Authors:
Tim van Timmeren Department of Social Health and Organizational Psychology, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
ABC Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1001 NK, The Netherlands
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Tim van Timmeren in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0282-8269
,
Ruth J. van Holst ABC Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1001 NK, The Netherlands
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Ruth J. van Holst in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-1184-9355
, and
Anna E. Goudriaan ABC Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, 1001 NK, The Netherlands
Department of Psychiatry, Amsterdam Institute for Addiction Research, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam UMC, University of Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Arkin Mental Health, The Netherlands

Search for other papers by Anna E. Goudriaan in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8670-9384
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Dysfunction of the striatum, a brain region part of the mesolimbic reward system, is a key characteristic of addictive disorders, but neuroimaging studies have reported conflicting findings. An integrative model of addiction points to the presence or absence of addiction-related cues as an explanation for hyper- or hypoactivation, respectively, of the striatum.

Methods

To test this model directly, we investigated striatal activation during monetary reward anticipation in the presence versus absence of addiction-related cues using functional MRI. Across two studies, we compared 46 alcohol use disorder (AUD) patients with 30 matched healthy controls; and 24 gambling disorder (GD) patients with 22 matched healthy controls.

Results

During monetary reward anticipation, hypoactivation of the reward system was seen in AUD individuals compared to HCs. Additionally, a behavioral interaction was seen where gambling cues made participants, across groups, respond faster for bigger, but slower for smaller rewards. However, no striatal differences were seen in response to addiction-related cues between AUD or GD patients and their matched controls. Finally, despite substantial individual differences in neural activity to cue-reactivity and reward anticipation, these measures did not correlate, suggesting that they contribute independently to addiction aetiology.

Discussion and Conclusions

Our findings replicate previous findings of blunted striatal activity during monetary reward anticipation in alcohol use disorder but do not support the idea that addiction-related cues explain striatal dysfunction as suggested by the model.

Abstract

Background and aims

Dysfunction of the striatum, a brain region part of the mesolimbic reward system, is a key characteristic of addictive disorders, but neuroimaging studies have reported conflicting findings. An integrative model of addiction points to the presence or absence of addiction-related cues as an explanation for hyper- or hypoactivation, respectively, of the striatum.

Methods

To test this model directly, we investigated striatal activation during monetary reward anticipation in the presence versus absence of addiction-related cues using functional MRI. Across two studies, we compared 46 alcohol use disorder (AUD) patients with 30 matched healthy controls; and 24 gambling disorder (GD) patients with 22 matched healthy controls.

Results

During monetary reward anticipation, hypoactivation of the reward system was seen in AUD individuals compared to HCs. Additionally, a behavioral interaction was seen where gambling cues made participants, across groups, respond faster for bigger, but slower for smaller rewards. However, no striatal differences were seen in response to addiction-related cues between AUD or GD patients and their matched controls. Finally, despite substantial individual differences in neural activity to cue-reactivity and reward anticipation, these measures did not correlate, suggesting that they contribute independently to addiction aetiology.

Discussion and Conclusions

Our findings replicate previous findings of blunted striatal activity during monetary reward anticipation in alcohol use disorder but do not support the idea that addiction-related cues explain striatal dysfunction as suggested by the model.

Introduction

Patients with addictive disorders often show disrupted striatal reward processing (Bjork, Smith, Chen, & Hommer, 2012; Blum et al., 2000; Volkow & Morales, 2015). Findings have been inconsistent, however, as both hypo- and hyperactivations have been reported (Clark, Boileau, & Zack, 2019; Leyton & Vezina, 2013; Limbrick-Oldfield, Van Holst, & Clark, 2013). Previous findings have been interpreted in the context of several addiction-theories, with largely incompatible predictions about the direction of the striatal disruption. A hypoactive reward system (and related anhedonia) has been described either as a predisposition for the development of addictive behaviors (Blum et al., 2000) or as a consequence of chronic drug use and receptor down-regulation (Goldstein & Volkow, 2011; Koob & Le Moal, 2008), possibly together with the recruitment of ‘anti-reward’ systems (Koob & Le Moal, 2005). Alternatively, a hyperactive reward system has been described either as a vulnerability factor reflecting increased sensitivity to high rewards driving impulsive behaviors (Bjork et al., 2012) or as a result of incentive sensitization for environmental stimuli that become conditioned with the rewarding effects of the addictive behavior (Robinson & Berridge, 2008).

