Authors:
Nerilee Hing Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Nerilee Hing in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2150-9784
,
Matthew Rockloff Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Matthew Rockloff in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0080-2690
,
Alex M. T. Russell Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Alex M. T. Russell in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3685-7220
,
Matthew Browne Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Matthew Browne in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2668-6229
,
Philip Newall Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Philip Newall in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1660-9254
,
Nancy Greer Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Nancy Greer in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6045-1984
,
Daniel L. King College of Education, Psychology, and Social Work, Flinders University, Australia

Search for other papers by Daniel L. King in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1762-2581
, and
Hannah Thorne Experimental Gambling Research Laboratory, School of Health, Medical and Applied Sciences, CQUniversity, Australia

Search for other papers by Hannah Thorne in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2830-2911
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Purchasing loot boxes in digital games is akin to gambling as it involves risking money for a chance-based reward of uncertain value. Research has linked buying loot boxes to problem gambling amongst adolescents, but has not examined co-occurring gambling participation. This study examined links between loot box purchasing and problem gambling amongst adolescents while controlling for monetary gambling participation.

Methods

Two survey samples of Australians aged 12–17 years were recruited through advertisements (n = 843) and online panels (n = 826). They included n = 421 and n = 128 adolescents, respectively, who met criteria for problem gambling.

Results

Past-month loot box purchasing was significantly related to gambling problems in bivariate analyses. When including age, gender and past-month monetary gambling, loot box purchases were still associated with at-risk and problem gambling in both samples. As expected, these other predictors attenuated the predictive value of recent loot box purchases in relation to gambling problems. The odds-ratios, nevertheless, were still in the predicted direction and remained significant. When controlling for monetary gambling, age and gender, recent loot box purchasing increased the odds of problem gambling 3.7 to 6.0 times, and at-risk gambling 2.8 to 4.3 times.

Discussion and conclusions

While causal relationships between loot box purchasing and problem gambling remain unclear, the results indicate that loot boxes disproportionately attract adolescents experiencing gambling problems, adding to the financial stress already caused by gambling. Consumer protection measures, youth and parental education, and age restrictions on loot box games are needed to protect young people.

Abstract

Background and aims

Purchasing loot boxes in digital games is akin to gambling as it involves risking money for a chance-based reward of uncertain value. Research has linked buying loot boxes to problem gambling amongst adolescents, but has not examined co-occurring gambling participation. This study examined links between loot box purchasing and problem gambling amongst adolescents while controlling for monetary gambling participation.

Methods

Two survey samples of Australians aged 12–17 years were recruited through advertisements (n = 843) and online panels (n = 826). They included n = 421 and n = 128 adolescents, respectively, who met criteria for problem gambling.

Results

Past-month loot box purchasing was significantly related to gambling problems in bivariate analyses. When including age, gender and past-month monetary gambling, loot box purchases were still associated with at-risk and problem gambling in both samples. As expected, these other predictors attenuated the predictive value of recent loot box purchases in relation to gambling problems. The odds-ratios, nevertheless, were still in the predicted direction and remained significant. When controlling for monetary gambling, age and gender, recent loot box purchasing increased the odds of problem gambling 3.7 to 6.0 times, and at-risk gambling 2.8 to 4.3 times.

Discussion and conclusions

While causal relationships between loot box purchasing and problem gambling remain unclear, the results indicate that loot boxes disproportionately attract adolescents experiencing gambling problems, adding to the financial stress already caused by gambling. Consumer protection measures, youth and parental education, and age restrictions on loot box games are needed to protect young people.

Introduction

Loot boxes are digital containers that can be purchased or won within the majority of popular video games (Rockloff et al., 2020; Zendle et al., 2020a). When opened, loot boxes reveal virtual items such as weapons or special abilities that can enhance game-play performance and progression in games (Drummond & Sauer, 2018; Parent Zone, 2019). Loot boxes can also contain skins (cosmetic items) that have aesthetic or prestige value, and can sometimes be sold for real money, traded or gambled on third-party websites (Greer, Rockloff, Browne, Hing, & King, 2019; Parent Zone, 2019). Some loot boxes yield in-game currency that can be spent in the game to progress or purchase in-game items. In essence, loot boxes are like lucky dips where a prize is guaranteed but its value is unknown in advance of opening (Rockloff et al., 2021).

Purchasing loot boxes has similar structural characteristics to gambling because it involves spending money on a chance-based reward of uncertain value (Greer et al., 2019; Zendle, Meyer, & Over, 2019, 2020a, 2020b). Many loot boxes meet the legal criteria for gambling and are thought to have similar psychological drivers (Drummond, Sauer, Hall, Zendle, & Loudon, 2020). The random allocation of rewards of varying value means that players do not know how many loot boxes they need to purchase to obtain a desirable item (Drummond & Sauer, 2018). This variable ratio reinforcement schedule is similar to that used in electronic gaming machines (EGMs) and is known to encourage rapid uptake and persistent repetitive behaviour in the hope of being rewarded (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Griffiths, 2018). Loot boxes in games use other psychological techniques drawn from gambling to induce spending and persistence, including continual availability, in-game promotions, auditory and visual cues, and the use of electronic money over multiple micro-transactions that make expenditure difficult to track (Hing, Russell, Browne, et al., 2021a; Larche, Chini, Lee, Dixon, & Fernandes, 2019; Parent Zone, 2019). Given that loot boxes are not regulated as gambling in most jurisdictions, they typically lack consumer protections (King & Delfabbro, 2019a), such as age restrictions, clear information on the odds of winning (Xiao, Henderson, Yuhan, & Newall, 2021), and safer gambling tools.

Of key concern is the link between loot box purchasing and problem gambling. All 16 studies reviewed in a recent scoping study corroborated this relationship (Montiel, Basterra-González, Machimbarrena, Ortega-Barón, & González-Cabrera, 2022). Two meta-analyses of 13 studies (Spicer et al., 2021) and 15 studies (Garea, Drummond, Sauer, Hall, & Williams, 2021) found significant small-to-moderate, but clinically relevant, positive correlations of 0.27 and 0.26, respectively, between loot box spending and problem gambling symptomatology. All studies to date have been cross-sectional and cannot identify causal directions between loot box purchasing and problem gambling or if there is a third variable explanation instead. Nevertheless, these findings suggest that either loot boxes are increasing problem gambling symptoms, or they disproportionately attract vulnerable players (Close et al., 2021; Garea et al., 2021; Zendle & Cairns, 2018, 2019). Experimental studies have examined the psychological effects of loot boxes, finding these are similar to the effects of gambling in triggering arousal, reward responses and urges to continue the activity that could lead to problematic use (Brady and Prentice, 2021; Larche et al., 2019). Further, survey data from 7,767 loot box purchasers showed that the top 5% of spenders generated half of loot box revenue (Close et al., 2021). Since higher loot box expenditure is associated with problem gambling, profits are disproportionately derived from higher-risk gamblers.

