms from any domain were endorsed.

Methods

Data were collected from dual-frame computer-assisted telephone interviews of 5,000 respondents in the fourth Social and Economic Impact Study (SEIS) of Gambling in Tasmania. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analyse were conducted to identify low-risk gambling limits.

Results

PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm definitions produced similar overall limits: 30–37 times per year; AUD$510–$544 per year; expenditure comprising no more than 10.2–10.3% of gross personal income; 400–454 minutes per year; and 2 types of gambling activities per year. Acceptable limits (AUC ≥0.70) were identified for horse/dog racing, keno, and sports/other betting using the PGSI definition; and electronic gaming machines, keno, and bingo using the SGHS definition. The requirement that gamblers endorse two or more harms across different domains had a relatively negligible effect.

Discussion and conclusions

Although replications using alternative measures of harm are required, previous PGSI-based limits appear to be robust thresholds that have considerable potential utility in the prevention of gambling-related harm.

Introduction

The public health perspective, which frames gambling within a whole of population approach to inform policy for prevention and intervention practices, involves identifying determinants and subsequent harms of problematic gambling behaviour (Korn & Shaffer, 1999). Langham et al. (2016) functionally defined gambling-related harm as ‘any initial or exacerbated adverse consequence due to an engagement with gambling that leads to a decrement to the health or wellbeing of an individual, family unit, community or population’. They developed a conceptual framework that organised harms across broad domains: financial harm; relationship disruption, conflict or breakdown; emotional or psychological distress; decrements to health; cultural harm; reduced performance at work or study; and non-normative/criminal activity. Problem gambling severity and harm are closely coupled but conceptually distinct constructs, and harm that occurs below the clinical threshold of pathology is still relevant to policy (Browne & Rockloff, 2017; Delfabbro & King, 2019). Thus, a harm-reduction approach tends to be more focused on preventing harmful gambling rather than preventing the psychiatric condition of disordered gambling (Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Miller, Hodgins, & Wang, 2009).

In the alcohol field, low-risk drinking limits have been developed to distinguish between low and high-risk drinking behaviour (Room & Rehm, 2012). Although these limits have been subject to some criticism (Casswell, 2012; Room & Rehm, 2012), they serve as the basis for low-risk drinking guidelines that are promoted to the general public to help individuals make informed choices about their drinking habits and their associated risks (National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC], 2020; Room & Rehm, 2012). In contrast, there has been very little empirical research attempting to define levels of low-risk gambling, despite findings that many gamblers attempt to reduce their gambling by setting frequency, expenditure, and time limits (Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008). Gambling stakeholders and the general public believe that empirically-derived low-risk limits are important in preventing gambling-related harm (Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, & Wynne, 2008; Dowling et al., 2018).

To date, all low-risk gambling limits in population-representative samples have been identified using a risk ratio approach involving receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analyses (Brosowski et al., 2015; Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Dowling et al., 2018; Dowling et al., 2021). In Australia, Dowling et al. (2021) recently derived a set of overall low-risk gambling limits across population-representative telephone surveys conducted in Tasmania (the second and third Social and Economic Impact Study (SEIS) of Gambling in Tasmania) and the Australian Capital Territory (ACT: 2014 Survey on Gambling, Health, and Wellbeing in the ACT): gambling frequency of 20–30 times per year; gambling expenditure of AUD$380–$615 per year (USD$240–$388 per year); gambling expenditure comprising 0.83–1.68% of gross personal income; and participating in no more than 2 types of gambling activities per year. In this study, similar limits were identified from these two disparate Australian states and territories. The ACT limits were, however, consistently slightly more conservative than the Tasmanian limits, which is not unexpected given that the ACT population is characterised by slightly lower rates of gambling participation and a higher socio-economic profile (e.g. higher levels of working age people, employment, education and income (Dowling et al., 2021)). The findings indicated that a considerable proportion of gamblers exceeding a particular limit also exceeded other limits and the two gambling expenditure limits (gambling expenditure and gambling expenditure as a proportion of income) were consistently the best-performing.

Several studies have also derived low-risk limits for gamblers participating in specific gambling activities (Brosowski et al., 2015; Quilty, Murati, & Bagby, 2014). In a combined psychiatric outpatient and community sample, Quilty et al. (2014) identified low-risk gambling limits for instant scratch tickets (gambling for more than once per month), bingo (gambling for no longer than 135 minutes per session and spending no more than CAD$450 per year), casino table games (gambling for no more than never, gambling for no longer than 190 minutes per session, and spending no more than CAD$330–CAD$1,200 per year), and sports betting (spending no more than CAD$510–CAD$780 per year). Brosowski et al. (2015) subsequently identified some activity-specific limits in a representative dual-frame German dataset: electronic gaming machines (EGMs) (gambling no more than 3 days in the last year and no more than 10-days ever) and poker (nor more than 1–10 days ever).

These previous efforts to develop low-risk gambling limits have defined gambling-related harm using item subsets of problem gambling severity measures, predominantly the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI: Ferris & Wynne, 2001) (Brosowski et al., 2015; Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Dowling et al., 2018; Dowling et al., 2021; Quilty et al., 2014). It has been argued the PGSI is a viable instrument to measure gambling-related harm because it has some focus on negative consequences rather than only behavioural symptoms (Currie et al., 2009). This measure, however, comprises only a limited number of negative consequence items and there is some disagreement regarding which items are negative consequences (Browne & Rockloff, 2019a; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Ferris & Wynne, 2001). Moreover, some definitions of harm in the literature are based on only the negative consequence items (Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Dowling et al., 2018; Dowling et al., 2021) while others are based on all PGSI items (Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Dowling et al., 2018). Arguably, the PGSI includes items measuring financial, relationship, emotional/psychological, and health harms (Browne & Rockloff, 2019a), but does not assess harms across other domains. The exclusive reliance on measures of problem gambling severity to measure harm is a limitation in this literature compared with the alcohol literature, which has employed a range of harms to the drinker, including chronic disease from the volume of drinking over time, injury from specific drinking occasions, total mortality, hazardous behaviours, and delinquent behaviours (National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC], 2009; Room & Rehm, 2012).

Moreover, previous studies have been characterised by a lack of consensus on an aggregate limit of gambling-related harm using problem gambling severity measures. Although low-risk limits have generally been robust to variations in definitions of harm (Brosowski et al., 2015; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Dowling et al., 2018), superior ROC psychometric properties have generally been produced when gambling-related harm is defined as two or more negative consequences (Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Dowling et al., 2018). It has, however, been argued that people endorsing gambling-related problems in two different domains can reasonably be viewed as beginning to experience harm related to their gambling (Currie et al., 2006). To date, however, all available population-level research have employed 2+ harm definitions in which at least two harms from any domain are endorsed (Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Dowling et al., 2018; Dowling et al., 2021). There is therefore a need for research to explore the effect of using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains are endorsed.

