Authors:
Christopher M. Jones Institute for Public Health and Nursing Research, University of Bremen, Bremen, Free Hanseatic City of Bremen, Germany

Search for other papers by Christopher M. Jones in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3182-8244
and
Benjamin Noël Institute of Exercise Training and Sport Informatics, German Sport University Cologne, Cologne, Germany

Search for other papers by Benjamin Noël in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0074-4697
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

The sports betting market has been growing rapidly over the last years, as have reports of problematic gambling behavior associated with betting. Due to the well-documented gambling problems of famous athletes and the betting supportive nature of many sports-related environments, athletes have been highlighted as a potential group at-risk for problematic sports betting. However, there currently remains a lack of research on individual-level athlete-specific risk-factors or mechanisms that might contribute to the development and perpetuation of betting-related problems. Here, we examine the influence of two potential risk-factors on sports betting behavior and problems: erroneous beliefs and athletes' emotional involvement.

Methods

201 athletes with different levels of expertise completed a newly developed scale to assess both factors. Participants were sampled from the general German population, predominantly male (83.08%) and on average 29.52 (SD = 11.05) years old. We use principal components analysis to detect patterns of covariation, potentially due to the proposed underlying latent factors, and regression analyses to test associations of these factors with betting behavior and problems.

Results

We find that athletes' emotional involvement was strongly associated with betting problems whereas erroneous beliefs were not. However, distorted cognitions/beliefs were associated with higher volumes and more frequent betting activities.

Discussion and Conclusions

This might contribute to betting problems in the long run. These results highlight athletes' emotional involvement and erroneous beliefs as potential targets for future intervention and prevention efforts.

Abstract

Background and aims

The sports betting market has been growing rapidly over the last years, as have reports of problematic gambling behavior associated with betting. Due to the well-documented gambling problems of famous athletes and the betting supportive nature of many sports-related environments, athletes have been highlighted as a potential group at-risk for problematic sports betting. However, there currently remains a lack of research on individual-level athlete-specific risk-factors or mechanisms that might contribute to the development and perpetuation of betting-related problems. Here, we examine the influence of two potential risk-factors on sports betting behavior and problems: erroneous beliefs and athletes' emotional involvement.

Methods

201 athletes with different levels of expertise completed a newly developed scale to assess both factors. Participants were sampled from the general German population, predominantly male (83.08%) and on average 29.52 (SD = 11.05) years old. We use principal components analysis to detect patterns of covariation, potentially due to the proposed underlying latent factors, and regression analyses to test associations of these factors with betting behavior and problems.

Results

We find that athletes' emotional involvement was strongly associated with betting problems whereas erroneous beliefs were not. However, distorted cognitions/beliefs were associated with higher volumes and more frequent betting activities.

Discussion and Conclusions

This might contribute to betting problems in the long run. These results highlight athletes' emotional involvement and erroneous beliefs as potential targets for future intervention and prevention efforts.

Sports betting is an ever-growing business with rapidly increasing numbers of bettors and company turnover worldwide (Winters & Derevensky, 2019). Recent technological advances and the increasing incorporation of structural characteristics usually found in electronic gambling machines (Newall, Russell, & Hing, 2021) have even resulted in sports betting becoming the most prevalent form of online gambling worldwide (Statista, 2018). Accompanying these developments and expansive trend, more and more treatment seeking gamblers report sports betting as their principal gambling activity, which has seen sports betting develop into a major public health concern (Parke & Parke, 2019).

Simultaneously, several high-profile athletes from different sports – like former England international footballers Andros Townsend and Wayne Rooney, probably the best basketball player of all time, Michael Jordan (among having other gambling problems), and boxer Floyd “Money” Mayweather - have reportedly suffered from problematic sports betting. However, their struggles documented in mass media also point towards a potentially wider problem. Recent research has not only reported elevated levels of sports betting behavior but also betting related problems in athletes (e.g., Nowak, 2018; Ellenbogen, Jacobs, Derevensky, Gupta, & Paskus, 2008; Grall-Bronnec et al., 2016; Vinberg, Durbeej, & Rosendahl, 2020).

Understanding sports betting behavior and problems through a socio-ecological framework of individual-, social network-, community- and societal-levels of influence (Wardle, Reith, Langham, & Rogers, 2019), athletes combine several important general risk factors as well as some factors specific to sports fandom. While overall, young adult males appear to be the most at-risk group (Russell, Hing, & Browne, 2019), research has also highlighted several specific risk factors for those with a strong interest in sports (i.e., athletes and fans). Highlighting individual-level psychosocial factors, feelings of added loyalty towards one's favorite team/player, competition among peers and the demonstration of knowledge as well as feelings of competence have been found as potential drivers of betting behavior (Gordon, Gurrieri, & Chapman, 2015). Perceived elements of skill could thus enhance one's subjective social standing/acceptance and ego (Raymen & Smith, 2020) but also increase excitement (Jenkinson, Lacy-Vawdon, & Carroll, 2018). Additionally, sports bettors have been found to increase their excitement by placing a bet on a game they are watching, because they simply “enjoy a punt” (Jenkinson et al., 2018). It has thus been argued that a strong interest in sports and some features of sports fandom (e.g., identifying with an athlete or team; game related excitement; domain specific knowledge about sports) might increase individual vulnerability to sports betting related problems (Deans, Thomas, Daube, & Derevensky, 2016; Lopez-Gonzalez, Estévez, & Griffiths, 2017).