The association of loot box purchasing with problem gambling is particularly concerning in relation to adolescents. Most adolescents play video games (Brand, Jervis, Huggins, & Wilson, 2019) and purchasing loot boxes is a popular activity. In population studies in North America (DeCamp, 2021) and the UK (Gambling Commission, 2019), between one-sixth and one-quarter of adolescents reported purchasing loot boxes in the previous 12 months. Loot box purchasers are mostly male (Gambling Commission, 2019; Kristiansen & Severin, 2020) and the activity appears most popular amongst adolescents younger than 15 years (DeCamp, 2021; Hing, Russell, King, et al., 2021b). Adolescents report purchasing loot boxes mainly to gain in-game items, to obtain virtual in-game currency, or for in-game progress or competitive advantage (Hing, Russell, King, et al., 2021b; Rockloff et al., 2020). Other motivations include thrill and excitement, for prestige and appearance, to support game developers, to profit by selling or betting with loot box items, and because loot boxes may be perceived as good value (Zendle et al., 2019). Qualitative research is needed to gain a richer understanding of adolescents’ motivations for purchasing loot boxes, similar to a detailed analysis of adult motivations which identified seven themes: opening experience; value of box contents; game-related elements; social influences; emotive/impulsive influences; fear of missing out; triggers/facilitators (Nicklin et al., 2021).

Despite the popularity of purchasing loot boxes amongst youth, and the consistent link found between loot box purchasing and problem gambling in adults, only three studies have specifically examined this relationship in adolescents (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Rockloff et al., 2021; Zendle et al., 2019). In a representative sample of 1,137 Danes aged 12–16 years, loot box engagement was positively correlated with problem gambling severity when controlling for demographics, with a stronger relationship amongst those who had purchased or sold loot boxes (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020). In a panel sample of 911 Australians aged 12–17 years, loot box purchasing was similarly associated with gambling problems amongst both boys and girls when controlling for age (Rockloff et al., 2021). A survey of 1,155 older adolescents aged 16–18 years, recruited through online advertising, also found a moderate-to-large positive association between problem gambling and spending money on loot boxes, as well as past-month expenditure (Zendle et al., 2019).

Overall, the few studies conducted indicate a relationship between buying loot boxes and problem gambling amongst adolescents. However, only two studies have included adolescents younger than 16 years (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Rockloff et al., 2021), even though purchasing loot boxes appears to be most popular amongst younger adolescents (DeCamp, 2021; Hing, Russell, King, et al., 2021b). Further, these studies have not controlled for gambling on other activities, even though numerous studies have found that engaging in a wider range of gambling activities is one of the strongest predictors of problem gambling (Baggio, Gainsbury, Berchtold, & Iglesias, 2016; Gainsbury, Russell, Wood, Hing, & Blaszczynski, 2015; LaPlante, Nelson, & Gray, 2014; Philander & MacKay, 2014). Controlling for participation in other forms provides valuable insight into whether there is a direct impact of loot box play on problems, or whether the association is due to concomitant play on other forms. This approach was taken by Wardle and Zendle (2021) who analysed data from a weighted sample of 3,549 emerging adults aged 16–24 years in Great Britain. Those who purchased loot boxes were 11.4 times more likely to experience problem gambling than non-purchasers. Importantly, this relationship remained significant with odds of 4.5 after controlling for monetary gambling participation.

The current study uses a similar approach to Wardle and Zendle (2021) but in samples of Australian adolescents aged 12–17 years. The aim is to examine the association between loot box purchasing and gambling problems when controlling for socio-demographic factors and gambling participation. Based on the literature reviewed above, we hypothesise that loot box purchasing will predict problem and at-risk gambling when controlling for participation in monetary gambling. To our knowledge, this is the first adolescent-specific study to examine links between loot box purchasing and problem gambling in the context of adolescents’ monetary gambling activities.

Methods

Recruitment

Teenagers aged 12–17 years were recruited as the research focused on adolescents below the legal age of gambling (18 years in Australia). Participation required parental/guardian permission and the adolescent’s informed consent. Only residents of the Australian state of New South Wales (NSW) were eligible to participate as this was the funding agency’s jurisdiction.

To ensure robust numbers of participants in sub-populations of interest (problem and at-risk gamblers, loot box purchasers), we recruited two non-probability samples likely to have higher gambling involvement than general population samples. While representative samples are not essential to test relationships between variables (Russell, Browne, Hing, Rockloff, & Newall, 2021), we analysed all relationships separately in the two samples to enhance confidence in the results. The two samples were obtained using different recruitment methods to lessen the likelihood that results were an artefact of a particular recruitment method.

The first sample (Advertisements sample, N = 843) was recruited through email and online advertisements. Adults in NSW who had previously participated in studies by our research laboratory were emailed to invite any eligible adolescents in their household to complete the survey. Advertisements were also posted on Facebook, Instagram and Twitter, and in the funding agency’s online communications. To incentivise participation, respondents could enter a draw to win an AU$100 shopping voucher. The second sample (Qualtrics sample, N = 826) was recruited through Qualtrics, a commercial panel provider who sources participants from several panels and compensates participants based on each panel’s rewards system. Any duplicate responses within and between samples, identified through the email addresses and unique codes for follow-up that respondents provided, were deleted.

Procedure

Cognitive testing of the draft survey instrument was conducted with 12 adolescents aged 12–17 years who had engaged in simulated gambling, made microtransactions in video games, and/or seen gambling advertisements on social media. The interviewer encouraged participants to articulate their thinking process as they were answering the questions, with retrospective probes after each question to obtain further detail. Participants were also asked to highlight any confusion and uncertainty. Minor adjustments were made to some terminology (e.g., “money” was replaced with “real money”), question phrasing (to reduce length where possible), response options (e.g., adding a “don’t know” option) and survey flow, but standard scales remained unchanged. After being presented with a participant information sheet and informed consent preamble, participants completed a 15-min online survey between 16 April and 23 May 2020. The survey for both samples was deployed on the Qualtrics survey software platform.

Participants

The two samples gained 1,669 usable survey responses.