Study aims

Given that problem gambling severity and gambling-related harm are not synonymous (Browne & Rockloff, 2017; Delfabbro & King, 2019), there is a need to identify low-risk gambling limits using alternative measures of gambling-related harm (Dowling et al., 2018, 2021). To date, this important advance has been hindered by the absence of validated measures of harms attributable to gambling with interpretable scoring procedures. Browne, Goodwin, and Rockloff (2018), however, have recently developed and validated a new brief instrument, the Short Gambling Harms Scale (SGHS), for use as a population-level measure of past-year gambling-related harm. The primary aim of this study was to compare the overall and activity-specific low-risk gambling limits identified for the Tasmanian population when gambling-related harm was defined using the negative consequence items of the PGSI (PGSI-Harm) and the SGHS items. A secondary aim was to compare low-risk limits derived using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains (e.g. financial and relationship) were endorsed with a definition of harm in which at least two harms from any domain were endorsed.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Secondary analysis of population data from the fourth Social and Economic Impact Study (SEIS) of Gambling in Tasmania (ACIL Allen, Deakin University, Central Queensland University, & Social Research Centre, 2017) was conducted. This study was selected for analysis as it was the first available population-representative study in Australia to collect gambling-related harms using the SGHS, as well as collect continuous expenditure data across multiple gambling activities. Data was collected via Computer Assisted Telephone Interviews (CATIs) with 5,000 residents of Tasmania aged 18 years and over from 13 June to 7 August 2017 (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics). Due to a focus on state-level estimates, the collected sample was stratified according to broad geographic regions in proportion to the population. A dual-frame sample design was employed, with a 50% landline phone and 50% mobile phone split. The sample was drawn from three sources: random digit dial (RDD) landline, pre-screened RDD mobile sample and listed mobile phone numbers. Respondents for the landline sample were selected using the youngest male aged 18 years and over method, while respondents for the mobile phone samples were taken to be any person who answered the phone, was a resident of Tasmania and was 18 years of age or older.

Table 1.

Sample descriptive statistics

CharacteristicProportion (95% CI)
Gender (male)48.8% (47.1, 50.6)
Age (years)
 18–2411.1% (9.5, 12.8)
 25–3415.1% (13.6, 16.8)
 35–4415.1% (13.8, 16.4)
 45–5417.2% (16.0, 18.4)
 55–6417.7% (16.7, 18.8)
 65+23.8% (22.7, 25.0)
Household structure
 Couple with no children10.5% (9.5, 11.7)
 Couple with children still at home34.5% (32.8, 36.3)
 Couple with children not living at home21.5% (20.3, 22.7)
 Single person household (no children)10.8% (9.9, 11.8)
 Single with children still at home7.4% (6.5, 8.6)
 Single with children not living at home6.5% (5.9, 7.1)
 Group or shared household4.5% (3.7, 5.5)
 In some other arrangement4.2% (3.5, 5.0)
Occupational status
 Paid full-time employed32.6% (31.0, 34.3)
 Paid part-time employed21.9% (20.4, 23.4)
 Looking for work3.7% (3.0, 4.7)
 Not in the labour force41.8% (40.1, 43.5)
Educational attainment
 Less than secondary school22.3% (20.9, 23.7)
 Secondary school20.9% (19.3, 22.5)
 Vocational or trade qualification40.8% (39.0, 42.5)
 University graduate16.0% (15.1, 17.1)
Country of birth (Australia)
 Australia85.8% (84.6, 86.9)
Problem Gambling Severity Index category
 Non-gambling41.5% (39.8, 43.3)
 Non-problem gambling51.8% (50.0, 53.5)
 Low-risk gambling4.8% (4.0, 5.6)
 Moderate-risk gambling1.4% (1.0, 1.9)
 Problem gambling0.6% (0.4, 1.0)
 Problem Gambling Severity Index score – M (SD)0.2 (1.5)
 Short Gambling Harms Scale score – M (SD)0.1 (1.0)

A total of 90,315 calls were placed to 26,476 sample records to achieve 5,000 interviews. Using the American Association for Public Opinion Research [AAPOR] Response Rate 3, which proportionally allocates records with an unknown outcome as either in-scope or out-of-scope based on the distribution of records with a known call outcome (American Association of Public Opinion Research, 2011), the final combined response rate for the survey was 41.5%. A pilot test of 50 interviews was undertaken to ensure internal logic. A 2-stage approach to weighting was undertaken: (1) calculation of a design weight to adjust for the probability of being sampled by landline and mobile phone and (2) adjusting this weight by iterative proportion fitting so the final weight conformed to population benchmarks for telephone status, age, sex, region, education and country of birth. The average interview duration was 15 minutes.

Measures

Gambling consumption indices and measures of harm

The overall and activity-specific low-risk gambling limits were derived on the basis of multiple indices of gambling consumption (Table 2) and using items from the PGSI and SGHS. The 9-item PGSI (Ferris & Wynne, 2001) has been adopted as the preferred instrument for measuring problem gambling severity in population-level research in Australia and generally displays good psychometric properties (Currie, Hodgins, & Casey, 2013; Ferris & Wynne, 2001; Neal, Delfabbro, & O’Neil, 2005). The PGSI negative consequence items are identified in Table 3. The 10-item SGHS (Browne et al., 2018) was developed for use as a population-level measure of past year gambling-related harm to monitor the impact of gambling on the community. The SGHS was based on an initial item pool of 72 specific harms caused by problematic gambling and validated using an internet panel sample of 1,524 past year gamblers. Ten items were selected using an item selection algorithm with the goals of maximising sensitivity and construct coverage. Each has a binary response option and total scores are calculated by summing the number of endorsed items. The SGHS is highly correlated with the 72-item checklist (0.94) and achieves a very small percentage of false negatives in terms of identifying persons otherwise harmed by gambling (4.8%). It displays very strong reliability (α = 0.93; coefficient omega = 0.83), homogeneity and unidimensionality; and significant associations with the PGSI, gambling consumption, lower quality of life, and addictive gambling symptoms.

Table 2.

Gambling consumption indicesa

Gambling frequencyb, cTypical wording was: ‘In the last 12 months, how many times per week, per month or per year have you played/bet on [gambling activity]?’ for different modalities (e.g. venue, telephone, racetrack, off-course venue, internet on mobile device, internet using desktop computer) of each gambling activity. Annual gambling frequency was calculated by standardising each response to an estimated yearly frequency then summing these yearly frequencies across gambling activities.
Gambling expenditureb, c/session expenditurecTypical wording was: ‘In the past 12 months, approximately how much money, on average, did you spend during each session of [gambling activity]?’ for each specific gambling activity. Total annual gambling expenditure was calculated by multiplying gambling frequency with session expenditure estimates for each activity then summing these yearly gambling expenditures across all gambling activities. Gambling expenditure was assessed only in terms of amount of money lost.
Gambling expenditure as a proportion of gross annual personal incomeb, cGross annual personal income was assessed: ‘Could you please tell me your approximate annual personal income before tax?’ In contrast to previous Tasmanian SEIS surveys, categories were refined to $10,000 increments for the purposes of these analyses (from less than $10,000 to $150,000 or more). To derive expenditure as a proportion of income, we used the mid-point of each category's range to represent the respective income category (e.g. $10,000 to $19,999 became $15,000). For the final income category (e.g. $150,000 or more) in which no mid-point exists, the same $5,000 interval that was applied (i.e. $155,000). Total annual gambling expenditure was then divided by the mid-point income value to derive gambling expenditure as a proportion of income. A small number (n = 4) of estimates exceeding 100% were removed from the dataset.
Number of types of gambling activitiesbThe number of types of gambling activities was based on participation across each gambling activity, with the exception of informal private games due to low participation. Typical wording was: ‘I am going to start by reading a list of popular gambling activities and find out if you have played them for money in the previous 12 months. In the last 12 months, have you…?’
Gambling durationb, c/session durationcTypical wording was: “In the past 12 months, how much time on average did you spend playing/betting on [gambling activity] during each visit/session/transaction of [gambling activity]”. Responses were recorded in minutes. Total annual gambling duration was calculated by multiplying gambling frequency with session duration estimates for each gambling activity then summing these yearly gambling durations across all gambling activities.