Over the past decades, each of these theories has found scientific support, resulting in ample but inconsistent evidence for dysfunctions in the human reward system in addicted populations. It has been proposed that these seemingly contradictory findings may be integrated by considering the presence versus absence of addiction-related cues (Leyton & Vezina, 2013, 2014). Stimuli regularly associated with the addictive behavior become conditioned through repeated association with their rewarding effects – ultimately leading to sensitized neurobiological responses and craving (Vezina & Leyton, 2009). Hyperactive striatal motivational states thus develop in the presence of addiction-related cues, a phenomenon known as cue-reactivity. Simultaneously, a progressively diminished interest towards rewards unrelated to the addiction is reflected by a hypoactive reward system. Indeed, a review of the human substance use and gambling disorder literature suggests that many inconsistencies can be explained by factoring in the effects of addiction-related cues (Leyton & Vezina, 2013).

Among individuals suffering from addiction, contextual cues are known to have physiological effects (e.g. increased heart rate) and trigger craving (Carter & Tiffany, 1999), bias attention (Field, Munafò, & Franken, 2009), increase motivation (Leyton & Vezina, 2013) and modify automatic tendencies to respond for substances (Wiers et al., 2007). Cue-reactivity has frequently been investigated in functional neuroimaging studies to identify the neural substrates of craving in alcohol (Zeng et al., 2021), drug (Zilberman, Lavidor, Yadid, & Rassovsky, 2019) and behavioral addictions (Starcke, Antons, Trotzke, & Brand, 2018). Reward processing has been studied using the Monetary Incentive Delay Task [MIDT] (Knutson, Westdorp, Kaiser, & Hommer, 2000), with a meta-analysis across 25 studies providing evidence for consistent striatal hypoactivation during the anticipation of a monetary reward across studies in addicted populations (Luijten, Schellekens, Kühn, Machielse, & Sescousse, 2017). Several studies have previously manipulated the type of outcome to compare neural activity during the anticipation of addiction-relevant versus addiction-irrelevant outcomes. For example (Bühler et al., 2010), found that nicotine dependent individuals showed reduced striatal activation during the anticipation of both monetary (non-addiction-related) and cigarette (addiction-related) outcomes. Similarly, in gambling disorder, where money itself is an addiction-relevant outcome that can produce reinforcing effects similar to drug use (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2010), ventral striatal hypoactivity is seen during the anticipation of addiction-irrelevant outcomes (e.g. erotic pictures, Sescousse, Barbalat, Domenech, & Dreher, 2013); and addiction-relevant (i.e. monetary) outcomes (Fauth-Bühler et al., 2014; Luijten et al., 2017). Although it may seem counterintuitive that even addiction-relevant outcomes are associated with striatal hypoactivations, it's important to realize that abstract stimuli are used in the MIDT during the anticipation phase to signal various types of outcomes (see Clark et al. (2019) for a relevant discussion related to gambling). According to Leyton and Vezina's model, hyperactivity depends on the presence of addiction-relevant cues. Hence, striatal hyperactivity would only be expected when addiction-relevant cues are included in the anticipation phase. Such a study would not only be able to directly investigate the pervasiveness of ‘striatal ups and downs’, but would also allow for an evaluation of the interaction between addiction-related cues and monetary reward anticipation for which there is some evidence (Freeman, Morgan, Beesley, & Curran, 2012). Additionally, the proposed design could directly address whether striatal ‘ups’ in response to addiction-cues correlate with striatal ‘downs’ during the processing of natural rewards within addicted individuals. These could be independent (risk-)factors constituting different addiction-subtypes, or dependent factors that simultaneously develop with addiction, as suggested by previous work (Volkow, Koob, & McLellan, 2016; Wrase et al., 2007).