While athletes might share most of these risk factors (i.e., they most certainly have an interest in the sport they are engaged in and potentially also admire athletes/teams representing world class), they have also been brought into focus as a potential high-risk group with specific distinguishable risk factors by different researchers (e.g., Derevensky et al., 2019; Grall-Bronnec et al., 2016; Weiss & Loubier, 2010). On an individual level, they have been hypothesized to be at an elevated risk for problematic betting due to a general tendency for taking risks and competing with others (Curry & Jiobu, 1995). While the perceived influence of sports specific knowledge on betting outcomes has been reported to be high in fans, too, athletes have additionally been reported to prefer allegedly skill-based forms of gambling (Weiss & Loubier, 2010). This preference has in turn been associated with strong gambling related cognitive distortions in general (Myrseth, Brunborg, & Eidem, 2010) and hints towards an even stronger influence of distorted cognitions in athletes (Derevensky et al., 2019). On an contextual level, research has highlighted the availability of betting venues (Curry & Jiobu, 1995) and more generally high levels of social acceptability of sports betting in athletic environments (for a review: Derevensky et al., 2019) as potentially contributing to the development and perpetuation of betting-related problems. These different factors emphasize the strong individual and contextual relationship between active sports participation and sports betting behavior – over and above the effects of fandom. In turn, this could lead to (former) athletes betting more on sports than fans (Winters & Derevensky, 2019), being more likely to bet on the sport they have played (Weiss & Loubier, 2010) and showing higher prevalence rates for betting-related problems (e.g., Ellenbogen et al., 2008; Grall-Bronnec et al., 2016; Nowak, 2018; Vinberg et al., 2020).

Summarizing these findings, two important clusters of individual-level psychosocial risk factors for sports betting related problems in athletes have been suggested to be distinguishable from more general risk factors (Russell et al., 2019) as well as potentially exceeding the influence of those specific to fans. Both could thus contribute to the development and perpetuation of betting related problems. Firstly, athletes' multilayered emotional involvement has been reported to be an important driver of sports betting. That is, fandom of players/teams, identification with one's sport and players/teams involved, involvement with peers as well as the excitement when watching sports in general is considered exerting an influence (Winters & Derevensky, 2019). Secondly, the perceived influence of domain specific knowledge has been reported to not only contribute to feelings of competency, but also drive actual betting behavior because of expected earnings and perceived higher chances of winning based on one's sports-specific knowledge (Derevensky et al., 2019). However, this perceived influence of knowledge has not only been shown to be false (Khazaal et al., 2012), but also represent the result of distorted cognitions and erroneous beliefs (Cantinotti, Ladouceur, & Jacques, 2004). On the one hand, higher knowledge is not related to a higher probability of winning – here, athletes fall for an illusion of control and overconfidence. On the other hand, athletes might overestimate their ability to predict match outcomes based on information on a team's past performances – this closely resembles the gambler's fallacy.

Likewise, cognitive distortions and erroneous beliefs have been shown to be important predictors of behavior and problems alike across different forms of gambling. They have been argued to play a key role in the development and perpetuation of gambling behavior and influence gambling severity. Medium and robust effect sizes have been reported for different cognitions and beliefs across different types of gambling (for a review: Goodie & Fortune, 2013). Typical distorted cognitions/erroneous beliefs can include different forms of gambling related expectancies, beliefs about predictive control, distorted reframing/interpretation of gambling outcomes, experience and attribution of near-misses and an illusion of control. Although these cognitions and beliefs are not considered diagnostic criteria, they have still been targeted by different interventions and in clinical treatment (Goodie & Fortune, 2013). However, no central process behind the different distortions and beliefs has been identified. Instead, a variety of distinguishable gambling type specific beliefs can be reported – some of them found more regularly and similar in their manifestations across several types of gambling (e.g. gambler's fallacy, cf. Kahneman & Tversky, 1972). In a recent review Goodie and Fortune (2013) accordingly argue that broader identification and assessment of specific beliefs related to each type of gambling can add value to clinical treatment and entry points for prevention (e.g., defining target groups at specific risk). Regarding sports betting specific beliefs and cognitions, recent research suggests that established scales like the Gamblers' Beliefs Questionnaire (GBQ, Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan, 2002) might not be valid as the luck/perseverance and illusion of control-subscales were highly correlated (r = 0.87) instead of representing distinguishable latent constructs (Russell et al., 2019).

In spite of this growing evidence base for the importance of erroneous beliefs/distorted cognitions and emotional involvement in the development and perpetuation of sports betting problems on the one hand and elevated rates of problem gambling driven by facilitating sports-related environments (e.g., availability, social norms etc.) in athletes on the other hand, these two streams of research have yet to be connected. This is especially surprising considering the emphasis sport betting related advertisements have put on emotional involvement as well as potential monetary gains based on domain-specific knowledge (Lopez-Gonzalez, Guerrero-Solé, Estévez, & Griffiths, 2018).

Aims of the present research

The present research thus aims to explore the influence of sports betting-specific cognitive distortions/erroneous beliefs and emotional involvement with sports on betting behavior and problems alike. Both factors are assumed to play a central role in the evaluation of athletes as a group at risk for sports betting related problems.

Specifically, we hypothesize that higher rates of emotional involvement and cognitive distortions/erroneous beliefs are associated with increased betting behavior (number of bets, amount spent; research question 1) as well as symptom experience during the past 12 months (status as problem gambler, number of symptoms, symptom occurrence; research question 2) in athletes.

Methods

Overview

To test whether erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions and emotional involvement are associated with betting related symptoms as well as behavioral outcomes (e.g. betting frequency, wagered amount) in athletes, we conducted a cross-sectional online study.

Sampling

A total sample of 641 athletes or former athletes was recruited from the general population of North Rhine-Westphalia with a special focus of our advertisement efforts on the German Sport University Cologne, Germany, in late 2017. From the total sample, 201 athletes met all inclusion criteria. Inclusion criteria were as follows: Participants had to be at least 18 years old, be able to respond to the online questionnaire in German, have bet money on sports events during the last 12 months and report intermediate or higher levels of expertise in a given sport.