Unless otherwise indicated all gambling indices were based on estimates from nine gambling activities: EGMs, horse or greyhound racing, instant scratch tickets, lotteries, keno, casino table games, bingo, sports or other event betting, and informal private games.

Gambling consumption index for overall low-risk gambling limits.

Gambling consumption index for activity-specific limits.

Table 3.

Operationalisation of harm domains using PGSI negative consequence items and SGHS items

CategoryMeasureItem
FinancialPGSIBet more than you could really afford to lose
PGSIBorrowed money or sold anything to get money to gamble
PGSIGambling has caused any financial problems for you or your household
SGHSReduction of your available spending money
SGHSReduction of your savings
SGHSLess spending on recreational expenses such as eating out, going to movies or other entertainment.
SGHSSold personal items
SGHSIncreased credit card debt
RelationshipPGSIFelt people criticised your betting or told you that you had a gambling problem, regardless of whether or not you thought it was true
SGHSSpent less time with people you care about
Emotional/PsychologicalPGSIFelt that you might have a problem with gambling
PGSIFelt guilty about the way you gamble, or what happens when you gamble
SGHSHad regrets that made you feel sorry about your gambling
SGHSFelt ashamed of your gambling
SGHSFelt distressed about your gambling
SGHSFelt like a failure
HealthPGSIGambling has caused you any health problems, including a feeling of stress or anxiety

Definitions of gambling-related harm

For the primary aim, endorsement of two or more of the seven negative consequence PGSI items (PGSI-Harm) was compared to endorsement of two or more SGHS items (SGHS-Harm). For the secondary aim, harm domains were operationalised using the combined set of PGSI negative consequence items and SGHS items (Table 3); and endorsement of two or more items across any domain of harm (Any-domain-Harm) was compared to endorsement of two or more harms across different domains (Cross-domain-Harm).

Data analytic strategy

All analyses employed weighted data and were conducted in Stata-14 (StataCorp, 2015). ROC analyses were conducted to identify optimal low-risk gambling limits across the multiple gambling consumption indices and definitions of harm. In these ROC analyses, the sensitivity and 1-specificity for each level of gambling consumption index was plotted and the area under the curve (AUC) of the resulting graph was calculated. AUC values were interpreted according to established guidelines: low (0.50–0.70), moderate (0.70–0.90), and high (>0.90) (Swets, Dawes, & Monahan, 2000). In this study, optimal cut-offs displaying moderate to high classification accuracy (AUC value ≥0.70) were considered acceptable (Brosowski et al., 2015; Swets et al., 2000). The level of gambling behaviour that had the maximum Youden Index value (Youden, 1950), relative to all other levels of gambling behaviour, was deemed the optimal cut-off (with equal weighting given to sensitivity and specificity) (see Ruopp, Perkins, Whitcomb, & Schisterman, 2008). Missing data were removed via pairwise deletion. Missing data was low for key gambling consumption and harm variables (range <1–3%), except expenditure as a proportion of income (11%). This was due to the high rate of missing data for income, which is very common in large population-based research (Kim, Egerter, Cubbin, Takahashi, & Braveman, 2007) due to respondents considering income information too private or sensitive and not knowing their income (Davern, Rodin Beebe, & Call, 2005). This statistical approach replicated that employed in Dowling et al. (2021) in order to facilitate direct comparisons to the overall low-risk, limits derived from the PGSI in the second and third SEIS studies of Gambling in Tasmania and the 2014 Survey on Gambling, Health and Wellbeing in the ACT.

In the first set of ROC analyses, overall low-risk gambling limits were identified in the sample of past-year gamblers for PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm. In the second set of ROC analyses, low-risk gambling limits for each gambling activity (see Table 2) were identified using PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm in samples of respondents who reported past-year participation in each specific gambling activity (e.g. EGM limits were derived using only EGM gamblers) and with consumption measures specific to each activity (e.g. EGM expenditure not overall gambling expenditure). In the third set of ROC analyses, overall low-risk gambling limits were identified in the sample of past-year gamblers using Any-domain-Harm and Cross-domain-Harm. Further characterisation of the low-risk limits is presented in terms of absolute risk (i.e., proportion of people who were above the limit who experienced harm) and risk ratio (i.e., ratio of the incidence rate in individuals who were above the limit, relative to the incidence rate in individuals who were below the limit). The data analysis scripts for this study can be found on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/qu85y/).

Ethics

This project was approved by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee (2017–145). Respondents were informed about the study and all provided informed consent.

Results

PGSI and SGHS definitions of harm: Overall low-risk gambling limits

The overall low-risk gambling limits were all acceptable using both PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm, with all displaying similar moderate classification accuracy (Table 4). PGSI-Harm was exceeded by 3.9% of gamblers and 2.3% of the population. Using this definition, the overall limits were exceeding any of the following: gambling frequency of 37 times per year; gambling expenditure of AUD$544 per year; gambling expenditure comprising 10.32% of gross personal income; gambling duration of 454 minutes (7 h, 34 min) per year; and 2 types of gambling activity per year. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and reported harm ranged from 5.8 to 24.0%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 4.3-fold–9.8-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Table 4.

Identification of overall low-risk gambling limits using each definition of gambling-related harma

Low-risk gambling limitPGSI-HarmbSGHS-HarmcAny-domain-HarmdCross-domain-Harme
Proportion of population (including non-gamblers) exceeding each definition of harm2.26% (95% CI 1.74, 2.94)2.63% (95% CI 2.11, 3.28)4.24% (95% CI 3.53, 5.08)3.29% (95% CI 2.67, 4.06)
Proportion of gamblers exceeding each definition of harm3.87% (95% CI 2.98, 5.01)4.51% (95% CI 3.62, 5.61)7.25% (95% CI 6.06, 8.66)5.63% (95% CI 4.58, 6.92)
Gambling frequency per yearCut-off37302933
AUC (95% CIs)0.75 (0.66, 0.84)0.75 (0.68, 0.81)0.72 (0.66, 0.78)0.72 (0.66, 0.79)
Sensitivity, specificity0.64, 0.730.69, 0.680.65, 0.670.62, 0.70
N2,7922,7812,7932,793
Absolute risk7.47.811.99.9
Relative risk4.33.22.83.3
Gambling expenditure per yearCut-off544510440501
AUC (95% CIs)0.79 (0.72, 0.71)0.79 (0.73, 0.85)0.77 (0.72, 0.81)0.77 (0.71, 0.83)
Sensitivity, specificity0.72, 0.710.73, 0.700.71, 0.680.69, 0.70
N2,7082,6972,7092,709
Absolute risk8.69.013.210.9
Relative risk5.34.83.74.0
Gambling expenditure a proportion of gross personal incomeCut-off10.310.29.49.8
AUC (95% CIs)0.73 (0.67, 0.79)0.71 (0.65, 0.76)0.68 (0.64, 0.72)0.70 (0.66, 0.75)
Sensitivity, specificity0.55, 0.920.53, 0.930.48, 0.910.52, 0.92
N2,3102,3032,3112,311
Absolute risk24.022.430.4
Relative risk9.86.88.0
Gambling duration per year (minutes)Cut-off454400353410
AUC (95% CIs)0.77 (0.68, 0.86)0.76 (0.69, 0.83)0.75 (0.70, 0.81)0.77 (0.70, 0.84)
Sensitivity, specificity0.65, 0.760.65, 0.730.65, 0.730.65, 0.75
N2,7872,7772,7882,788
Absolute risk11.111.218.914.3
Relative risk7.95.75.35.9
Number of types of gambling activitiesCut-off2222
AUC (95% CIs)0.75 (0.69, 0.81)0.75 (0.69, 0.81)0.75 (0.71, 0.80)0.74 (0.69, 0.79)
Sensitivity, specificity0.78, 0.580.77, 0.580.76, 0.600.75, 0.59
N2,8332,8222,8342,834
Absolute risk5.86.610.68.2
Relative risk6.14.24.54.5

Bold typeface indicates AUC ≥ 0.70.