Here we assessed monetary reward anticipation in the presence and absence of addiction-related cues to directly address these open questions. We adapted the MIDT (Knutson et al., 2000) to include neutral and addiction-related background images during the anticipation phase with varying levels of rewards, resulting in a full-factorial design with the factors monetary reward-type (low and high) and cue-type (neutral and addiction-related). This enabled us to separately study (i) general (monetary) reward anticipation, (ii) processing of addiction-related cues and (iii) their interaction, in both addicted and healthy control groups. We conducted two studies: one in patients with alcohol use disorder [AUD] and one in patients with gambling disorder [GD]. Both groups were separately matched to healthy control participants [HCs] and tested during fMRI-scanning to assess striatal functioning during the MIDT task in the presence or absence of addiction-related cues. We hypothesized patient groups to show blunted striatal activity during monetary reward anticipation (i.e. striatal ‘downs’) in the absence of addiction-related cues, but increased motivation and neural reward-processing (i.e. striatal ‘ups’) in the presence of addiction-related cues compared to HCs.

Methods & materials

More details about the methods and results are provided in the Supplementary Material.

Participants

All patients received treatment and were recruited from a local addiction treatment centre. AUD patients were detoxified (>2 weeks) and recently diagnosed with AUD without Axis 1 comorbidity. GD patients were included if they were recently diagnosed with and started therapy for GD but were not obliged to abstain from gambling. Patient groups were recruited through advertisements and our subject-database. All subjects underwent the MINI structured psychiatric interview (Sheehan, Lecrubier, & Sheehan, 1998), to confirm the absence of psychiatric disorders (except for DSM-5 AUD/GD in the AUD/GD patient group, respectively). Participants were included after meeting the inclusion criteria (see Supplementary Methods) and were reimbursed with 50 euros plus additional task earning (∼20 euros).

Experimental procedure

After providing written consent, participants underwent ∼1 h of interviews, questionnaires, and cognitive tests. To facilitate craving effects, all fMRI sessions commenced in the afternoon (between 12:15 and 5:30 pm). These data were collected as part of a more extensive study protocol which included another fMRI task and a resting-state fMRI scan (total scanning duration was 90 min), data of which have been presented elsewhere (van Timmeren, Zhutovsky, van Holst, & Goudriaan, 2018, van Timmeren et al., 2020; van Timmeren, Piray, Goudriaan, & van Holst, 2023).

Experimental paradigm

To investigate the effects of reward anticipation, cue-reactivity, and their interaction, we adapted the MIDT to include addiction-related cues (alcohol- and gambling-relevant cues in the AUD-/GD-study, respectively). A total of 28 alcohol and neutral pictures were selected from The Geneva Appetitive Alcohol Pictures database (Billieux et al., 2011) supplemented by pictures from a previous study (Sjoerds, van den Brink, Beekman, Penninx, & Veltman, 2014) and pictures from the internet, and contained images of beer, wine, and spirits. The 28 gambling pictures were selected from a previous study (Goudriaan, de Ruiter, van den Brink, Oosterlaan, & Veltman, 2010), supplemented by pictures from the internet, and contained images of casinos and slot machines. Neutral pictures were matched (independently to the alcohol and gambling pictures) for setting, color-distribution, and complexity.