Advertisements sample

The Advertisements sample questionnaire was initiated by 1,473 participants. Nine did not give informed consent and 60 did not meet the age and location criteria. Of the remaining 1,404 participants, 561 did not complete the survey. There were no data integrity issues detected with the remaining responses, giving a total sample of 843 (60.0% completion rate). The Advertisements sample (30.6% female) had a mean age of 14 years. As expected, the Advertisements sample had higher rates of gambling problems than found in population studies: 31.0% were non-gamblers, 10.9% non-problem gamblers, 8.2% at-risk gamblers and 49.9% in the problem gambling category. Adolescents in the households of our previous gambling research participants were expected to have elevated rates of problem gambling, given the strong link between parental and youth gambling problems (Dowling et al., 2017; McComb & Sabiston, 2010).

Qualtrics sample

The Qualtrics sample questionnaire was initiated by 4,101 participants, but 2,364 did not indicate parental consent, 119 did not give their own consent, 520 did not meet the age and location criteria, and 42 failed data quality checks (completed the survey too rapidly or failed an attention check). Of the remaining 1,056 participants, 230 did not complete the survey, giving a total of 826 responses (78.2% completion rate). Checks were also performed to assess data quality, such as tests for straightlining or inappropriate responses, that would indicate a non-serious attempt. In this particular dataset, no such issues were found, and thus no further exclusions were made. The Qualtrics sample (44.8% female) had a mean age of 14 years. As expected, the Qualtrics sample had higher rates of gambling problems than found in population studies: 50.7% were non-gamblers, 24.6% non-problem gamblers, 9.2% at-risk gamblers and 15.5% in the problem gambling category.

Prior studies using online panels, like the Qualtrics sample, have also found elevated rates of problem gambling (Russell et al., 2021; Williams and Volberg, 2012).

Measures

Dependent variable

Past-year problematic gambling was assessed with the 9-item DSM-IV-MR-J which has been validated amongst youth (Fisher, 2000) and is the most widely used measure of youth problematic gambling in Australian and recent UK prevalence studies (Hing, Russell, King, et al., 2021b). Respondents who endorsed 4 or more items were classified as experiencing problem gambling; those with 2–3 endorsed items were classified as at-risk.

Independent variables

Loot box questions were preceded by the following explanation and pictures of 18 loot boxes found in popular video games: “Many video games offer loot boxes. Loot boxes are in-game items which can be purchased with real money, in-game currency, or awarded for free. When opened, loot boxes contain a random selection of virtual items (e.g., weapons, cosmetic items known as skins, or in-game currency). Some loot boxes are shown below. As you can see, they don’t always look like a box. They can also appear as chests, crates, caches, packs, cards, etc.” Participants were asked when, if ever, they had last: 1) opened a free loot box during a game, 2) paid real money to get a loot box or key, and 3) used virtual currency that was purchased with real money to get a loot box. In line with the Young People and Gambling Survey 2019 in the UK (Gambling Commission, 2019), response options were: “In the last 7 days”, “In the last 4 weeks”, “In the last 12 months”, “More than 12 months ago” and “Never”. For the current analyses, a composite variable was created to capture last-4-week loot box purchasing by combining response options 2 and 3 for “In the last 7 days” and “In the last 4 weeks”. This past-month timeframe was used to focus the analyses on recent loot box purchasers and follows the procedure of Wardle and Zendle (2021) in collapsing categories in this manner.

Participation in monetary gambling was asked for 11 activities (Table 1). Response options were: “In the last 7 days,” “In the last 4 weeks,” “In the last 12 months,” “More than 12 months ago,” and “Never”, based on response options used by the Gambling Commission (2019). For each activity, only last-4-week gambling was included in the analyses which combined the first two response options into a composite variable for each activity. A further composite variable was created that summed the number of past-month gambling activities of each respondent. This past-month timeframe was used to focus the analyses on recent gamblers, and again follows the procedure of Wardle and Zendle (2021) in collapsing categories in this manner.

Table 1.

Percentage of past 4-week loot box purchasers (LBP) who had gambled on each monetary form with the last 4 weeks AND biserial correlations^ of past 4-week loot box purchasing (y/n) with past 4-week gambling participation on each form (y/n)

Advertisements, n = 389 Qualtrics, n = 143
Count Percent r LBP, form^ P Count Percent r LBP, form^ P
Lottery games 161 41.4 0.095 0.02* 75 52.4% 0.218 >0.01**
Pokies# 216 55.5 0.108 >0.01** 41 28.7 0.230 >0.01**
Race betting 37 9.5 0.160 >0.01** 43 30.1 0.193 >0.01**
Keno 10 2.6 −0.055 0.18 37 25.9 0.238 >0.01**
Sport betting 14 3.6 0.027 0.52 36 25.2 0.173 >0.01**
Casino games 13 3.3 0.069 0.10 26 18.2 0.207 >0.01**
Bingo 157 40.4 0.070 0.09 48 33.6 0.198 >0.01**
Poker 15 3.9 0.033 0.43 30 21.0 0.210 >0.01**
Esports betting 216 55.5 0.157 >0.01** 50 35.0 0.278 >0.01**
Fantasy sports betting 227 58.4 0.145 >0.01** 39 27.3 0.215 >0.01**
Private betting 245 63.0 0.165 >0.01** 54 37.8 0.142 >0.01**

^ includes adolescents who gambled on any form within the last 12 months: Advertisements n = 582, Qualtrics n = 407. # “Pokies” is a commonly used name in Australia for electronic gaming machines. **significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Demographic characteristics included age (in years) and gender (male, female, other).

Analyses

By virtue of the substantial differences in recruitment methods between the Advertisements and Qualtrics samples, all analyses were conducted separately for the two sources. Analyses were restricted to the subset of persons who gambled at least once in the past 12 months, since non-gamblers were necessarily not at risk for problem gambling. From the original 1,669 responses, 989 had gambled within the prior 12 months (59.3%).

The analyses shown in Table 1 used biserial correlation to examine the relationships between last-4-week loot box purchasing (0 = no, 1 = yes) and last-4-week participation in 11 forms of gambling drawn from the NSW Gambling Survey 2019 (0 = no, 1 = yes) (Browne et al., 2019). These analyses demonstrate the concordance between loot box purchasing and gambling participation. The principal purpose of the study was to examine the relationship between loot box purchasing and gambling problems, while controlling for participation in other forms of gambling. Correlation matrices, in Table 2, computed the univariate relationships between the dependent variable, gambling problems, and last-4-week loot box purchasing as well as covariates (age and gender). Multinomial regressions predicting at-risk gambling and problem gambling (DSM-IV-MR-J) status were computed in Table 3 based on past-4-week loot box purchasing as the sole predictor for the purposes of comparison with the results including covariates. Another set of multinomial regressions, again predicting at-risk and problem gambling, was computed for Tables 4 and 5 with the addition of covariates including number of gambling forms engaged in during the last 4 weeks, age and gender, for the Advertisements sample and Qualtrics sample, respectively.