Endorsement of ≥ 2 PGSI negative consequence items.

Endorsement of ≥ 2 SGHS items.

Endorsement of ≥ 2 PGSI negative consequence items or SGHS items across any domain of harm.

Endorsement of ≥ 2 PGSI negative consequence items or SGHS items across different domains of harm.

Similarly, SGHS-Harm was exceeded by 4.5% of gamblers and 2.6% of the population. Using this definition, the overall limits were exceeding any of the following: gambling frequency of 30 times per year; gambling expenditure of AUD$510 per year; gambling expenditure comprising 10.2% of gross personal income; gambling duration of 400 minutes (6 h, 40 min) per year; and 2 types of gambling activities per year. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and who reported harm ranged from 6.6 to 22.4%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 3.2-fold to 6.8-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Across all true positive cases, 38–56% were true positives for both PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm: gambling frequency per year = 38%; gambling expenditure per year = 41%; gambling expenditure a proportion of gross personal income = 56%; gambling duration per year = 47%; number of types of gambling activities = 39%. There was, however, considerable overlap in those exceeding the overall low-risk gambling limits using PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm. Over 90% of respondents exceeding the SGHS-Harm limits also exceeded the PGSI-Harm limits: gambling frequency per year = 94%; gambling expenditure per year = 92%; gambling expenditure a proportion of gross personal income = 100%; gambling duration per year = 94%; number of types of gambling activities = 100%.

PGSI and SGHS definitions of harm: Activity-specific limits

Few of the optimal low-risk gambling limit cut-offs identified for specific gambling activities displayed acceptable levels of discrimination, using either PGSI-Harm (Table 5) or SGHS-Harm (Table 6), with most displaying low classification accuracy. The limits relating to gambling frequency per year, gambling expenditure per year, and session expenditure yielded acceptable levels of classification performance for PGSI-Harm, while the limits relating to gambling expenditure and gambling duration per year were the most likely to be acceptable for SGHS-Harm.

Table 5.

ROC analyses for each gambling activity using PGSI-Harma

EGMsHorse/dog racingInstant scratch ticketsLotteryKenoCasino table gamesBingoSports/other event betting
Proportion of gamblers on the specific gambling activity exceeding the definition of harm10.13% (95% CI 7.58, 13.41)8.19% (95% CI 5.16, 12.77)5.88% (95% CI 3.96, 8.63)3.30% (95% CI 2.41, 4.51)5.91% (95% CI 4.30, 8.05)11.72% (95% CI 6.71, 19.68)3.22% (95% CI 0.56, 16.55)15.45% (95% CI 8.51, 26.40)
Gambling frequency per yearCut-off11166114112036
AUC (95% CIs)0.66 (0.57, 0.75)0.76 (0.65, 0.87)0.60 (0.49, 0.71)0.48 (0.38, 0.58)0.73 (0.65, 0.81)0.65 (0.51, 0.79)0.26 (0.16, 0.36)0.51 (0.30, 0.73)
Sensitivity, specificity0.49, 0.760.70, 0.680.54, 0.600.06, 0.950.63, 0.700.73, 0.491.00, 0.110.25, 0.86
N78345790120801,10116485125
Absolute risk18.612.0
Relative risk6.84.1
Gambling expenditure per yearCut-off42045052728150150.02,600700
AUC (95% CIs)0.65 (0.54, 0.76)0.78 (0.64, 0.91)0.59 (0.48, 0.71)0.51 (0.42, 0.60)0.73 (0.65, 0.81)0.61 (0.4, 0.82)0.12 (−0.10, 0.33)0.56 (0.36, 0.76)
Sensitivity, specificity0.45, 0.800.67, 0.740.47, 0.670.22, 0.810.61, 0.730.52, 0.640.00, 0.960.30, 0.86
N76144189620381,09115583123
Absolute risk21.611.8
Relative risk11.53.3
Gambling expenditure a proportion of gross personal incomeCut-off7.37.00.07.04.10.014.05.7
AUC (95% CIs)0.65 (0.59, 0.71)0.73 (0.60, 0.85)0.56 (0.45, 0.66)0.58 (0.50, 0.66)0.59 (0.54, 0.63)0.54 (0.42, 0.68)0.32 (0.27, 0.37)0.69 (0.53, 0.85)
Sensitivity, specificity0.44, 0.890.57, 0.770.30, 0.860.40, 0.940.37, 0.920.80, 0.380.00, 1.000.58, 0.98
N652384759172395514164113
Absolute risk30.8
Relative risk6.9
Gambling duration per yearCut-off450380201201442403090
AUC (95% CIs)0.64 (0.54, 0.75)0.82 (0.74, 0.91)0.55 (0.44, 0.67)0.52 (0.41, 0.63)0.68 (0.60, 0.76)0.67 (0.51, 0.82)0.79 (0.18, 0.48)0.63 (0.45, 0.80)
Sensitivity, specificity0.46, 0.770.79, 0.710.43, 0.650.28, 0.770.63, 0.630.55, 0.700.94, 0.210.64, 0.54
N77244287419811,04015379114
Absolute risk19.2
Relative risk8.9
Session expenditureCut-off5525514181508025
AUC (95% CIs)0.61 (0.50, 0.72)0.72 (0.62, 0.82)0.53 (0.43, 0.63)0.63 (0.56, 0.70)0.66 (0.56, 0.75)0.53 (0.32, 0.74)0.04 (0.00 0.25)0.70 (0.51, 0.88)
Sensitivity, specificity0.38, 0.820.75, 0.580.72, 0.330.73, 0.470.48, 0.760.28, 0.800.00, 0.960.57, 0.71
N76944490120451,09114979116
Absolute risk16.224.1
Relative risk9.22.2
Session durationCut-off602554251504520
AUC (95% CIs)0.57 (0.47, 0.67)0.72 (0.61, 0.83)0.45 (0.35, 0.56)0.57 (0.49, 0.66)0.57 (0.48, 0.66)0.60 (0.41, 0.79)0.47 (0.33, 0.61)0.64 (0.42, 0.85)
Sensitivity, specificity0.34, 0.780.70, 0.620.00, 1.000.48, 0.630.43, 0.670.38, 0.840.99, 0.350.49, 0.71
N78144988519981,05015479114
Absolute risk15.4
Relative risk4.7

Bold typeface indicates AUC ≥ 0.70.

Table 6.