We used a 2 × 2 full-factorial design with reward magnitude (Big reward = €0.50 coin and Small reward = €0.01 coin) and cue-type (addiction-related and neutral background pictures) as factors, resulting in four conditions: ‘Big reward/Addiction cue’ [BA], ‘Big reward/Neutral cue’ [BN], ‘Small reward/Addiction cue’ [SA] ‘Small reward/Neutral cue’ [SN]. The primary dependent measure was fMRI BOLD response during the MIDT reward anticipation stage. The task comprised 28 trials per condition; trials were presented pseudo randomly and the total task duration was ∼23 min. Figure 1a shows the experimental design, more detailed information about the procedure is included in the Supplementary information.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Schematic overview of the experimental design and behavioral (reaction time) results. (a) Participants were instructed to respond to a target as quickly as possible in order to gain monetary rewards. During each trial, participants could earn 1 or 50 cents, indicated by a coin overlaid on an addiction-related or neutral background picture (‘cue’). Next, a crosshair was shown for a variable period and participants were instructed to respond as fast as possible to the target. Feedback about current and cumulative earnings was provided, followed by a fixation cross before a new trial started. (b) Mean reaction time (in ms) on the task for each condition, showing faster responses for big than small rewards in both studies. Moreover, in the GD study, a significant interaction was found between reward- and cue-type, such that responses were faster for bigger rewards and slower for smaller rewards in the presence of gambling cues, whereas this relation was reversed in small reward condition. Error bars indicate 95% confidence interval

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 12, 2; 10.1556/2006.2023.00015

Following fMRI acquisition, participants were asked to indicate how strongly each background-picture induced craving on a 7-point Likert scale (i.e. subjective craving). Technical failures resulted in missing craving data of six participants in the AUD study (three AUD patients and three HCs) and seven participants in the GD study (two GD patients and five HCs).

Magnetic resonance imaging

Acquisition

Participants entered the 3T Phillips MRI-scanner in head-first supine position and were able to view the screen using a mirror attached to the head-coil. We acquired 595 T2*-weighted multiecho planar functional MRI volumes (voxel-size: 3 mm3) for analysis. Additionally, a structural T1-weighted image (voxel-size: 1 mm3) was collected. See Supplementary Materials for details.

fMRI analysis

All functional MRI data were analysed using SPM12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, United Kingdom). Raw multiecho fMRI data were first combined into single volumes using the PAID-method (Poser, Versluis, Hoogduin, & Norris, 2006). Preprocessing of the fMRI data was identical to van Timmeren et al. (2018) and involved motion correction, slice-time correction, co-registration, normalization and smoothing (see Supplementary Materials for details). A first-level general linear model was constructed for each participant, including individual regressors for all four conditions during the anticipation phase, modeled as a 4 s box-car function at stimulus-onset. Because we did not add an interstimulus interval after the target, our design did not allow us to independently model the outcome phase, as often done in MIDT tasks to compare neural activity during wins versus losses. This was a deliberate decision, as we wanted optimal power to compare monetary reward and addiction-related neural activity, and there is no interaction with addiction-related cues in the outcome phase. Outcome (win/loss) and key presses were included as regressors of no interest. Realignment parameters were entered as six nuisance regressors and low frequency drifts were removed using a high-pass filter (128-s cutoff). Regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function.

Three first-level contrast images were constructed for each participant to assess the effect of (1) reward type (monetary reward anticipation = big reward versus small reward: [BA + BN]>[SA + SN]); (2) cue type (cue-reactivity = addiction-related cues versus neutral cues: [BA + SA]>[BN + SN]); and (3) their interaction ([BA + SN]>[BN + SA]). Three a priori striatal regions of interests were derived from the Oxford-Imanova Striatal Structural Atlas (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk): bilateral ventral striatum, caudate and putamen. For each participant, parameter estimates were extracted and averaged across voxels for the three ROIs separately to investigate regional activation for the relevant contrasts (i.e., monetary reward anticipation, cue-reactivity and their interaction effect). Additionally, single-subject contrast images were entered into second-level random-effects analysis, comparing within-group activation (one-sample t tests) and between-group differences (two-sample t tests). These whole brain analyses were additionally conducted to describe any non-striatal group differences for the monetary reward anticipation, cue-reactivity and their interaction. These results were corrected using cluster-defining threshold of p = 0.001 with whole-brain familywise error [FWE] set at p < 0.05 (pFWE < 0.05). Anatomical brain regions were identified using the Automated Anatomical Labelling [AAL] atlas in SPM12 (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002).