Table 2.

Correlation Matrices

Panel A: Advertisements sample
Problem gambling (DSM-IV-MR-J) Paid for LB last 4 weeks Monetary gambling last 4 weeks Age (years) Gender (male)
Problem gambling (DSM-IV-MR-J)
Paid for LB last 4 weeks 0.23**
Number of gambling forms last 4 weeks 0.45** 0.21**
Age (years) −0.02 −0.04 0.05
Gender (m = 1, f = 0) 0.02 0.07 −0.01 −0.09*
Panel B: Qualtrics sample
Problem gambling (DSM-IV-MR-J) Paid for LB last 4 weeks Monetary gambling last 4 weeks Age (years) Gender (male)
Gambling problems (DSM-IV-MR-J)
Paid for LB last 4 weeks 0.39**
Number of gambling forms last 4 weeks 0.47** 0.35**
Age (years) 0.10* −0.05 0.13*
Gender (m = 1, f = 0) 0.09 0.09 −0.12* −0.09

**significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

**significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Table 3.

Multinomial logistic regression with loot box purchasing sole predictor of at-risk and problem gambling

Panel A: Advertisements sample
At-risk gambling Problem gambling
B SE OR Wald st B SE OR Wald st
(Intercept) −1.326*** 0.273 23.606 0.560*** 0.157 12.755
Paid for LB in last 4 weeks 1.945*** 0.360 6.992 29.228 1.842*** 0.252 6.306 53.401
Panel B: Qualtrics sample
At-risk gambling Problem gambling
B SE OR Wald st B SE OR Wald st
(Intercept) −1.280*** 0.165 60.226 −1.259*** 0.164 59.226
Paid for LB in last 4 weeks 1.121*** 0.302 3.067 13.788 2.114*** 0.262 8.284 65.109

Non problem gambling is the reference category for the model. ***significant at the P < 0.001 level.

Non problem gambling is the reference category for the model. ***significant at the P < 0.001 level (2-tailed).

Table 4.

Multinomial logistic regression for the Advertisements sample (all predictors of at-risk and problem gambling)

At-risk gambling^ Problem gambling^
B SE OR Wald st B SE OR Wald st
(Intercept) −5.271** 2.001 6.938 −2.717 1.719 2.497
Paid for LB in last 4 weeks (y/n) 1.452** 0.465 4.270 9.761 1.315** 0.399 3.725 10.882
Number of gambling forms last 4 weeks 1.819*** 0.216 6.164 70.582 2.010*** 0.205 7.465 96.413
Age (years) 0.066 0.129 1.068 0.263 −0.021 0.112 0.979 0.035
Gender (m = 1, f = 0) 0.167 0.487 1.182 0.122 0.154 0.425 1.167 0.132

^Non problem gambling is the reference category for the model. ***significant at the 0.001 level. **significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

Table 5.

Multinomial logistic regression for Qualtrics sample (all predictors of at-risk and problem gambling)

At-risk gambling^ Problem gambling^
B SE OR Wald st B SE OR Wald st
(Intercept) −3.657* 1.506 5.894 −5.853*** 1.592 13.514
Paid for LB in last 4 weeks 1.015** 0.313 2.761 10.499 1.792*** 0.296 6.000 36.732
Number of gambling forms last 4 weeks 0.151 0.078 1.163 3.758 0.439*** 0.068 1.551 41.610
Age (years) 0.121 0.096 1.129 1.573 0.231* 0.100 1.259 5.315
Gender (m = 1, f = 0) 0.632* 0.291 1.882 4.736 0.434 0.287 1.543 2.277

^Non problem gambling is the reference category for the model. ***significant at the 0.001 level. **significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed). *significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Institutional Review Board of Central Queensland University approved the study. All subjects were informed about the study, and all provided informed consent. Parental consent was sought for those younger than 18 years of age.

Results

Descriptive results for monetary gambling and gambling problems

Table 1 illustrates the percentage of past 4-week loot box purchasers (LBP) who gambled on each of 11 forms of monetary gambling also within the four weeks prior to survey administration. The results are split by sample: Advertisements and Qualtrics. In addition, Table 1 shows the biserial correlations between past 4-week LBP purchasing and past 4-week gambling participation on each form. In the Advertisements sample, past 4-week LBPs were more likely to have gambled in the last four weeks on lottery games, pokies (electronic gaming machines), races, esports, fantasy sports and private betting. In the Qualtrics sample, past 4-week LBPs were more likely to have gambled on all 11 forms.

Correlations

Shown in Table 2 are rank-order correlations between DSM-IV-MR-J problem gambling (scored 0 = not-PG, 1 = PG) and the predictor variables of loot box purchasing in last four weeks (0 = no, 1 = yes), total number of the 11 types of gambling engaged in within the last four weeks (detailed in Table 1), age and gender (m/f – “other” not analysed as there were too few respondents). Results are shown separately for the Advertisements and Qualtrics samples in Panels A and B, respectively. In the Advertisements sample, neither age nor gender were correlated with problem gambling as expected, although all other predictors were correlated with problem gambling. Only gender failed to correlate significantly with problem gambling within the Qualtrics sample. The failure of age (in the Advertisement sample) and gender (in both samples) to correlate with gambling problems may result from low power and a restricted range. All participants were young (i.e., 12–17 years old) and most were male. For both samples, loot box purchasing within the last four weeks correlated with a calculated total of monetary forms of gambling in the same period. Lastly, there were marginally younger-aged males relative to females in the Advertisements sample.

Loot box purchasing as a sole predictor of gambling problems

Multinomial logistic regressions were performed to predict both at-risk gambling and problem gambling relating to those who purchased loot boxes within the last four weeks. For the Advertisements sample, results are shown in Table 3 Panel A. The odds-ratios (OR) indicated that people who purchased loot boxes recently were 6.99 times as likely to be at-risk gamblers, and 6.31 times as likely to score as problem gamblers as opposed to gamblers without problems.

Results for the Qualtrics sample are shown in Table 3 Panel B. The odds-ratio (OR) revealed that people who recently purchased loot boxes were 3.07 times as likely to be at-risk gamblers, and 8.28 times as likely to score as problem gamblers as opposed to gamblers without problems. As can be determined from Table 3, the implied CIs around the beta weights show that there was no significant difference between these two OR ratios.

All predictors of at-risk and problem gambling

Multinomial regressions were calculated to predict at-risk and problem gambling from loot box purchasing within the last four weeks along with additional covariate predictors including a count of the number of 11 different monetary gambling activities undertaken within the last four weeks (detailed in Table 1), age and gender.