ROC analyses for each gambling activity using SGHS-Harma

EGMsHorse/dog racingInstant scratch ticketsLotteryKenoCasino table gamesBingoSports/other event betting
Proportion of gamblers on the specific gambling activity exceeding the definition of harm9.84% (95% CI 7.52, 12.79)8.85% (95% CI 6.13, 12.61)6.54% (95% CI 4.64, 9.14)5.06% (95% CI 3.94, 6.48)6.91% (95% CI 5.24, 9.05)13.59% (95% CI 8.65, 20.71)8.33% (95% CI 2.89, 21.70)16.97% (95% CI 10.35, 26.58)
Gambling frequency per yearCut-off1124613112636
AUC (95% CIs)0.69 (0.61, 0.77)0.60 (0.49, 0.72)0.57 (0.47, 0.67)0.53 (0.46, 0.61)0.71 (0.65, 0.78)0.57 (0.45, 0.69)0.73 (0.49, 0.96)0.53 (0.34, 0.72)
Sensitivity, specificity0.53, 0.770.42, 0.750.50, 0.600.57, 0.480.61, 0.700.61, 0.480.75, 0.590.26, 0.85
N78145689920681,09816484126
Absolute risk13.016.7
Relative risk3.34.0
Gambling expenditure per yearCut-off2408006520813040120600
AUC (95% CIs)0.74 (0.66, 0.81)0.60 (0.48, 0.73)0.57 (0.46, 0.67)0.56 (0.49, 0.64)0.73 (0.67, 0.80)0.58 (0.46, 0.71)0.85 (0.74, 0.95)0.56 (0.38, 0.75)
Sensitivity, specificity0.64, 0.700.38, 0.800.38, 0.720.56, 0.530.64, 0.700.76, 0.390.95, 0.680.31, 0.84
N75944089420261,08815582124
Absolute risk18.412.223.2
Relative risk3.42.7b
Gambling expenditure a proportion of gross personal incomeCut-off7.39.60.06.74.00.00.05.7
AUC (95% CIs)0.65 (0.59, 0.71)0.64 (0.53, 0.75)0.48 (0.39, 0.57)0.57 (0.51, 0.63)0.60 (0.55, 0.64)0.44 (0.38, 0.5)0.40 (0.31, 0.49)0.65 (0.52, 0.79)
Sensitivity, specificity0.44, 0.890.41, 0.890.70, 0.330.38, 0.940.38, 0.910.79, 0.370.93, 0.320.52, 0.98
N652383757171595314163114
Absolute risk
Relative risk
Gambling duration per yearCut-off3303602535135803601,140
AUC (95% CIs)0.70 (0.61, 0.78)0.63 (0.51, 0.74)0.55 (0.45, 0.65)0.56 (0.49, 0.63)0.70 (0.64, 0.77)0.56 (0.43, 0.68)0.79 (0.61, 0.96)0.48 (0.29, 0.67)
Sensitivity, specificity0.57, 0.720.51, 0.670.37, 0.710.61, 0.480.68, 0.620.72, 0.370.85, 0.610.15, 0.89
N77044187219711,03715379115
Absolute risk16.911.019.3
Relative risk2.82.510.0
Session expenditureCut-off303451316252020
AUC (95% CIs)0.71 (0.64, 0.77)0.61 (0.50, 0.73)0.53 (0.44, 0.63)0.60 (0.54, 0.66)0.68 (0.60, 0.76)0.56 (0.43, 0.70)0.79 (0.65, 0.93)0.64 (0.48, 0.80)
Sensitivity, specificity0.68, 0.620.48, 0.690.73, 0.330.74, 0.430.55, 0.710.77, 0.340.94, 0.570.58, 0.62
N76744389920331,08814979117
Absolute risk17.213.4
Relative risk4.1b
Session durationCut-off4530542015090180
AUC (95% CIs)0.61 (0.52, 0.69)0.54 (0.42, 0.67)0.53 (0.42, 0.64)0.55 (0.48, 0.62)0.62 (0.54, 0.69)0.52 (0.35, 0.68)0.73 (0.62, 0.83)0.40 (0.22, 0.58)
Sensitivity, specificity0.47, 0.690.41, 0.660.28, 0.790.44, 0.630.56, 0.610.23, 0.810.86, 0.620.02, 0.98
N77944988319881,04715479115
Absolute risk13.7
Relative risk5.3

Bold typeface indicates AUC ≥0.70.

No relative risk estimate available as none of the gamblers not exceeding the limit endorsed gambling-related harm on the SGHS.

Using PGSI-Harm, acceptable activity-specific limits were identified for horse/dog race gambling (16 times per year, AUD$450 per year, 7.0% of gross personal income, 380 minutes per year, AUD$25 per session, 25 minutes per session), keno (11 times per year, AUD$150 per year), and sports/other event betting (AUD$25 per session). This definition of harm was exceeded by 8.2% of horse or dog race gamblers, 5.9% of keno players, and 15.5% of sports or other event bettors. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and who reported harm ranged from 11.8 to 30.8%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 3.3-fold to 11.5-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Using SGHS-Harm, acceptable activity-specific limits were identified for EGM gambling (AUD$240 per year, 330 minutes per year, AUD$30 per session), keno (11 times per year, $130 per year, 135 minutes per year), and bingo (6 times per year, $120 per year, 360 minutes per year, $20 per session, 90 minutes per session). This definition of harm was exceeded by 9.8% of EGM gamblers, 6.91% of keno players, and 8.3% of bingo players. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and who reported harm ranged from 11.0 to 23.2%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 2.5-fold to 10.0-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Any-domain and cross-domain definitions of harm

Given the similarities between the results obtained using PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm (optimal low-risk limits, proportions exceeding limits, absolute and risk ratio estimates), the PGSI negative consequence items and SGHS items were pooled to explore the effect of using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains are endorsed on the low-risk limits. The low-risk gambling limits were all acceptable using both Any-domain-Harm and Cross-domain-Harm (with all displaying moderate classification accuracy), with one exception (Any-domain-Harm gambling expenditure as a proportion of income limit) (Table 4).

Any-domain-Harm was exceeded by 7.3% of gamblers and 4.2% of the population. Using this definition, the overall limits were exceeding any of the following: gambling frequency of 29 times per year; gambler expenditure of AUD$440 per year; gambling duration of 353 minutes (5 h, 53 min) per year; and 2 types of gambling activity per year. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and who reported harm ranged from 10.6 to 18.9%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 2.8-fold to 5.3-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Cross-domain Harm was exceeded by 5.6% of gamblers and 3.3% of the population. Using this definition, the overall limits were exceeding any of the following: gambling frequency of 33 times per year; gambling expenditure of AUD$501 per year; gambling expenditure comprising 9.8% of gross personal income; gambling duration of 410 minutes (6 h, 50 min) per year; and 2 types of gambling activity per year. The proportion of gamblers who exceeded these limits and who reported harm ranged from 9.9 to 30.4%; while exceeding these limits was associated with a 3.3-fold to 8.0-fold increase in the risk of experiencing harm, relative to gamblers who did not exceed the limits.

Discussion

Comparison of PGSI and SGHS

In this study, both PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm overall low-risk gambling limits produced acceptable ROC parameters, with all displaying similar moderate classification accuracy. PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm derived remarkably similar overall low-risk gambling limits, although the limits derived using PGSI-Harm were consistently slightly higher. They also captured very similar proportions of gamblers in the general population, suggesting that both sets of items are measuring harms of comparable severity; they also provided similar assessments of absolute and relative (ratio) risk. These similarities support the robustness of the limits derived in previous research using the PGSI as the measure of harm. Moreover, they are consistent with prior findings that diverse specific ‘symptoms’ of harm function as indicators of a single, unidimensional construct (Browne & Rockloff, 2019a) and do not appear to support suggestions the SGHS is assessing trivial harms relative to the PGSI or opportunity costs, rather than bona fide personal impacts (Delfabbro & King, 2019). However, while there was considerable overlap in those exceeding the low-risk limits using each definition of harm, PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm appear to classify respondents differently, with only one-third to one-half of respondents classified as true positive cases for both definitions of harm. This is of concern given that both the PGSI negative consequence items and SGHS items are intended to capture the same underlying construct of gambling-related harm. This finding highlights the need for further research exploring differences in true prevalence estimates when employing different operationalisations of gambling-related harm given the relatively low prevalence rates of problem gambling and gambling-related harm in the general population.