Exploratory analyses

Addiction severity, chronicity and abstinence

We additionally explored whether individual differences in striatal functioning in the clinical groups were associated with several clinical measures found to be relevant in previous studies: in both groups, we looked at craving levels, which have been related to increased ventral striatal cue-reactivity (Filbey et al., 2008) and blunted ventral striatal monetary reward processing (Wrase et al., 2007) in AUD, while in GD craving levels have been found to correlate with increased insular cue-reactivity (Goudriaan et al., 2010; Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2017). We also looked at duration of abstinence, which has previously been associated with striatal cue-reactivity, although positively in AUD (Li et al., 2014), but negatively in GD (Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2017). Finally, we looked at addiction severity and duration, which have been related to striatal cue-reactivity (Claus, Ewing, Filbey, Sabbineni, & Hutchison, 2011; Ihssen, Cox, Wiggett, Fadardi, & Linden, 2011; Sjoerds et al., 2014; Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010).

In the group of patients with AUD, the following outcomes were used: AUD severity (AUDIT, range 0–40; Saunders, Aasland, Babor, de la Fuente, & Grant, 1993), obsessive alcohol-related thoughts (OCDS, range 0–20; De Wildt et al., 2005) and lifetime alcohol intake (kg) (Skinner & Sheu, 1982). Within the GD group, GD severity was measured with the PGSI (Ferris & Wynne, 2001). In both groups, we looked at subjective craving (obtained by rating the pictures post-scanning, see Experimental procedure), duration of addiction problems (years) and abstinence (days). Pearson's correlations were done between these factors and the three striatal ROIs during monetary reward anticipation and cue-reactivity. Considering the multitude of tests (involving 3 ROIs, 2 events of interest and a total of 9 clinical factors), we refrain from making any statistical inferences (i.e. using p-values) and only report results for moderate to high correlations (Pearson's correlation coefficient >0.3). All per test in the supplementary data.

Relation between striatal ‘ups’ and ‘downs’

To test whether increased striatal cue-reactivity (striatal ‘ups’) and diminished striatal monetary reward anticipation (striatal ‘downs’) were related to each other within individual patients, Pearson correlations were performed (for both patient groups separately) using the extracted parameter estimates for each of the three ROIs. Evidence for the null hypothesis (i.e. no relation between the two factors) were substantiated by Bayes Factors using Bayesian correlations.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out using JASP, version 0.8.6 (JASP Team, 2018). Demographics and clinical data were analyzed for group differences with two-sampled t-tests and Pearson's chi-square tests for each study separately. Non-normally distributed data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U-tests for group comparisons. Mixed ANOVAs were used to analyze mean reaction times and number of hits, using reward magnitude (big or small) and cue type (addiction or neutral) as within-subject factors and group (patients or controls) as between-subject factor. Striatal group differences were analyzed using independent t-tests by taking the extracted parameter estimates for the three main fMRI contrasts: monetary reward anticipation, cue-reactivity and their interaction effect.

A significance threshold of p < 0.05 (two-tailed) was considered significant. Striatal analyses were additionally conducted using corresponding Bayesian analyses, using JASP's default Cauchy prior (0.707). The resulting Bayes Factor10 (BF10) indicates how much more likely the data are under the alternative hypothesis (H1) than under the null hypothesis (H0). We also report the BF01, which quantifies the relative evidence in favor of the null hypothesis, or in other words the amount of support for the absence of an effect. BF between 1 and three is considered to reflect anecdotal evidence, BF > 3 reflects substantial support and values larger than 10 reflect strong support (Wetzels et al., 2011). See (Wagenmakers et al., 2018) for an introduction into Bayesian hypothesis testing.

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the local Ethical Review Board of the Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands (2014_345). All subjects provided written informed consent.

Results

Results are reported for the AUD and GD study separately. All main neuroimaging results are available online at https://neurovault.org/collections/4199/.