As shown in Table 4 for the analyses of the Advertisements sample, the odds ratio revealed that loot box purchases within the last four weeks were still predictive of at-risk gambling, P < 0.01 (OR = 4.27). Likewise, the odds ratio revealed that loot box purchases within the last four weeks was also predictive of gambling problems, P < 0.01 (OR = 3.73). Consistent with expectations, a count of the number of 11 real money forms of gambling participation was predictive of both at-risk (OR = 6.16) and problem gambling (OR = 7.47). Lastly, however, neither age nor gender were predictive of at-risk or problem gambling in the Advertisements sample, P > 0.05, ns.

Table 5 shows the results of the multinomial logistic regression calculations for all predictors in the Qualtrics sample. Consistent with our hypotheses, loot box purchases in the last four weeks were still predictive of both at-risk (OR = 2.76) and problem gambling (OR = 6.00) in the presence of the other predictor variables. Moreover, both a count of the number of 11 gambling forms participated in within the last four weeks predicted problem gambling (OR = 1.55). Gender (male) predicted at-risk gambling (OR = 1.88) although not (significantly) problem gambling. Older ages predicted problem gambling (1.26 odds for each 1+ year older), although not (significantly) at-risk gambling.

Discussion

This study examined relationships between loot box purchasing, monetary gambling participation, and at-risk and problem gambling in two samples of adolescents. Consistent with previous research (Wardle & Zendle, 2021), recent loot box purchasing was significantly and positively associated with recent monetary gambling in the two samples, both for the total number of gambling forms as well as for most individual forms. These included pokies, race betting, lottery-type games, poker, sports betting, esports betting, fantasy sports betting and private gambling. These results indicate a convergence of behaviour between loot box purchasing and monetary gambling amongst adolescents but cannot clarify any causal directions. However, these results do indicate that a subset of adolescents is drawn to engaging in both activities. This is not surprising given the similarities between loot box purchasing and gambling that include wagering money on the chance to win a prize whose value is unknown in advance (Greer et al., 2019; Zendle et al., 2019, 2020a, 2020b).

As predicted and found in previous research with adolescents (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Rockloff et al., 2021; Zendle et al., 2019), recent loot box purchasing was significantly related to both at-risk and problem gambling in bivariate analyses for both samples. When including other predictors, such as age, gender and, most critically, recent (i.e., past-4-week) participation in monetary gambling forms, loot box purchasing was still associated with at-risk and problem gambling in both samples. As expected, the addition of other predictors attenuated the predictive value of recent loot box purchasing in relation to at-risk and problem gambling. The odds-ratios, nevertheless, were still in the predicted direction and remained significant. When controlling for monetary gambling, age, and gender, recent loot box purchasing increased the odds of problem gambling 3.7 to 6.0 times, and at-risk gambling 2.8 to 4.3 times. These odds are similar to those found in Wardle and Zendle’s (2021) study of emerging adults, where loot box purchasing increased the odds of problem gambling 4.5 times when controlling for monetary gambling participation.

One interpretation of the current results is that loot box purchasing has a direct causal effect on risk for problem gambling symptoms in adolescents. Loot boxes operate with gambling-like mechanics where the award of prizes based on a variable reinforcement schedule may increase chasing and persistence (Drummond & Sauer, 2018; Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Griffiths, 2018). Opportunities for repetitive continuous play, auditory and visual reinforcement, and the difficulties in keeping track of multiple electronic microtransactions, may also increase impaired control over loot box spending (Hing, Russell, Browne, et al., 2021; Larche et al., 2019; Parent Zone, 2019). Experimental studies have found that loot box purchasing triggers arousal, reward responses and urges to persist (Brady and Prentice, 2021; Larche et al., 2019). Several researchers have argued that loot box purchasing should be categorised as a type of gambling, and that its structural characteristics and psychological effects increase the risk of problem gambling (Drummond et al., 2018, 2020; Rockloff et al., 2020; Zendle et al., 2019, 2020a, 2020b).

An alternative explanation for the results is that young people with gambling problems are disproportionately attracted to purchasing loot boxes. By controlling for the number of other forms played, this indirect association is rendered less plausible, but is not eliminated. Individuals experiencing problem gambling tend to gamble on a wider range of activities (Binde, Romild, & Volberg, 2017; LaPlante et al., 2014). The opportunity to purchase loot boxes presents an additional gambling-like activity that can be undertaken by these more involved gamblers. Future research could examine whether there is a third variable explanation for the convergence of loot box purchasing and problem gambling symptomatology. Interestingly, Wardle and Zendle (2021) found that impulsivity attenuated but did not negate the significance of this relationship in emerging adults. However, other factors, such as risk-taking propensity, mental health problems and family functioning, may be worth examining as potential third variable explanations given their association with youth problem gambling (Volberg, Gupta, Griffiths, Ólason, & Delfabbro, 2011).

Regardless of any causality or a third variable explanation, the results indicate that loot boxes disproportionately attract spending from vulnerable adolescents experiencing gambling problems. Opportunities to purchase loot boxes in games are therefore adding to the financial stress that gambling is already causing them. Like gambling, the gaming industry relies on vulnerable players for much of its revenue (Close et al., 2021). This is even more concerning when these players are minors who lack the cognitive maturity to assess risk, are susceptible to poor and impulsive decision-making, and have heightened sensitivity to marketing and peer pressure (Emond & Griffiths, 2020; King, Delfabbro, Kaptsis, & Zwaans, 2014; Volberg et al., 2011). Moreover, there was no correlation between purchasing loot boxes in the last 4 weeks and age, indicating that younger adolescents may be no less likely to engage in this activity. Previous studies have found higher annual prevalence of loot box purchasing amongst adult gamers compared to adolescent gamers, but the reverse finding in general population studies (Montiel et al., 2022). Our results suggest that adolescents aged 12–17 years may be similar in their level of loot box engagement.