Participants had a mean age of 29.52 (SD = 11.05), were mostly male (83.08%) and reported high socio-economic status (33.83% had completed at least a bachelor's degree, 34.83% earning more than 2000 Euro per month). 47.26% of participants (had) competed on a regional level (intermediate expertise), 32.84% on a supra-regional level (higher expertise) and 11.94 % even on a national level (highest expertise, cf. Swann, Moran, & Piggott, 2015). 53.73% of participants mostly bet on games of their main type of sport. Participants were additionally asked to rate how often (0 = “never” to 100 = “very often”) they had gambled at a casino (M = 7.12; SD = 16.16), an online casino (M = 8.65; SD = 21.89), a lottery (M = 17.23; SD = 29.40), using electronic gambling machines (M = 10.54; SD = 23.35), playing cards (M = 11.74; SD = 20.20) or playing other types of skill-games (M = 9.64; SD = 19.63) during the last 12 months.

Measures

Item development

To date, no scales exist to assess the specific influence of erroneous beliefs, cognitive distortions and emotional involvement on sports betting behavior. However, several scales have been developed and validated to accurately assess erroneous beliefs and cognitive distortions across many other different forms of gambling. Thus, we took several steps to develop new items for this study (the final item list in German and English can be found in the supplementary materials; for details on all other preliminary items, please contact the authors). First, we created a preliminary list of 33 items, loosely based on the widely used GBQ (Steenbergh et al., 2002) and the Gambling Cognitions Inventory (GCI; McInnes, Hodgins, & Holub, 2014). Here, we tried to not only incorporate both underlying factors of the GBQ (luck/perseverance, illusion of control) and the skill/attitude factor of the GCI, biased memory processes (Items 7 and 8), but to explicitly highlight the assumption of the importance of domain specific knowledge within these statements. We also included items addressing the framing of near-misses (Items 9 and 10) as well as the vast amount of options and ways to customize bets (Items 2, 6 and 18), as these might also contribute to the illusion of control – especially when connected to assumed domain knowledge. To additionally address the potential key role of emotional involvement in shaping sports betting behavior, we added domain specific statements (e.g., “Betting on my team even increases my hopes and fears!”).

We then asked two expert practitioners working with treatment seeking sports bettors in Cologne, Germany, to provide feedback and help reduce any redundancies. This process resulted in the 18-item list we provided participants with (all German and English item wordings can be found in the Supplementary Materials). All items were worded as statements and participants rated their agreement on a visual analog scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “absolutely” (100; numeric values were not presented).

Outcome variables

Bets per week and monthly expenses on sports betting were assessed as behavioral outcomes. We asked participants how many bets on sports they had placed per week on average during the last 12 months, with options on an 8-point scale ranging from “never” to “30 or more”. Additionally, participants were asked to specify how much money they had spent on average per month during the last 12 months. Here, participants replied to an open question with only numerical values accepted. It's important to note here, that participants were explicitly asked to only include money they had directly invested and exclude all respent winnings or prizes. In addition to these self-reported behavioral outcomes, we assessed the number of DSM-V criteria (gambling disorder), occurrence of at least one DSM-V criterion as well as experiencing more than three DSM-V criteria (cut-off value for problematic/pathological gambling) during the last 12 months. To assess these nine criteria, we rephrased the original German DSM-V items and instead asked regarding their sports betting e.g., “Thinking about the last 12 months, have you felt the need to increase the amounts of money in order to achieve the desired excitement?” with participants stating “yes”, “no” or “I don't know”. This approach is in line with recent research and enables us to use the same cut-off values for problematic and pathological gambling used in other studies and the DSM-V (although the DSM-V additionally allows to specify current severity as “mild”, “moderate”, “severe”). Additionally, Goodie and Fortune (2013) in a recent review suggested to not only use screening instruments for gambling problems like the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS, Lesieur & Blume, 1987) but to rely on diagnostic criteria instead. This approach offers several benefits as for example a higher degree of classification accuracy (Lakey, Goodie, Lance, Stinchfield, & Winters, 2007) – which could be especially important when generalizing findings to potential intervention or treatment groups.

Analysis

We used principal components analysis (PCA) to detect patterns of covariation between different items which might be due to underlying latent factors, e.g., erroneous thoughts/cognitive distortions and emotional involvement (Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of principal components analysis with oblimin rotation for 18 items (N = 201)

Item Pattern matrix Structure matrix
Cognitive component Involvement component Cognitive component Involvement component Communalities
Item 1 0.47 0.43 0.64 0.62 0.57
Item 2 0.80 0.79 0.29 0.63
Item 3 0.50 0.30 0.62 0.49 0.45
Item 4 0.67 0.22 0.76 0.48 0.61
Item 5 0.64 0.20 0.72 0.45 0.55
Item 6 0.71 0.72 0.31 0.52
Item 7 0.63 0.57 0.34
Item 8 0.55 0.57 0.28 0.33
Item 9 0.45 0.31 0.57 0.48 0.41
Item 10 0.42 0.31 0.54 0.48 0.38
Item 11 0.57 0.64 0.41 0.44
Item 12 0.82 0.84 0.37 0.71
Item 13 0.58 0.41 0.65 0.46
Item 14 0.20 0.69 0.47 0.77 0.62
Item 15 0.81 0.24 0.78 0.62
Item 16 0.70 0.63 0.42
Item 17 0.29 0.53 0.50 0.64 0.49
Item 18 0.88 −0.29 0.77 0.66
Eigenvalue 7.47 1.74
% of variance 32.78 18.42

Note: N = 201. Participants responded to the items on visual analog scales ranging from “not at all” (0) to “absolutely” (100; numeric values were not presented). Factor loadings < 0.2 are suppressed. Italic depicts items not retained in the reduced scales.