Study 1: AUD

Demographics and clinical characteristics

The groups did not significantly differ on age (AUD: mean = 46.5, SD = 10.8; HCL mean = 44.8, SD = 9.9), handedness, gender, years of education and IQ. As expected, the AUD group had significantly more smokers (p < 0.001) and scored higher on all factors related to alcohol use: AUDIT, lifetime alcohol intake, kg pure alcohol use and OCDS (all p < 0.001). Demographics and clinical characteristics are summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

Behavioral results

Task performance

The repeated measures ANOVAs of reaction time and number of hits indicated a main effect of reward size: as expected, participants were faster (F1,74 = 38.9; p < 0.001, η2 = 0.34) and more accurate (F1,74 = 20.2; p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21) for big compared to small rewards (Fig. 1B and S1). No significant main or interaction effects of cue-type or group were found.

Craving ratings

For the cue-induced craving ratings there was a group by cue-type interaction (F1,68 = 32.2, p < 0.001). Post-hoc tests revealed this interaction was driven by significantly higher craving ratings for alcohol cues in AUD patients (mean = 3.49, SD = 1.92) compared to neutral cues in AUD patients (mean = 1.44, SD = 0.58; t42 = 7.40 p < 0.001) and compared to alcohol cues in HCs (mean = 1.49, SD = 0.70; t68 = 5.18 p < 0.001).

Imaging results

Figure 4b includes a visual overview of average ventral striatal activity on the different task conditions for each group.

Reward anticipation

In line with our hypothesis, AUD patients showed significantly decreased activity, relative to HCs, while anticipating big versus small monetary rewards in all three ROIs: the ventral striatum, putamen and caudate (two-sampled t-test: all p < 0.001; see Table 1). Whole-brain sensitivity analyses further showed pallidum, insula, hippocampus and supplementary motor area extending to the right middle and medial cingulate and inferior OFC (pFWE<0.05, Fig. 3A).

Table 1.

Region of interest results Study 1

tdfpCohen's dBF₁₀BF₀₁
Study 1: Group comparisons for reward anticipation effect (AUD vs HC)
Ventral striatum−3.72374<0.001−0.8745496.4171.819e-4
Caudate−4.69374<0.001−1.10169.3720.014
Putamen−5.08274<0.001−1.1931437.5976.956e-4
Study 1: Group comparisons for cue reactivity effect (AUD vs HC)
Ventral striatum0.082740.9350.0190.2434.118
Caudate−0.058740.954−0.0140.2434.123
Putamen0.586740.5590.1380.2813.561

(Bayesian) independent samples t-tests were used to test group comparisons between AUD patients and HCs on the effect of reward anticipation (big > small) and cue reactivity (alcohol > neutral cues), for all three regions of interest. BF: Bayes Factor, with BF10 quantifying the evidence for the alternative hypothesis and BF01 quantifying the evidence for the null hypothesis See Statistical Analysis for more information.

Cue-reactivity

Contrary to our hypothesis, the contrast comparing alcohol with neutral cues showed no significant group differences in striatal activation as revealed by ROI analyses (see Table 1), nor on the whole brain level. Within the AUD group, this cue-reactivity contrast revealed significantly increased activation in several clusters in the bilateral precuneus extending to the middle cingulate, the bilateral frontal superior medial extending to the anterior cingulate cortex [ACC], the left frontal inferior orbital cortex and several occipital regions (left angular and right lingual gyrus), all pFWE<0.05; see Fig. 2B. In contrast, HCs showed increased activity only in the primary visual cortex. Thus, neural responses to alcohol cues in the AUD group were dissociable from neutral cues, but not in striatal regions nor significantly different from HCs.