Conclusions

This study is the first to our knowledge to examine the relationship between loot box purchasing and gambling problems amongst adolescents, when controlling for monetary gambling participation. While limited by self-report data which may be subject to recall and social desirability biases, and its non-probability samples, the consistency of results for both samples strengthens the credibility of the findings. The main finding, that loot box purchasing independently predicts problem gambling and at-risk gambling amongst young people, supports the need for consumer protection tools in games with loot boxes. Noticeably absent from these games are the harm minimisation measures typically provided for online gambling (King & Delfabbro, 2019b; Drummond, Sauer, & Hall, 2019). These include limit-setting options, expenditure statements, clear information about the odds of winning, safer play messages, self-exclusion and time-out options, and links to help services. These measures could be required of digital games that allow loot box purchases. Public education targeting children, adolescents and parents could also raise awareness of the risk of harm from loot box purchasing. In the absence of these consumer protection measures, loot boxes can be considered potentially more harmful than online gambling, especially given they can be legally accessed by minors within the digital games that many young people play. Further, the consistent association between loot box purchasing and gambling problems found in this and other adolescent research (Kristiansen & Severin, 2020; Rockloff et al., 2021; Zendle et al., 2019) supports the desirability of restricting the sale of these games to those of legal gambling age.

Funding sources

Funding for this study was provided by the NSW Government’s Responsible Gambling Fund, with support from the NSW Office of Responsible Gambling.

Authors’ contribution

NH, AR, MR, MB, PN, NG and DK designed the study and the survey instrument. AR cleaned the data and computed scales. MR conducted the statistical analyses and compiled the results tables. NH completed the first draft of the manuscript. All authors refined and approved the submitted version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest in relation to this manuscript.

References

  • Baggio, S. , Gainsbury, S. M. , Berchtold, A. , & Iglesias, K. (2016). Co-morbidity of gambling and Internet use among Internet and land-based gamblers: Classic and network approaches. International Gambling Studies, 16(3), 500517. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2016.1242148.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Binde, P. , Romild, U. , & Volberg, R. A. (2017). Forms of gambling, gambling involvement and problem gambling: Evidence from a Swedish population survey. International Gambling Studies, 17(3), 490507. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2017.1360928.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brady, A. , & Prentice, G. (2021). Are loot boxes addictive? Analyzing participant’s physiological arousal while opening a loot box. Games and Culture, 16(4), 419433. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412019895359.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, J. E. , Jervis, J. , Huggins, P. M. , & Wilson, T. W. (2019). Digital Australia 2020. Everleigh: Interactive Games and Entertainment Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M. , Rockloff, M. , Hing, N. , Russell, A. , Boyle, C. M. , Rawat, V. , & Sproston, K. (2019). NSW gambling survey, 2019. NSW Office of responsible gambling. https://www.responsiblegambling.nsw.gov.au/research2/nsw-gambling-survey-2019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Close, J. , Spicer, S. G. , Nicklin, L. L. , Uther, M. , Lloyd, J. , & Lloyd, H. (2021). Secondary analysis of loot box data: Are high-spending “whales” wealthy gamers or problem gamblers? Addictive Behaviors, 117, 106851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.106851.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DeCamp, W. (2021). Loot boxes and gambling: Similarities and dissimilarities in risk and protective factors. Journal of Gambling Studies, 37, 189201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-020-09957-y.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N. A. , Shandley, K. A. , Oldenhof, E. , Affleck, J. M. , Youssef, G. J. , Frydenberg, E. , & Jackson, A. C. (2017). The intergenerational transmission of at‐risk/problem gambling: The moderating role of parenting practices. The American Journal on Addictions, 26(7), 707712. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.12599.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , & Sauer, J. D. (2018). Video game loot boxes are psychologically akin to gambling. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(8), 530532. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0360-1.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , & Hall, L. C. (2019). Loot box limit‐setting: A potential policy to protect video game users with gambling problems? Addiction, 114, 934935. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14583.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , Hall, L. C. , Zendle, D. , & Loudon, M. R. (2020). Why loot boxes could be regulated as gambling. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(10), 986988. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0900-3.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Emond, A. M. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Gambling in children and adolescents. British Medical Bulletin, 136(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10.1093/bmb/ldaa027.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferster, C. B. , & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

  • Fisher, S. (2000). Developing the DSM-IV-DSM-IV criteria to identify adolescent problem gambling in non-clinical populations. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2), 253273. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1009437115789.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gainsbury, S. M. , Russell, A. , Wood, R. , Hing, N. , & Blaszczynski, A. (2015). How risky is Internet gambling? A comparison of subgroups of Internet gamblers based on problem gambling status. New Media & Society, 17(6), 861879. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813518185.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gambling Commission . (2019). Young people and gambling survey 2019: A research study among 11–16 year olds in Great Britain. London: Great Britain.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Garea, S. S. , Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , Hall, L. C. , & Williams, M. N. (2021). Meta-analysis of the relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming and loot box spending. International Gambling Studies, 120. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2021.1914705.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Greer, N. , Rockloff, M. , Browne, M. , Hing, N. , & King, D. L. (2019). Esports betting and skin gambling: A brief history. Journal of Gambling Issues, 43, 128146. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2019.43.8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2018). Is the buying of loot boxes in video games a form of gambling or gaming? Gaming Law Review, 22(1), 5254. https://doi.org/10.1089/glr2.2018.2216.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hing, N. , Russell, A. M. T. , Browne, M. , Rockloff, M. , Greer, N. , Rawat, V. , … Woo, L. (2021a). The second national study of interactive gambling in Australia (2019–20). Melbourne: Gambling Research Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hing, N. , Russell, A. M. T. , King, D. , Rockloff, M. , Browne, M. , Greer, N. , … Coughlin, S. (2021b). NSW youth gambling study 2020. Sydney: NSW Responsible Gambling Fund.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. & Delfabbro, P. H. (2019a). Video game monetization (e.g., ‘loot boxes’): A blueprint for practical social responsibility measures. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17, 166179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-0009-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. & Delfabbro, P. H. (2019b). Loot box limit-setting is not sufficient on its own to prevent players from overspending: A reply to Drummond, Sauer, and Hall. Addiction, 114, 13241325. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14628.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. , Delfabbro, P. H. , Kaptsis, D. , & Zwaans, T. (2014). Adolescent simulated gambling via digital and social media: An emerging problem. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 305313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.048.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kristiansen, S. , & Severin, M. C. (2020). Loot box engagement and problem gambling among adolescent gamers: Findings from a national survey. Addictive Behaviors, 103, 106254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106254.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LaPlante, D. A. , Nelson, S. E. , & Gray, H. M. (2014). Breadth and depth involvement: Understanding Internet gambling involvement and its relationship to gambling problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 396403. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033810.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Larche, C. J. , Chini, K. , Lee, C. , Dixon, M. J. , & Fernandes, M. (2019). Rare loot box rewards trigger larger arousal and reward responses, and greater urge to open more loot boxes. Journal of Gambling Studies, 123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09913-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McComb, J. L. , & Sabiston, C. M. (2010). Family influences on adolescent gambling behavior: A review of the literature. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26, 503520. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9181-5.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Montiel, I. , Basterra-González, A. , Machimbarrena, J. M. , Ortega-Barón, J. , & González-Cabrera, J. (2022). Loot box engagement: A scoping review of primary studies on prevalence and association with problematic gaming and gambling. Plos One, 17(1), e0263177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263177.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nicklin, L. L. , Spicer, S. G. , Close, J. , Parke, J. , Smith, O. , Raymen, T. , & Lloyd, J. (2021). "It’s the attraction of winning that draws you in”—A qualitative investigation of reasons and facilitators for videogame loot box engagement in UK gamers. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(10), 2103. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10102103.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parent Zone. (2019). The rip-off games: How the new business model of online gaming exploits children . Retrieved from: https://parentzone.org.uk/system/files/attachments/The%20Ripoff%20Games%20-%20Parent%20Zone%20report.pdf