We then used the composite score for each scale as predictors of problematic/pathological gambling, occurrence of at least one diagnostic criterion during the last 12 months (logistic regression respectively), number of criteria experienced during the last 12 months, average bets placed per week during last 12 months and average money spent per month (linear regression respectively). Each criterion was predicted in a separate model and we corrected for multiple testing (Bonferroni) with a resulting α of 0.01. We used the same dataset for the PCA as well as the prediction of gambling problems and behavior.

All analyses were conducted using R (Version 3.6.1; R Core Team, 2018) and the R-package psych (Version 1.8.10; Revelle, 2018).

Ethics

The current study was approved by the Ethics board of the German Sport University Cologne (February 2016) in compliance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki 1975 as a part of a proposal for the “Aktionsplan gegen Sucht” (Action plan against addiction) by the Ministry for Health, Equalities, Care and Ageing of the State of North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany.

Results

Principal components analysis

A principal components analysis (PCA) with oblique rotation (“oblimin”) was conducted on the 18 pre-selected items (as described in the methods section). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure supported sampling adequacy (KMO = 0.90) with all KMO values for the individual items above 0.72. Between item correlations sufficiently large enough for PCA were indicated by the results of Barlett's test of sphericity, χ2 (153) = 1770.49, P < 0.001. To determine how many components to extract that account for more variance than is expected by chance (based on multiple sets of random data), we used parallel analysis. As Fig. 1 depicts, the scree-plot was rather ambiguous and the eigenvalue of the potential third component falls slightly below the average value for the corresponding components in the random data sets and should thus be excluded. Thus, we also explored a three-component solution (oblique rotation) but as the third component only consisted of two items and rendered interpreting the pattern matrix difficult, we settled for the suggested two-component solution. As no a priori theoretical reasoning suggested both factors to not be correlated, we then used oblique rotation that resulted in a more interpretable solution. Based on the resulting pattern matrix, we selected items that strongly resembled simple structure by meeting the following criteria: a) loading of above 0.45 on one component and b) lesser cross-loadings than 0.30 on the other component (see e.g. Brown, 2015). A second principal component analysis, conducted on this reduced set of items, revealed that the two-component solution accounted for 54.17% of the variance (component 1: 35.00%, component 2: 19.17%).

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Scree plot with vertical line depicting suggested maximum number of components. To create this plot, we adapted R-code provided by (Sakaluk & Short, 2017)

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 10, 3; 10.1556/2006.2021.00034

Component 1 consisted of nine items aiming to capture erroneous beliefs and cognitive distortions and was thus labeled “cognitive component”. Component 2 consisted of five items aiming to capture different aspects of emotional involvement and was thus labeled “involvement component”. While component 1 indicated nearly excellent internal consistency (α = 0.89), component 2 yielded at least acceptable levels (α = 0.76). However, this might be due to the lower number of items forming component 2.

Composite scores

We created composite scores for both components by taking the mean of all item-values per participant. We did not include weighting for each item (DiStefano, Zhu, & Mindrila, 2009). Both composite score-distributions resemble a normal distribution, with “involvement”-scores slightly left- and “cognitive”-scores slightly right-skewed (Table 2; additional visualizations can be found in the supplements).

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for the reduced two-factor solution (“cognitive component”, “involvement component”; N = 201)

Factor No. of items M (SD) Skewness Kurtosis Cronbach's α
Cognitive Comp. 9 39.87 (23.57) 0.19 −0.76 0.89
Involvement Comp. 5 48.31 (21.84) −0.28 −0.48 0.76

Note: M and SD represent mean and standard deviation, respectively.

Predicting gambling behavior

Addressing research question 1, we had hypothesized that participants' betting behavior was associated with their erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions and their emotional involvement. However, only participants' erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions were significantly associated with the number of bets per week (with emotional evolvement not significant due to Bonferroni correcting) and average monthly expenses. While a one-point higher cognition-scale value was associated with on average 0.03 more bets per week, it was also associated with spending 6.68 Euros more per week on average.

Predicting gambling problems

Addressing research question 2, we had hypothesized that participants dichotomized status as non-problem or problem/pathological gamblers was associated with their erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions and their emotional involvement. Tables 3 and 4 show that only participants emotional involvement was significantly associated with problem/pathological gambling (β = 0.05, P < 0.01, OR = 1.05) and the number of criteria experienced (β = 0.04, P < 0.01). However, when looking at experience of any criterion during the past 12 months, we find that erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions as well as emotional involvement both are significantly associated (Table 3). The stronger effect emotional involvement exerts on gambling problems becomes even more apparent when taking the concurrent effect sizes into account.

Table 3.

Summary of logistic regression models: Parameter estimates, standard errors, odds ratios with confidence intervals of each covariate

Predictor Parameter Estimate (SE) Odds Ratio (95% CI)
Model 1:
Intercept −5.107*** (0.830)
 Cognitive Comp. 0.019 (0.011) 1.02 (0.99, 1.04)
Involvement Comp. 0.047*** (0.014) 1.05 (1.02, 1.08)
Model 2:
Intercept −2.566*** (0.474)
 Cognitive Comp. 0.021** (0.008) 1.02 (1.01, 1.04)
Involvement Comp. 0.027** (0.009) 1.03 (1.01, 1.05)

Note: Model 1: Logistic regression model predicting problem gambling (DSM-V); Model 2: Logistic regression model predicting symptom (DSM-V) occurrence during past 12 months.

* P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001, bold: P < 0.05.

Table 4.