Fig. 2:
Fig. 2:

AUD patients showed (a) decreased activity during monetary reward anticipation relative to HCs and (b) increased activity to addiction-related compared to neutral cues, while (c&d) a negative association was found between the level of obsessive alcohol-related thoughts (OCDS) and reward anticipation (big > small) in AUD. (a) Whole-brain statistical parametric map showing blunted monetary reward anticipation (big > small rewards) in AUD patients compared to HCs (AUD < HC). Results shown at p < 0.05, FWE-corrected. (b) Cue-reactivity (alcohol > neutral cues) within AUD group. Results shown at p < 0.05, FWE-corrected. (c) Whole-brain statistical parametric map for the correlation between OCDS scores and the big > small contrast, shown at p < 0.001 uncorrected. (d) Correlation between OCDS and mean parameter estimates extracted from the putamen

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 12, 2; 10.1556/2006.2023.00015

Reward x cue-type interaction

The reward-magnitude–by–cue-type interaction revealed no significant differences (cluster-wise pFWE>0.05) in BOLD activity between AUD patients and HCs, or within the group of AUD individuals, both within the striatal ROIs and on the whole-brain.

Study 2: gambling disorder

The GD and HC groups were matched on age (GD: mean = 35.5, SD = 12.4; HC: mean = 35.4, SD = 15.8), gender, years of education, alcohol use and smoking status, but differed on handedness, IQ and factors related to gambling. Median abstinence duration for gamblers was 9 weeks (range 1–100). Demographics and clinical characteristics are presented in Supplementary Table 2.

Behavioral results

Task performance

Similar to the results in Study 1, main effects of reward were seen for reaction time (F1,44 = 63.9; p < 0.001, η2 = 0.59) and number of hits (F1,44 = 26.3; p < 0.001, η2 = 0.37), driven by participants being faster and more accurate for big compared to small rewards (Fig. 1B and S1). Moreover, significant reward*cue-type interactions were found for both reaction time (F1,44 = 4.7; p = 0.035, η2 = 0.10) and hits (F1,44 = 4.6; p = 0.037, η2 = 0.09): in the presence of gambling cues, GD and HC participants were faster when playing for big rewards, but slower when playing for small rewards. No significant main or interaction effects of group were found.

Craving ratings

As expected, there was a group-by-cue-type interaction for the post-scan craving ratings (F1,37 = 12.6, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.08), driven by higher ratings for gambling (mean = 3.98, SD = 2.65) compared to neutral cues (mean = 1.32, SD = 0.81) in GD patients (t21 = 5.15, p < 0.001) and compared to gambling cues in HCs (mean = 1.75, SD = 0.80; t37 = 3.34 p = 0.002; neutral cues HCs: mean = 1.24, SD = 0.05).

Imaging results

Reward anticipation

Contrary to our hypothesis, the ROI analyses showed no significant group differences in striatal activation in the contrast comparing anticipation of big monetary rewards versus small monetary rewards (Table 2). On the whole-brain level, GD patients showed decreased activation relative to HCs during reward anticipation in two clusters, with one peak in the left angular gyrus extending to the temporal middle and superior gyrus, and one peak in the right temporal superior gyrus extending to the temporal middle gyrus and hippocampus (pFWE<0.05; Fig. 3A).

Table 2.

Region of interest results Study 2

tdfpCohen's dBF₁₀BF₀₁
Study 2: Group comparisons for reward anticipation effect (GD vs HC)
Ventral striatum−0.795440.431−0.2350.3782.646
Caudate−1.094440.280−0.3230.4742.108
Putamen−1.348440.184−0.3980.6081.645
Study 2: Group comparisons for cue reactivity effect (GD vs HC)
Ventral striatum1.209440.2330.3570.5271.896
Caudate−0.135440.893−0.0400.2953.393
Putamen−0.699440.488−0.2060.3572.804

(Bayesian) independent samples t-tests were used to conduct group comparisons between GD patients and HCs on the effect of reward anticipation (big > small) and cue reactivity (gambling > neutral cues), for all three regions of interest. BF: Bayes Factor, with BF10 quantifying the evidence for the alternative hypothesis and BF01 quantifying the evidence for the null hypothesis. See Statistical Analysis for more information.

Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.