While the highest AUCs were identified for the limit relating to gambling expenditure, the highest relative (ratio) and absolute risks were identified for the limit relating to gambling expenditure as a proportion of income. These findings are consistent with the previous Australian study, which suggested that limits relating to gambling expenditure (gambling expenditure and gambling expenditure as a proportion of income) were consistently the best-performing (Dowling et al., 2021). Based on previous evidence (Dowling et al., 2021), promoting these gambling expenditure limits will likely identify gamblers who exceed other limits. The limits identified in the current study are generally at the lower end of the range identified in Canada (Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009) and elsewhere (Brosowski et al., 2015), but are very similar to the previous PGSI-based Tasmanian limits (Dowling et al., 2021). The exception is the gambling expenditure as a proportion of gross personal income limit. While this limit was very similar for PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm (10.2 and 10.3%, respectively), it is considerably higher in this study than in the previous study (1.7%). Given that the gambling expenditure limit is very similar across these studies, the most likely explanation is the use of very refined personal gross income categories, which were deliberately introduced into the fourth SEIS for the purposes of these analyses (Dowling et al., 2021). Given that this limit is consistently well-performing and may be preferable to the absolute expenditure limit as it provides a standardised index that is not confounded by annual income (Currie et al., 2017; Dowling et al., 2021; Weinstock, Ledgerwood, & Petry, 2007), future research replicating this limit using more refined income categories is required.

Not all activity-specific limits identified using both PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm displayed acceptable classification accuracy, which is consistent with previous research (Brosowski et al., 2015; Quilty et al., 2014). This is likely due to relatively lower involvement on some specific gambling activities, as well as the inability to link gambling-related harm to specific gambling activities due to the use of a global measure of harm. Limits were identified for horse/dog racing, keno, and sports/other betting using PGSI-Harm; and for EGM gambling, keno, and bingo using SGHS-Harm. Only the bingo-specific expenditure and session duration limits can be compared to previous research, both of which were lower than those previously identified in Canada (Quilty et al., 2014). These differences are likely because of the smaller sample sizes in the current dataset than in previous research (Brosowski et al., 2015; Quilty et al., 2014). The PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm keno limits, however, were very similar. Taken together, these findings highlight the need for further research evaluating activity-specific limits for Australia using larger population-representative studies.

Effect of defining harms across different domains

It is generally agreed that gambling harm occurs on a continuum from mild to severe (Browne & Rockloff, 2019b). However, for the pragmatic purpose of setting low-risk gambling limits using a risk ratio approach, it is necessary to adopt a threshold which defines an unacceptable level of harm. In the absence of consensus on where this limit lies, the effect of using a definition of harm in which at least two harms across different domains were endorsed was explored using the pool of PGSI negative consequence and SGHS items. Although Cross-domain-Harm produced more robust and consistently higher limits, and was exceeded by slightly fewer gamblers, than Any-domain-Harm, the requirement that gamblers endorse harms across at least two different domains of harm had a relatively negligible effect. Moreover, the limits were very similar to those identified using PGSI-Harm and SGHS-Harm. Taken together, these findings are consistent with previous findings that, when using a risk ratio approach, low-risk gambling limits are generally robust to methodological variations in operationalising harm, suggesting that different harm outcomes index similar constructs (Brosowski et al., 2015; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009).

Study limitations and future research

There has been considerable recent debate regarding the measurement of gambling-related harm that is useful to inform public policy and regulation (Angus, Anjoul, Shannon, & Blaszczynski, 2019; Delfabbro & King, 2019) and validated and interpretable measures have been slow to develop. Although it is the most widely employed measure of gambling harm, the SGHS only includes items across a limited number of domains, although scale scores appear to capture well the index of gambling-related harm assessed by the full 72-item checklist. The SGHS has also been criticised on the grounds that it fails to differentiate between opportunity cost and general harm and may encourage an overestimation of the degree of harm experienced by lower-risk gamblers (Delfabbro & King, 2019). Future research replicating these findings using alternative instruments as they emerge (Quilty, Watson, & Bagby, 2015; Shannon, Anjoul, & Blaszczynski, 2017) or more objective indicators of harm (Dowling et al., 2021) is therefore recommended. The robustness of the limits across measures suggests that using either measure is appropriate, although they appear to classify respondents differently in terms of true positives. Given that gambling-related harm is understood as on a continuum ranging from negligible to extreme, however, an ideal additive measure would comprise a linear metric scale, in which there are equal differences in magnitude between scores, which represent equal differences on decrements to health and wellbeing. While it is likely that the SGHS is superior to the PGSI in approximating these decrements, research validating harms-based measures on a metric scale is required.

More broadly, this research provides an important step in refining low-risk gambling limits when using a risk ratio approach, which has been employed by all of the available literature, to date. This approach, however, requires the selection of a somewhat arbitrary threshold for gambling-related harm, suggesting that a reasonable approach may be to identify a robust estimate over multiple samples that explicates where harms reliably exceed any recreational benefits from gambling (Rockloff, Browne, Russell, Merkouris, & Dowling, 2019) or to examine thresholds that are associated with a non-negligible expected decrement to health and wellbeing. Alternatively, like low-risk drinking limits (NHMRC, 2020; Rehm, Room, & Taylor, 2008; Room & Rehm, 2012), low-risk gambling limits could be made on the basis of the amount of absolute risk that can be tolerated (Dowling et al., 2021; Markham, Young, & Doran, 2016). Previous findings that gambling risk (dose-response) curves are either r-shaped or linear rather than J-shaped (Dowling et al., 2018; Markham et al., 2016) and that gambling consumption lower than the low-risk limits also confers a considerable degree of risk (Dowling et al., 2021) raise questions regarding the degree to which there is any level of gambling behaviour that is not associated with harm (Dowling et al., 2021; Markham et al., 2016). However, further research determining the amount of tolerable absolute risk from gambling (Rehm, Lachenmeier, & Room, 2014), as well as the actual shape of gambling risk curves (Currie et al., 2017; Markham et al., 2016), is required before absolute risk methods can be employed to identify low-risk gambling limits (Dowling et al., 2021).

Other limitations of this research include the under-representation of younger adults, use of self-reported measures of gambling involvement, the inability to link gambling-related harms to specific gambling activities, and the reduced sample sizes for analysing each game separately. Further research is required to identify a consistent set of gambling activity-specific limits.

Study implications

This study advances the currently available literature by deriving limits using a measure of harms attributable to gambling. The similarity in the limits identified using the PGSI and the SGHS suggest that previous PGSI-based limits are robust data-derived thresholds that provide important information about an individual’s risk for experiencing harm based on their gambling consumption. The current study also replicates the overall limits identified in Australia (Dowling et al., 2021), suggesting that they could serve as working guidelines for the consideration of gambling stakeholders to usefully augment the currently available behavioural ‘responsible gambling’ guidelines. Low-risk limits can allow consumers to make informed choices about personal risk; serve as a cost-effective screening method for identifying people at higher-risk for gambling-related harm; monitor the prevalence of gambling-related harm in population-level research; investigate the efficacy of secondary intervention efforts; and be applied in tertiary intervention settings for gamblers selecting a moderation goal (Currie et al., 2017; Currie et al., 2011; Currie et al., 2006; Currie, Hodgins, Wang, El-Guebaly, Wynne, et al., 2008; Currie et al., 2009; Quilty et al., 2014; Weinstock et al., 2007).