Summary of linear regression models: Parameter estimates, confidence intervals and fit statistics

Predictor b b

95% CI (LL, UL)
beta beta

95% CI (LL, UL)
Fit
Model 1:
Intercept −1.045** (−1.77, −0.32)
 Cognitive Comp. 0.016* (0.00, 0.03) 0.164 (0.02, 0.31)
Involvement Comp. 0.037*** (0.02, 0.05) 0.350 (0.20, 0.50)
R 2 = 0.211**

95% CI (0.11, 0.30)
Model 2:
 Intercept 0.653 (−0.37, 1.68)
Cognitive Comp. 0.031** (0.01, 0.05) 0.231 (0.08, 0.39)
 Involvement Comp. 0.029* (0.01, 0.05) 0.203 (0.05, 0.36)
R 2 = 0.145**
95% CI (0.06, 0.23)
Model 3:
 Intercept −101.081 (−270.48, 68.32)
Cognitive Comp. 6.683** (3.28, 10.09) 0.311 (0.15, 0.47)
 Involvement Comp. 0.509 (−3.17, 4.18) 0.022 (−0.14, 0.18)
R 2 = 0.104**
95% CI (0.03, 0.18)

Note: Model 1: linear regression model predicting number of symptoms (DSM-V) during past 12 months; Model 2: linear regression model predicting average number of bets per week; Model 3: linear regression model predicting average monthly expenses on sports betting during past 12 months. A significant b-weight indicates the beta-weight is also significant. b represents unstandardized regression weights. beta indicates the standardized regression weights. LL and UL indicate the lower and upper limits of a confidence interval, respectively.

* P < 0.05, ** P < 0.01, *** P < 0.001, bold: P < 0.05.

Discussion

The present research aimed to explore the influence of sports betting-specific cognitive distortions/erroneous beliefs and emotional involvement on sports betting behavior (number of bets, amount spent) and problems (status as problem gambler, number of symptoms, symptom occurrence) alike.

In general, athlete participants reported high levels of emotional involvement when connecting betting and sports. On an item-level, the increased excitement is less connected to the act of betting itself (e.g., Items 13 and 17) than to the event connected with the bet (i.e., betting on live events, betting on one's favorite team/player). This is in line with previous research on non-athletes highlighting team/player-loyalty (Gordon et al., 2015) and watching games live (Jenkinson et al., 2018) as important factors driving betting-related excitement. In support of our first hypothesis, these higher rates of emotional involvement were associated with sports betting related problems. Specifically, stronger involvement was associated with problem gambling, the experience of any DSM-V criteria during the last 12 months as well as the number of experienced criteria. These findings expand on previous research that had characterized emotional involvement as typical for sports bettors (Gordon et al., 2015; Jenkinson et al., 2018; Raymen & Smith, 2020) but not yet relevant for psychopathology. Here, we not only show associations with status as problem gambler and symptom experience in general, but rather a linear association of criteria experienced and strength of emotional involvement. Comparing these findings with other recent research on sports betting-specific risk factors in non-athlete samples (e.g. Russell et al., 2019, finding no significant associations in multivariate models), this might indicate first evidence for athletes as a group at risk: Russell et al. (2019) reported that while the expected excitement as well as sports involvement were associated with gambling problems in bivariate models, they were not when included in a multivariate (and additional penalized) model. This difference between the two groups points towards emotional involvement as a potential entry point for future interventions and treatment when targeting athletes.

As gambling problems and gambling behaviors have been reported to be only moderately correlated (r between 0.34 and 0.61; Cowie et al., 2017), examining just one of both does not paint the full picture. Interestingly, in the present study athletes' emotional involvement was not associated with betting behavior outcomes. This might be due to the strong similarities several of the items of the involvement component show with the DSM-V criteria. While these criteria only implicitly integrate concrete gambling behavior, they put a more explicit focus on regulatory and affective aspects – both are strongly represented in our scale, too.

Athlete participants not only report high levels of emotional involvement, but also high levels of sports betting related erroneous beliefs and cognitive distortions. On an item-level, we found strong support for the various beliefs and distortions we presented. While we find less support for customization options contributing to perceived control over the outcome, we find stronger support for the assumed importance of domain specific knowledge. Additionally, participants viewed sports betting outcomes as less random than gambling outcomes and reported potentially distorted memory processes (Items 7 and 8). In support of our second hypothesis, these stronger erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions were associated with more bets per week as well as higher monthly expenses. This is in line with research across different forms of gambling (Cowie et al., 2017) and expands these to the specific sports betting context. Cowie et al. (2017) however report an important distinction: While Luck/Chance-related distortions were associated with gambling behavior as well as problems, Skill/Attitude-related distortions were only associated with gambling behavior. The authors interpret these findings as in line with research suggesting games of skill to bring about persistent behavior (Dickerson, 1993), hypothesizing that higher perceived control over the game could result in higher perceived control of one's gambling and thus more gambling behavior. Following this line of thought, we hypothesize that this perceived control over one's gambling could also result in less reported problem gambling. Interestingly, in the present research erroneous beliefs/cognitive distortions were neither associated with problem gambling nor the experience of any DSM-V criteria during the last 12 months or the number of experienced criteria. While this is in stark contrast to recent efforts by Russell et al. (2019) who reported not-sports specific beliefs/distortions (assessed with the GBQ) to be associated with sports betting problems – even in a penalized multivariate model including a diverse set of predictors – it might be interpreted as supporting the position highlighted before (Cowie et al., 2017).

To summarize these findings, we show that specifically assessing sports betting related cognitions and beliefs could add value to the understanding of related problems (and behavior, as key aspect of the continuous and manifold underlying process) and their development in athletes. Here, the present research expands on previous efforts reporting sports betting specific risk factors in non-athlete samples (Russell et al., 2019), efforts on athlete-specific risk factors (e.g., Derevensky et al., 2019; Grall-Bronnec et al., 2016; Weiss & Loubier, 2010) as well as work on different types of cognitive distortions and their influence on gambling problems and behavior (Cowie et al., 2017). By specifically targeting these cognitions and beliefs as well as athletes' emotional involvement, this approach might add entry points for clinical treatment as well as prevention efforts and policy decisions.