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Philander, K. S. , & MacKay, T. L. (2014). Online gambling participation and problem gambling severity: Is there a causal relationship? International Gambling Studies, 14(2), 214227. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2014.893585.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. , Russell, A. M. , Greer, N. , Lole, L. , Hing, N. , & Browne, M. (2021). Young people who purchase loot boxes are more likely to have gambling problems: An online survey of adolescents and young adults living in NSW Australia. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(1), 3541. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. , Russell, A. M. T. , Greer, N. , Lolé, L. , Hing, N. , & Browne. M. (2020). Loot boxes: Are they grooming youth for gambling? Sydney: NSW Responsible Gambling Fund.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russell, A. M. T. , Browne, M. , Hing, N. , Rockloff, M. , & Newall, P. (2021). Are any samples representative or unbiased? Reply to Pickering and Blaszczynski. International Gambling Studies, 112. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wpdfy.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spicer, S. G. , Nicklin, L. L. , Uther, M. , Lloyd, J. , Lloyd, H. , & Close, J. (2021). Loot boxes, problem gambling and problem video gaming: A systematic review and meta-synthesis. New Media & Society. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448211027175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volberg, R. A. , Gupta, R. , Griffiths, M. D. , Ólason, D. T. , & Delfabbro, P. (2011). 3. An international perspective on youth gambling prevalence studies. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 22, 338. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110255690.21.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wardle, H. , & Zendle, D. (2021). Loot boxes, gambling, and problem gambling among young people: Results from a cross-sectional online survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 24(4), 267274. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2020.0299.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2012). Population assessment of problem gambling: Utility and best practices. Report prepared for the Ontario problem gambling research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of health and Long Term Care. Retrieved from https://opus.uleth.ca/handle/10133/3094.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xiao, L. Y. , Henderson, L. L. , Yuhan, Y. , & Newall, P. W. S. (2021). Gaming the system: Sub-optimal compliance with loot box probability disclosure regulations in China. Behavioural Public Policy. https://doi.org/10.1017/bpp.2021.23.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , & Cairns, P. (2018). Video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: Results of a large-scale survey. Plos One, 13(11), e0206767. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214167.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , & Cairns, P. (2019). Loot boxes are again linked to problem gambling: Results of a replication study. Plos One, 14(3), e0213194. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213194.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Cairns, P. , Barnett, H. , & McCall, C. (2020b). Paying for loot boxes is linked to problem gambling, regardless of specific features like cash-out and pay-to-win. Computers in Human Behavior, 102, 181191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Meyer, R. , Cairns, P. , Waters, S. , & Ballou, N. (2020a). The prevalence of loot boxes in mobile and desktop games. Addiction, 115(9), 17681772. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14973.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Meyer, R. , & Over, H. (2019). Adolescents and loot boxes: Links with problem gambling and motivations for purchase. Royal Society Open Science, 6(6), 190049. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.190049.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baggio, S. , Gainsbury, S. M. , Berchtold, A. , & Iglesias, K. (2016). Co-morbidity of gambling and Internet use among Internet and land-based gamblers: Classic and network approaches. International Gambling Studies, 16(3), 500517. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2016.1242148.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Binde, P. , Romild, U. , & Volberg, R. A. (2017). Forms of gambling, gambling involvement and problem gambling: Evidence from a Swedish population survey. International Gambling Studies, 17(3), 490507. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2017.1360928.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brady, A. , & Prentice, G. (2021). Are loot boxes addictive? Analyzing participant’s physiological arousal while opening a loot box. Games and Culture, 16(4), 419433. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412019895359.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, J. E. , Jervis, J. , Huggins, P. M. , & Wilson, T. W. (2019). Digital Australia 2020. Everleigh: Interactive Games and Entertainment Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Browne, M. , Rockloff, M. , Hing, N. , Russell, A. , Boyle, C. M. , Rawat, V. , & Sproston, K. (2019). NSW gambling survey, 2019. NSW Office of responsible gambling. https://www.responsiblegambling.nsw.gov.au/research2/nsw-gambling-survey-2019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Close, J. , Spicer, S. G. , Nicklin, L. L. , Uther, M. , Lloyd, J. , & Lloyd, H. (2021). Secondary analysis of loot box data: Are high-spending “whales” wealthy gamers or problem gamblers? Addictive Behaviors, 117, 106851. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.106851.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DeCamp, W. (2021). Loot boxes and gambling: Similarities and dissimilarities in risk and protective factors. Journal of Gambling Studies, 37, 189201. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-020-09957-y.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dowling, N. A. , Shandley, K. A. , Oldenhof, E. , Affleck, J. M. , Youssef, G. J. , Frydenberg, E. , & Jackson, A. C. (2017). The intergenerational transmission of at‐risk/problem gambling: The moderating role of parenting practices. The American Journal on Addictions, 26(7), 707712. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.12599.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , & Sauer, J. D. (2018). Video game loot boxes are psychologically akin to gambling. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(8), 530532. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0360-1.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , & Hall, L. C. (2019). Loot box limit‐setting: A potential policy to protect video game users with gambling problems? Addiction, 114, 934935. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14583.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , Hall, L. C. , Zendle, D. , & Loudon, M. R. (2020). Why loot boxes could be regulated as gambling. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(10), 986988. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0900-3.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Emond, A. M. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Gambling in children and adolescents. British Medical Bulletin, 136(1), 2129. https://doi.org/10.1093/bmb/ldaa027.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferster, C. B. , & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