Conclusion

This study was designed in response to criticisms that low-risk gambling limits have exclusively been derived using item subsets of problem gambling severity measures. The findings suggest that the overall limits identified using the PGSI, the most commonly employed measure of problem gambling severity, and the SGHS, a measure specifically designed to measure the harms attributable to gambling, are similar and that the requirement that gamblers endorse harms across different domains of harm had a relatively negligible effect on these limits. Although replication using instruments that measure harm across additional domains is required, the findings suggest that previous PGSI-based limits are robust thresholds that have considerable potential utility in the prevention of gambling-related harm.

Funding sources

The study involved the analysis of data collected for the Fourth Social and Economic Impact Study of Gambling in Tasmania, which was funded by the Tasmanian Government Department of Treasury and Finance.

Authors’ contribution

ND conceptualised and designed the study. CG and GY were involved in data preparation, statistical analysis, and interpretation of data. ND, SM, GY, MB, MR and PM obtained funding. ND wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript. All authors had full access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare in relation to this article. The 3-year declaration of interest statement of this research team is as follows: ND, SM, GY, MB, MR and PM have received funding from multiple sources, including government departments and the Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation (through hypothecated taxes from gambling revenue). ND, SM and GY have also received funding from the National Association for Gambling Studies (NAGSs), a not-for-profit organisation with individual members across all stakeholder groups, which derives its funding from member fees and conference proceeds. ND and SM have also received funding from the International Center for Responsible Gaming (ICRG), a charitable organisation, which derives its funding from through contributions from multiple stakeholder groups (with funding decisions the responsibility of an independent scientific advisory board). ND is the recipient of a Deakin University Faculty of Health Mid-Career Fellowship. SM is the recipient of a New South Wales Office of Responsible Gambling Postdoctoral Fellowship and has formerly been the Victorian state representative (unpaid) on the NAGS Executive Committee. None of the authors have knowingly received research funding from the gambling, tobacco, or alcohol industries or any industry-sponsored organisation.

References

  • ACIL Allen, Deakin University, Central Queensland University, & Social Research Centre. (2017). Fourth social and economic impact study of gambling in Tasmania. Tasmania: Tasmanian Department of Treasury and Finance.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Association of Public Opinion Research. (2011). Standard definitions: final disposition of case codes and outcome rates for surveys (7 ed.). AAPOR.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Angus, D. J., Anjoul, F., Shannon, K., & Blaszczynski, A. (2019). Gambling related harms – Community and clinical comparisons. Addiction Research & Theory, 28(3), 194203. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2019.1622001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brosowski, T., Hayer, T., Meyer, G., Rumpf, H. J., John, U., Bischof, A., et al. (2015). Thresholds of probable problematic gambling involvement for the German population: Results of the pathological gambling and epidemiology (PAGE) study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29(3), 794804. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000088.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., Goodwin, B. C., & Rockloff, M. J. (2018). Validation of the Short gambling harm screen (SGHS): A tool for assessment of harms from gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(2), 499512. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9698-y.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2017). The dangers of conflating gambling-related harm with disordered gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(3), 317320. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.059.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2019a). Measuring behavioural dependence in gambling: A case for removing harmful consequences from the assessment of problem gambling pathology. Journal of Gambling Studies. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09916-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2019b). Measuring harm from gambling and estimating its distribution in the population. In H. Bowden-Jones, C. Dickson, C. Dunand & O. Simon (Eds.), Harm reduction for problem gambling: A public health approach. Routledge.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Casswell, S. (2012). Why have guidelines at all? A critical perspective. Drug and Alcohol Review, 31(2), 151152. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00376.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., & Casey, D. M. (2013). Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(2), 311327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Casey, D. M., El-Guebaly, N., Smith, G. J., Williams, R. J., et al. (2017). Deriving low-risk gambling limits from longitudinal data collected in two independent Canadian studies. Addiction, 112(11), 20112020. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13909.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Casey, D. M., el-Guebaly, N., Smith, G. J., Williams, R. J., et al. (2011). Examining the predictive validity of low-risk gambling limits with longitudinal data. Addiction, 107(2), 400406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03622.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., & Wynne, H. (2008). In pursuit of empirically based responsible gambling limits. International Gambling Studies, 8(2), 207227. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459790802172265.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., Wynne, H., & Chen, S. (2006). Risk of harm among gamblers in the general population as a function of level of participation in gambling activities. Addiction, 101(4), 570580. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01392.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., Wynne, H., & Miller, N. V. (2008). Replication of low-risk gambling limits using canadian provincial gambling prevalence data. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(3), 321335. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-008-9091-y.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Miller, N., Hodgins, D. C., & Wang, J. L. (2009). Defining a threshold of harm from gambling for population health surveillance research. International Gambling Studies, 9(1), 1938. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459790802652209.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davern, M., Rodin, H., Beebe, T. J., & Call, K. T. (2005). The effect of income question design in health surveys on family income, poverty and eligibility estimates. Health Services Research, 40(5), 15341552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6773.2005.00416.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delfabbro, P., & King, D. L. (2019). Challenges in the conceptualisation and measurement of gambling-related harm. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(3), 743755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09844-1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N., Greenwood, C., Merkouris, S., Youssef, G., Suomi, A., & Room, R. (2018). The identification of empirically derived Australian responsible gambling limits. In Report prepared for the Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N. A., Youssef, G. J., Greenwood, C., Merkouris, S. S., Suomi, A., & Room, R. (2021). The development of empirically derived Australian low-risk gambling limits. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10, 167. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10020167.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Kim, S., Egerter, S., Cubbin, C., Takahashi, E. R., & Braveman, P. (2007). Potential implications of missing income data in population-based surveys: An example from a postpartum survey in California. Public Health Reports, 122(6), 753763. https://doi.org/10.1177/003335490712200607.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Korn, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (1999). Gambling and the health of the public: Adopting a public health perspective. Journal of Gambling Studies, 15(4), 289365.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Langham, E., Thorne, H., Browne, M., Donaldson, P., Rose, J., & Rockloff, M. (2016). Understanding gambling related harm: a proposed definition, conceptual framework, and taxonomy of harms. BMC Public Health, 16, 80. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-2747-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Markham, F., Young, M., & Doran, B. (2016). The relationship between player losses and gambling-related harm: Evidence from nationally representative cross-sectional surveys in four countries. Addiction, 111, 320330. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13178.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC]. (2020). Australian guidelines to reduce health risks from drinking alcohol. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neal, P., Delfabbro, P., & O’Neil, M. (2005). Problem gambling and harm: toward a national definition. Melbourne: Office of Gaming and Racing.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quilty, L. C., Avila Murati, D., & Bagby, R. M. (2014). Identifying indicators of harmful and problem gambling in a Canadian sample through receiver operating characteristic analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 229237. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032801.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quilty, L. C., Watson, C., & Bagby, M. R. (2015). CPGI-population harm: A supplement to the Canadian problem gambling index. Canadian Journal of Addiction, 6(2), 2028.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rehm, J., Lachenmeier, D. W., & Room, R. (2014). Why does society accept a higher risk for alcohol than for other voluntary or involuntary risks? BMC Medicine, 12, 16.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. J., Browne, M., Russell, A. M. T., Merkouris, S. S., & Dowling, N. A. (2019). A quantification of the net consumer surplus from gambling participation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(4), 11471162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09845-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Room, R., & Rehm, J. (2012). Clear criteria based on absolute risk: Reforming the basis of guidelines on low-risk drinking. Drug and Alcohol Review, 31(2), 135140. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00398.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ruopp, M. D., Perkins, N. J., Whitcomb, B. W., & Schisterman, E. F. (2008). Youden Index and optimal cut-point estimated from observations affected by a lower limit of detection. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 419430. https://doi.org/10.1002/bimj.200710415.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shannon, K., Anjoul, F., & Blaszczynski, A. (2017). Mapping the proportional distribution of gambling-related harms in a clinical and community sample. International Gambling Studies, 17(3), 366385. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2017.1333131.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StataCorp. (2015). Stata: Release 14.1. Statistical software. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.