Limitations

However, the present research has some limitations. Looking only at athletes without comparing them to non-athletes, our findings can only serve as a first hint towards declaring athletes as a group at risk because of their emotional involvement and distorted cognitions/erroneous beliefs. Future research should address these questions, especially in the light of current advertising practices (Lopez-Gonzalez et al., 2018). To classify participants as problematic/pathological gamblers, we used DSM-V criteria. However, it has been argued that these criteria might be a better fit in clinical samples and additionally lack the continuous scale of problem severity the Problem Gambling Severity Index offers (Currie, Hodgins, & Casey, 2013). From a methodological standpoint, several limitations should be highlighted. Survey-based research mostly relies on self-report, thus, behavioral outcomes are potentially biased. Additionally, we created new scales to assess emotional involvement and cognitive distortions/erroneous beliefs. Although they are associated with the focal outcomes and results are in line with existing research, both sub-scales are in need of additional validation and generalizable information on their psychometric properties. Additionally, some of the items (e.g., Item 2) could be rephrased and more closely aligned with conceptions of distorted cognitions – this would lend additional validity to all following claims. The preliminary nature of both scales is further based on the rather small sample size. Although more than ten participants per item included in the PCA completed the survey, a larger and more diverse sample could substantiate generalization of findings. Although young males with higher education have been repeatedly reported as risk group, also including more non-male athletes and participants from lower SES groups should benefit the course. This also means, that the findings reported above can in no way be generalized across genders. Especially in light of mixed results on female athletes' gambling behavior and problems (Nelson et al., 2007; Vinberg et al., 2020) we caution against any undue generalization.

Implications

In spite of these limitations, the present research also shows some potential. We find that both subscales help to expand on previous findings as they are associated with betting related problems as well as behavior in athletes. This is especially relevant in the light of recent findings suggesting that established general gambling related cognitive distortion scales like the GBQ might not be valid in the context of sports betting (Russell et al., 2019) and more general requests to identify specific beliefs/cognitions related to each type of gambling (Goodie & Fortune, 2013). Accordingly, we adopted a sports betting specific perspective, highlighting specific entry points for future treatment as well as prevention efforts and policy decisions.

Funding sources

This research was supported by a grant from the Ministry for Health, Equalities, Care and Ageing of the State of North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany, (now: Ministry for Regional Identity, Communities and Local Government, Building and Gender Equality) as a part of the “Aktionsplan gegen Sucht” (“Action plan against addiction”), project number 17. However, the funding source had no involvement in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of the data, writing of this paper as well as in the decision to submit this article for publication.

Authors' contribution

CMJ and BN contributed equally to the concept and design of the study, the statistical analyses as well as interpretation of the findings and the writing of this paper. Thus, both authors had access to the full data and take responsibility for the integrity of the findings presented here. BN obtained the funding for this project.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare no conflict of interest.

Human and animal rights and informed consent

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the local institutional ethics committee and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study. This consent was obtained from each participant before starting the survey.

Supplementary material

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00034.

All data and analysis scripts that support the findings of this study will be made available upon publication at https://osf.io/rns7g/

References

  • Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research (2nd ed, pp. xvii, 462). The Guilford Press.