  • Fisher, S. (2000). Developing the DSM-IV-DSM-IV criteria to identify adolescent problem gambling in non-clinical populations. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2), 253273. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1009437115789.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gainsbury, S. M. , Russell, A. , Wood, R. , Hing, N. , & Blaszczynski, A. (2015). How risky is Internet gambling? A comparison of subgroups of Internet gamblers based on problem gambling status. New Media & Society, 17(6), 861879. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444813518185.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gambling Commission . (2019). Young people and gambling survey 2019: A research study among 11–16 year olds in Great Britain. London: Great Britain.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Garea, S. S. , Drummond, A. , Sauer, J. D. , Hall, L. C. , & Williams, M. N. (2021). Meta-analysis of the relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming and loot box spending. International Gambling Studies, 120. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2021.1914705.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Greer, N. , Rockloff, M. , Browne, M. , Hing, N. , & King, D. L. (2019). Esports betting and skin gambling: A brief history. Journal of Gambling Issues, 43, 128146. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2019.43.8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2018). Is the buying of loot boxes in video games a form of gambling or gaming? Gaming Law Review, 22(1), 5254. https://doi.org/10.1089/glr2.2018.2216.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hing, N. , Russell, A. M. T. , Browne, M. , Rockloff, M. , Greer, N. , Rawat, V. , … Woo, L. (2021a). The second national study of interactive gambling in Australia (2019–20). Melbourne: Gambling Research Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hing, N. , Russell, A. M. T. , King, D. , Rockloff, M. , Browne, M. , Greer, N. , … Coughlin, S. (2021b). NSW youth gambling study 2020. Sydney: NSW Responsible Gambling Fund.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. & Delfabbro, P. H. (2019a). Video game monetization (e.g., ‘loot boxes’): A blueprint for practical social responsibility measures. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17, 166179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-0009-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. & Delfabbro, P. H. (2019b). Loot box limit-setting is not sufficient on its own to prevent players from overspending: A reply to Drummond, Sauer, and Hall. Addiction, 114, 13241325. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14628.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L. , Delfabbro, P. H. , Kaptsis, D. , & Zwaans, T. (2014). Adolescent simulated gambling via digital and social media: An emerging problem. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 305313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.048.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kristiansen, S. , & Severin, M. C. (2020). Loot box engagement and problem gambling among adolescent gamers: Findings from a national survey. Addictive Behaviors, 103, 106254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106254.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LaPlante, D. A. , Nelson, S. E. , & Gray, H. M. (2014). Breadth and depth involvement: Understanding Internet gambling involvement and its relationship to gambling problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 396403. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033810.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Larche, C. J. , Chini, K. , Lee, C. , Dixon, M. J. , & Fernandes, M. (2019). Rare loot box rewards trigger larger arousal and reward responses, and greater urge to open more loot boxes. Journal of Gambling Studies, 123. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-019-09913-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McComb, J. L. , & Sabiston, C. M. (2010). Family influences on adolescent gambling behavior: A review of the literature. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26, 503520. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9181-5.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Montiel, I. , Basterra-González, A. , Machimbarrena, J. M. , Ortega-Barón, J. , & González-Cabrera, J. (2022). Loot box engagement: A scoping review of primary studies on prevalence and association with problematic gaming and gambling. Plos One, 17(1), e0263177. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263177.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nicklin, L. L. , Spicer, S. G. , Close, J. , Parke, J. , Smith, O. , Raymen, T. , & Lloyd, J. (2021). "It’s the attraction of winning that draws you in”—A qualitative investigation of reasons and facilitators for videogame loot box engagement in UK gamers. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(10), 2103. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10102103.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parent Zone. (2019). The rip-off games: How the new business model of online gaming exploits children . Retrieved from: https://parentzone.org.uk/system/files/attachments/The%20Ripoff%20Games%20-%20Parent%20Zone%20report.pdf

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Philander, K. S. , & MacKay, T. L. (2014). Online gambling participation and problem gambling severity: Is there a causal relationship? International Gambling Studies, 14(2), 214227. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2014.893585.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. , Russell, A. M. , Greer, N. , Lole, L. , Hing, N. , & Browne, M. (2021). Young people who purchase loot boxes are more likely to have gambling problems: An online survey of adolescents and young adults living in NSW Australia. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(1), 3541. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rockloff, M. , Russell, A. M. T. , Greer, N. , Lolé, L. , Hing, N. , & Browne. M. (2020). Loot boxes: Are they grooming youth for gambling? Sydney: NSW Responsible Gambling Fund.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russell, A. M. T. , Browne, M. , Hing, N. , Rockloff, M. , & Newall, P. (2021). Are any samples representative or unbiased? Reply to Pickering and Blaszczynski. International Gambling Studies, 112. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wpdfy.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spicer, S. G. , Nicklin, L. L. , Uther, M. , Lloyd, J. , Lloyd, H. , & Close, J. (2021). Loot boxes, problem gambling and problem video gaming: A systematic review and meta-synthesis. New Media & Society. https://doi.org/10.1177/14614448211027175.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volberg, R. A. , Gupta, R. , Griffiths, M. D. , Ólason, D. T. , & Delfabbro, P. (2011). 3. An international perspective on youth gambling prevalence studies. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 22, 338. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110255690.21.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wardle, H. , & Zendle, D. (2021). Loot boxes, gambling, and problem gambling among young people: Results from a cross-sectional online survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 24(4), 267274. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2020.0299.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2012). Population assessment of problem gambling: Utility and best practices. Report prepared for the Ontario problem gambling research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of health and Long Term Care. Retrieved from https://opus.uleth.ca/handle/10133/3094.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xiao, L. Y. , Henderson, L. L. , Yuhan, Y. , & Newall, P. W. S. (2021). Gaming the system: Sub-optimal compliance with loot box probability disclosure regulations in China. Behavioural Public Policy. https://doi.org/10.1017/bpp.2021.23.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , & Cairns, P. (2018). Video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: Results of a large-scale survey. Plos One, 13(11), e0206767. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214167.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , & Cairns, P. (2019). Loot boxes are again linked to problem gambling: Results of a replication study. Plos One, 14(3), e0213194. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0213194.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Cairns, P. , Barnett, H. , & McCall, C. (2020b). Paying for loot boxes is linked to problem gambling, regardless of specific features like cash-out and pay-to-win. Computers in Human Behavior, 102, 181191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Meyer, R. , Cairns, P. , Waters, S. , & Ballou, N. (2020a). The prevalence of loot boxes in mobile and desktop games. Addiction, 115(9), 17681772. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.14973.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zendle, D. , Meyer, R. , & Over, H. (2019). Adolescents and loot boxes: Links with problem gambling and motivations for purchase. Royal Society Open Science, 6(6), 190049. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.190049.

    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Aug 2024 0 120 59
Sep 2024 0 173 80
Oct 2024 0 446 102
Nov 2024 0 427 131
Dec 2024 0 519 76
Jan 2025 0 244 69
Feb 2025 0 48 30