  • Swets, J. A., Dawes, R. M., & Monahan, J. (2000). Psychological science can improve diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 1(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1111/1529-1006.001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstock, J., Ledgerwood, D. M., & Petry, N. M. (2007). Association between posttreatment gambling behavior and harm in pathological gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(2), 185193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.185.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Youden, W. J. (1950). Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer, 3, 3235. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0142.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ACIL Allen, Deakin University, Central Queensland University, & Social Research Centre. (2017). Fourth social and economic impact study of gambling in Tasmania. Tasmania: Tasmanian Department of Treasury and Finance.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Association of Public Opinion Research. (2011). Standard definitions: final disposition of case codes and outcome rates for surveys (7 ed.). AAPOR.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Angus, D. J., Anjoul, F., Shannon, K., & Blaszczynski, A. (2019). Gambling related harms – Community and clinical comparisons. Addiction Research & Theory, 28(3), 194203. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2019.1622001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brosowski, T., Hayer, T., Meyer, G., Rumpf, H. J., John, U., Bischof, A., et al. (2015). Thresholds of probable problematic gambling involvement for the German population: Results of the pathological gambling and epidemiology (PAGE) study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29(3), 794804. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000088.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., Goodwin, B. C., & Rockloff, M. J. (2018). Validation of the Short gambling harm screen (SGHS): A tool for assessment of harms from gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(2), 499512. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9698-y.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2017). The dangers of conflating gambling-related harm with disordered gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(3), 317320. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.059.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2019a). Measuring behavioural dependence in gambling: A case for removing harmful consequences from the assessment of problem gambling pathology. Journal of Gambling Studies. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09916-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M., & Rockloff, M. J. (2019b). Measuring harm from gambling and estimating its distribution in the population. In H. Bowden-Jones, C. Dickson, C. Dunand & O. Simon (Eds.), Harm reduction for problem gambling: A public health approach. Routledge.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Casswell, S. (2012). Why have guidelines at all? A critical perspective. Drug and Alcohol Review, 31(2), 151152. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00376.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., & Casey, D. M. (2013). Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(2), 311327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Casey, D. M., El-Guebaly, N., Smith, G. J., Williams, R. J., et al. (2017). Deriving low-risk gambling limits from longitudinal data collected in two independent Canadian studies. Addiction, 112(11), 20112020. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13909.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Casey, D. M., el-Guebaly, N., Smith, G. J., Williams, R. J., et al. (2011). Examining the predictive validity of low-risk gambling limits with longitudinal data. Addiction, 107(2), 400406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03622.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., & Wynne, H. (2008). In pursuit of empirically based responsible gambling limits. International Gambling Studies, 8(2), 207227. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459790802172265.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., Wynne, H., & Chen, S. (2006). Risk of harm among gamblers in the general population as a function of level of participation in gambling activities. Addiction, 101(4), 570580. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01392.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Hodgins, D. C., Wang, J., El-Guebaly, N., Wynne, H., & Miller, N. V. (2008). Replication of low-risk gambling limits using canadian provincial gambling prevalence data. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(3), 321335. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-008-9091-y.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R., Miller, N., Hodgins, D. C., & Wang, J. L. (2009). Defining a threshold of harm from gambling for population health surveillance research. International Gambling Studies, 9(1), 1938. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459790802652209.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davern, M., Rodin, H., Beebe, T. J., & Call, K. T. (2005). The effect of income question design in health surveys on family income, poverty and eligibility estimates. Health Services Research, 40(5), 15341552. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6773.2005.00416.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delfabbro, P., & King, D. L. (2019). Challenges in the conceptualisation and measurement of gambling-related harm. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(3), 743755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09844-1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N., Greenwood, C., Merkouris, S., Youssef, G., Suomi, A., & Room, R. (2018). The identification of empirically derived Australian responsible gambling limits. In Report prepared for the Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N. A., Youssef, G. J., Greenwood, C., Merkouris, S. S., Suomi, A., & Room, R. (2021). The development of empirically derived Australian low-risk gambling limits. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10, 167. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10020167.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Kim, S., Egerter, S., Cubbin, C., Takahashi, E. R., & Braveman, P. (2007). Potential implications of missing income data in population-based surveys: An example from a postpartum survey in California. Public Health Reports, 122(6), 753763. https://doi.org/10.1177/003335490712200607.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Korn, D. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (1999). Gambling and the health of the public: Adopting a public health perspective. Journal of Gambling Studies, 15(4), 289365.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Langham, E., Thorne, H., Browne, M., Donaldson, P., Rose, J., & Rockloff, M. (2016). Understanding gambling related harm: a proposed definition, conceptual framework, and taxonomy of harms. BMC Public Health, 16, 80. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-2747-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Markham, F., Young, M., & Doran, B. (2016). The relationship between player losses and gambling-related harm: Evidence from nationally representative cross-sectional surveys in four countries. Addiction, 111, 320330. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13178.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • National Health and Medical Research Council [NHMRC]. (2020). Australian guidelines to reduce health risks from drinking alcohol. Canberra: Commonwealth of Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neal, P., Delfabbro, P., & O’Neil, M. (2005). Problem gambling and harm: toward a national definition. Melbourne: Office of Gaming and Racing.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quilty, L. C., Avila Murati, D., & Bagby, R. M. (2014). Identifying indicators of harmful and problem gambling in a Canadian sample through receiver operating characteristic analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 229237. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032801.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quilty, L. C., Watson, C., & Bagby, M. R. (2015). CPGI-population harm: A supplement to the Canadian problem gambling index. Canadian Journal of Addiction, 6(2), 2028.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rehm, J., Lachenmeier, D. W., & Room, R. (2014). Why does society accept a higher risk for alcohol than for other voluntary or involuntary risks? BMC Medicine, 12, 16.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. J., Browne, M., Russell, A. M. T., Merkouris, S. S., & Dowling, N. A. (2019). A quantification of the net consumer surplus from gambling participation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(4), 11471162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09845-0.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Room, R., & Rehm, J. (2012). Clear criteria based on absolute risk: Reforming the basis of guidelines on low-risk drinking. Drug and Alcohol Review, 31(2), 135140. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1465-3362.2011.00398.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ruopp, M. D., Perkins, N. J., Whitcomb, B. W., & Schisterman, E. F. (2008). Youden Index and optimal cut-point estimated from observations affected by a lower limit of detection. Biometrical Journal, 50(3), 419430. https://doi.org/10.1002/bimj.200710415.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shannon, K., Anjoul, F., & Blaszczynski, A. (2017). Mapping the proportional distribution of gambling-related harms in a clinical and community sample. International Gambling Studies, 17(3), 366385. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2017.1333131.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StataCorp. (2015). Stata: Release 14.1. Statistical software. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.

  • Swets, J. A., Dawes, R. M., & Monahan, J. (2000). Psychological science can improve diagnostic decisions. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 1(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1111/1529-1006.001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstock, J., Ledgerwood, D. M., & Petry, N. M. (2007). Association between posttreatment gambling behavior and harm in pathological gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(2), 185193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.185.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Youden, W. J. (1950). Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer, 3, 3235. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-0142.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jul 2024 0 41 29
Aug 2024 0 88 31
Sep 2024 0 96 50
Oct 2024 0 333 45
Nov 2024 0 377 28
Dec 2024 0 503 17
Jan 2025 0 148 23