  • Cantinotti, M. , Ladouceur, R. , & Jacques, C. (2004). Sports betting: Can gamblers beat randomness? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18(2), 143147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.18.2.143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cowie, M. E. , Stewart, S. H. , Salmon, J. , Collins, P. , Al-Hamdani, M. , Boffo, M. , et al. (2017). Distorted beliefs about luck and skill and their relation to gambling problems and gambling behavior in Dutch gamblers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02245.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R. , Hodgins, D. C. , & Casey, D. M. (2013). Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(2), 311327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curry, T. J. , & Jiobu, R. M. (1995). Do motives matter? Modeling gambling on sports among athletes. Sociology of Sport Journal, 12(1), 2135. https://doi.org/10.1123/ssj.12.1.21.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deans, E. G. , Thomas, S. L. , Daube, M. , & Derevensky, J. (2016). “I can sit on the beach and punt through my mobile phone”: The influence of physical and online environments on the gambling risk behaviours of young men. Social Science & Medicine, 166, 110119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.017.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Derevensky, J. L. , McDuff, D. , Reardon, C. L. , Hainline, B. , Hitchcock, M. E. , & Richard, J. (2019). Problem gambling and associated mental health concerns in elite athletes: A narrative review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 53(12), 761766. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2019-100668.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dickerson, M. (1993). Internal and external determinants of persistent gambling: Problems in generalising from one form of gambling to another. Journal of Gambling Studies, 9(3), 225245. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01015920.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DiStefano, C. , Zhu, M. , & Mindrila, D. (2009). Understanding and using factor scores: Considerations for the applied researcher. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 14(20). https://doi.org/10.7275/da8t-4g52.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellenbogen, S. , Jacobs, D. , Derevensky, J. , Gupta, R. , & Paskus, T. (2008). Gambling behavior among college student-athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20(3), 349362. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200802056685.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goodie, A. S. , & Fortune, E. E. (2013). Measuring cognitive distortions in pathological gambling: Review and meta-analyses. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), 730743. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031892.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gordon, R. , Gurrieri, L. , & Chapman, M. (2015). Broadening an understanding of problem gambling: The lifestyle consumption community of sports betting. Journal of Business Research, 68(10), 21642172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.03.016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grall-Bronnec, M. , Caillon, J. , Humeau, E. , Perrot, B. , Remaud, M. , Guilleux, A. , et al. (2016). Gambling among European professional athletes. Prevalence and associated factors. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 35(4), 278290. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550887.2016.1177807.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jenkinson, R. , Lacy-Vawdon, C. de , & Carroll, M. (2018). Weighing up the odds: Young men, sports and betting (Victoria) [Report]. Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation. https://apo.org.au/node/184181.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kahneman, D. , & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khazaal, Y. , Chatton, A. , Billieux, J. , Bizzini, L. , Monney, G. , Fresard, E. , et al. (2012). Effects of expertise on football betting. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 7(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-7-18.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lakey, C. E. , Goodie, A. S. , Lance, C. E. , Stinchfield, R. , & Winters, K. C. (2007). Examining DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling: Psychometric properties and evidence from cognitive biases. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23(4), 479. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-007-9063-7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lesieur, H. R. , & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 11841188. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , Estévez, A. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Marketing and advertising online sports betting: A problem gambling perspective. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 41(3), 256272. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193723517705545.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , Guerrero-Solé, F. , Estévez, A. , & Griffiths, M. (2018). Betting is loving and bettors are predators: A conceptual metaphor approach to online sports betting advertising. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(3), 709726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9727-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McInnes, A. , Hodgins, D. C. , & Holub, A. (2014). The Gambling Cognitions Inventory: Scale development and psychometric validation with problem and pathological gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 14(3), 410431. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2014.923483.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Myrseth, H. , Brunborg, G. S. , & Eidem, M. (2010). Differences in cognitive distortions between pathological and non-pathological gamblers with preferences for chance or skill games. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(4), 561569. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9180-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nelson, T. F. , LaBrie, R. A. , LaPlante, D. A. , Stanton, M. , Shaffer, H. J. , & Wechsler, H. (2007). Sports betting and other gambling in athletes, fans, and other college students. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(4), 271283. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2007.10599425.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Newall, P. W. , Russell, A. M. , & Hing, N. (2021). Structural characteristics of fixed-odds sports betting products. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nowak, D. E. Jr . (2018). A meta-analytical synthesis and examination of pathological and problem gambling rates among college athletes. Journal for the Study of Sports and Athletes in Education, 12(3), 240257. https://doi.org/10.1080/19357397.2018.1525143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parke, A. , & Parke, J. (2019). Transformation of sports betting into a rapid and continuous gambling activity: A grounded theoretical investigation of problem sports betting in online settings. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17(6), 13401359. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-0049-8.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • R Core Team . (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved December 8, 2018, from https://www.R-project.org/.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raymen, T. , & Smith, O. (2020). Lifestyle gambling, indebtedness and anxiety: A deviant leisure perspective. Journal of Consumer Culture, 20(4), 381399. https://doi.org/10.1177/1469540517736559.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Revelle, W. (2018). Psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Evanston, Il: Northwestern University. Retrieved December 8, 2018, from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russell, A. M. T. , Hing, N. , & Browne, M. (2019). Risk factors for gambling problems specifically associated with sports betting. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(4), 12111228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09848-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sakaluk, J. K. , & Short, S. D. (2017). A methodological review of exploratory factor analysis in sexuality research: Used practices, best practices, and data analysis resources. The Journal of Sex Research, 54(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2015.1137538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Statista . (2018). Sports betting (gambling) dossier. In Statista - The Statistics Portal. Retrieved September 24, 2020, from https://www.statista.com/study/18015/sports-betting-statista-dossier/.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the gamblers' beliefs questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swann, C. , Moran, A. , & Piggott, D. (2015). Defining elite athletes: Issues in the study of expert performance in sport psychology. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.004.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vinberg, M. , Durbeej, N. , & Rosendahl, I. (2020). Gambling and gambling problem among elite athletes and their professional coaches: Findings from a Swedish total population survey of participants in four sports. International Gambling Studies, 20(2), 262281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2020.1726990.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wardle, H. , Reith, G. , Langham, E. , & Rogers, R. D. (2019). Gambling and public health: We need policy action to prevent harm. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 365, l1807. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l1807.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weiss, S. M. , & Loubier, S. L. (2010). Gambling habits of athletes and nonathletes classified as disordered gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 144(6), 507521. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2010.503248.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Winters, K. C. , & Derevensky, J. L. (2019). A review of sports wagering: Prevalence, characteristics of sports bettors, and association with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Issues, 43(0), Article 0. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2019.43.7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Supplementary Materials

  • Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research (2nd ed, pp. xvii, 462). The Guilford Press.

  • Cantinotti, M. , Ladouceur, R. , & Jacques, C. (2004). Sports betting: Can gamblers beat randomness? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18(2), 143147. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.18.2.143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cowie, M. E. , Stewart, S. H. , Salmon, J. , Collins, P. , Al-Hamdani, M. , Boffo, M. , et al. (2017). Distorted beliefs about luck and skill and their relation to gambling problems and gambling behavior in Dutch gamblers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02245.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Currie, S. R. , Hodgins, D. C. , & Casey, D. M. (2013). Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(2), 311327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curry, T. J. , & Jiobu, R. M. (1995). Do motives matter? Modeling gambling on sports among athletes. Sociology of Sport Journal, 12(1), 2135. https://doi.org/10.1123/ssj.12.1.21.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deans, E. G. , Thomas, S. L. , Daube, M. , & Derevensky, J. (2016). “I can sit on the beach and punt through my mobile phone”: The influence of physical and online environments on the gambling risk behaviours of young men. Social Science & Medicine, 166, 110119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.08.017.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Derevensky, J. L. , McDuff, D. , Reardon, C. L. , Hainline, B. , Hitchcock, M. E. , & Richard, J. (2019). Problem gambling and associated mental health concerns in elite athletes: A narrative review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 53(12), 761766. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2019-100668.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dickerson, M. (1993). Internal and external determinants of persistent gambling: Problems in generalising from one form of gambling to another. Journal of Gambling Studies, 9(3), 225245. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01015920.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DiStefano, C. , Zhu, M. , & Mindrila, D. (2009). Understanding and using factor scores: Considerations for the applied researcher. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 14(20). https://doi.org/10.7275/da8t-4g52.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellenbogen, S. , Jacobs, D. , Derevensky, J. , Gupta, R. , & Paskus, T. (2008). Gambling behavior among college student-athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20(3), 349362. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200802056685.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goodie, A. S. , & Fortune, E. E. (2013). Measuring cognitive distortions in pathological gambling: Review and meta-analyses. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(3), 730743. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031892.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gordon, R. , Gurrieri, L. , & Chapman, M. (2015). Broadening an understanding of problem gambling: The lifestyle consumption community of sports betting. Journal of Business Research, 68(10), 21642172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.03.016.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grall-Bronnec, M. , Caillon, J. , Humeau, E. , Perrot, B. , Remaud, M. , Guilleux, A. , et al. (2016). Gambling among European professional athletes. Prevalence and associated factors. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 35(4), 278290. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550887.2016.1177807.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jenkinson, R. , Lacy-Vawdon, C. de , & Carroll, M. (2018). Weighing up the odds: Young men, sports and betting (Victoria) [Report]. Victorian Responsible Gambling Foundation. https://apo.org.au/node/184181.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kahneman, D. , & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430454. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(72)90016-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khazaal, Y. , Chatton, A. , Billieux, J. , Bizzini, L. , Monney, G. , Fresard, E. , et al. (2012). Effects of expertise on football betting. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 7(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-7-18.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lakey, C. E. , Goodie, A. S. , Lance, C. E. , Stinchfield, R. , & Winters, K. C. (2007). Examining DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling: Psychometric properties and evidence from cognitive biases. Journal of Gambling Studies, 23(4), 479. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-007-9063-7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lesieur, H. R. , & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 11841188. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , Estévez, A. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Marketing and advertising online sports betting: A problem gambling perspective. Journal of Sport and Social Issues, 41(3), 256272. https://doi.org/10.1177/0193723517705545.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , Guerrero-Solé, F. , Estévez, A. , & Griffiths, M. (2018). Betting is loving and bettors are predators: A conceptual metaphor approach to online sports betting advertising. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(3), 709726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-017-9727-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McInnes, A. , Hodgins, D. C. , & Holub, A. (2014). The Gambling Cognitions Inventory: Scale development and psychometric validation with problem and pathological gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 14(3), 410431. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2014.923483.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Myrseth, H. , Brunborg, G. S. , & Eidem, M. (2010). Differences in cognitive distortions between pathological and non-pathological gamblers with preferences for chance or skill games. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(4), 561569. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9180-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nelson, T. F. , LaBrie, R. A. , LaPlante, D. A. , Stanton, M. , Shaffer, H. J. , & Wechsler, H. (2007). Sports betting and other gambling in athletes, fans, and other college students. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(4), 271283. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2007.10599425.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Newall, P. W. , Russell, A. M. , & Hing, N. (2021). Structural characteristics of fixed-odds sports betting products. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00008.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nowak, D. E. Jr . (2018). A meta-analytical synthesis and examination of pathological and problem gambling rates among college athletes. Journal for the Study of Sports and Athletes in Education, 12(3), 240257. https://doi.org/10.1080/19357397.2018.1525143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parke, A. , & Parke, J. (2019). Transformation of sports betting into a rapid and continuous gambling activity: A grounded theoretical investigation of problem sports betting in online settings. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17(6), 13401359. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-0049-8.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • R Core Team . (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved December 8, 2018, from https://www.R-project.org/.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raymen, T. , & Smith, O. (2020). Lifestyle gambling, indebtedness and anxiety: A deviant leisure perspective. Journal of Consumer Culture, 20(4), 381399. https://doi.org/10.1177/1469540517736559.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Revelle, W. (2018). Psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Evanston, Il: Northwestern University. Retrieved December 8, 2018, from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russell, A. M. T. , Hing, N. , & Browne, M. (2019). Risk factors for gambling problems specifically associated with sports betting. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(4), 12111228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09848-x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sakaluk, J. K. , & Short, S. D. (2017). A methodological review of exploratory factor analysis in sexuality research: Used practices, best practices, and data analysis resources. The Journal of Sex Research, 54(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2015.1137538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Statista . (2018). Sports betting (gambling) dossier. In Statista - The Statistics Portal. Retrieved September 24, 2020, from https://www.statista.com/study/18015/sports-betting-statista-dossier/.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the gamblers' beliefs questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swann, C. , Moran, A. , & Piggott, D. (2015). Defining elite athletes: Issues in the study of expert performance in sport psychology. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 16, 314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.07.004.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vinberg, M. , Durbeej, N. , & Rosendahl, I. (2020). Gambling and gambling problem among elite athletes and their professional coaches: Findings from a Swedish total population survey of participants in four sports. International Gambling Studies, 20(2), 262281. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2020.1726990.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wardle, H. , Reith, G. , Langham, E. , & Rogers, R. D. (2019). Gambling and public health: We need policy action to prevent harm. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 365, l1807. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l1807.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weiss, S. M. , & Loubier, S. L. (2010). Gambling habits of athletes and nonathletes classified as disordered gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 144(6), 507521. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2010.503248.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Winters, K. C. , & Derevensky, J. L. (2019). A review of sports wagering: Prevalence, characteristics of sports bettors, and association with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Issues, 43(0), Article 0. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2019.43.7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2024 0 244 34
Nov 2024 0 542 71
Dec 2024 0 597 43
Jan 2025 0 256 23
Feb 2025 0 231 28
Mar 2025 0 398 38
Apr 2025 0 0 0