GD patients showed decreased activity during monetary reward anticipation. Whole-brain statistical parametric map showing blunted reward anticipation in gamblers compared to controls in the left angular gyrus extending to the temporal middle and superior gyrus, the right temporal superior gyrus extending to the temporal middle gyrus and hippocampus

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 12, 2; 10.1556/2006.2023.00015

Cue-reactivity

Similar to the results from the AUD study, no significant differences were found between GD patients and HCs on the contrast comparing gambling with neutral cues, neither in the striatal ROIs (Table 2) nor whole-brain (pFWE>0.05). Within the group of patients with GD, increased activation of the bilateral calcarine and lingual gyrus was seen during cue-reactivity (pFWE<0.05, Fig. S2).

Reward x cue-type interaction

No significant differences were found for the reward-magnitude*cue-type interaction between the groups or within GD patients.

Exploratory analyses

Relation striatal activity and craving, severity, chronicity, and abstinence

Tables with all correlation coefficients are included in the supplementary material (Suppl Tables 5–8). Because these correlational analyses were exploratory, it's not meaningful to report p-values (Gelman & Loken, 2013), which is why we only present and interpret medium and larger effect sizes (r > 0.3). A moderate (r = −0.44) negative correlation was seen between OCDS scores and reward anticipation in the putamen (Fig. 2D). Thus, AUD patients who reported having more obsessive alcohol-related thoughts showed stronger hyporesponsive dorsal striatal activity during monetary reward processing. In the group of patients with GD, ventral striatal cue reactivity was moderately (r = 0.44) correlated to the duration of gambling problems, while the activity in the same regions during monetary reward anticipation showed a moderate (r = −0.37) negative association with duration of abstinence. All other correlations were relatively low.

Relation between striatal ‘ups’ and ‘downs’

No significant (negative) correlations were found between striatal activity during cue-reactivity (hypothesized ‘up’) and monetary reward anticipation (hypothesized ‘down’) in either AUD or GD patients (Fig. 4). Bayes Factors provided anecdotal evidence in AUD patients and substantial evidence in GD patients for an absence of such a relationship (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4), indicating that these factors are independently present across individual patients. Figures showing average striatal activity on the different task conditions for each group are included in the supplement (Figs S3 and S4).

Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.

Striatal region of interest analyses, showing the (absence of a) relationship between striatal hypo- and hyperactivity in the AUD and GD group (a), and ventral striatal activity for the four experimental conditions (b). (a) No correlation was found between hypoactivity (big > small) and hyperactivity (addiction-related > neutral cues) in either the AUD group or the GD group. BF₀₁ reflects Bayes Factors in favor of the null, reflecting how much more likely these data are to be observed under the hypothesis that there is no relationship between striatal hyperactivity and hypoactivity. (b) Extracted entral striatal activity during the four conditions, plotted separately for the patient groups and matched controls. Activity was significantly higher for big than small rewards in all groups. AUD patients showed significantly decreased activity during the processing of big rewards compared to HCs, i.e. hypoactivity. BOLD = blood oxygenation level dependent, a.u = arbitrary units; error bars represent 95% confidence intervals

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 12, 2; 10.1556/2006.2023.00015

Finally, to test the robustness of our findings and address demographical differences between clinical and controls groups, we conducted several sensitivity analyses. Results are reported in the Supplement.

Discussion

The present study assessed monetary reward anticipation in the presence versus absence of addiction-related cues in AUD and GD patients compared to HCs. Our results suggest that, compared to HCs, currently abstinent patients with AUD exhibited diminished striatal responses during monetary reward anticipation. Moreover, AUD patients showed increased neural responses to alcohol cues, but these activation patterns did not significantly differ from HCs. Conversely, participants with GD did not show decreased striatal neural responses during the anticipation of monetary rewards, nor increased activity in response to addiction-related cues compared to HCs.

In line with our hypothesis and previous studies (Beck et al., 2009; Wrase et al., 2007), AUD showed significantly decreased striatal responses during monetary reward anticipation compared to HCs. However, relative to HCs, participants with GD only showed decreased activity in temporal regions but no differences in striatal activity. Following previous findings in both AUD (Chase, Eickhoff, Laird, & Hogarth, 2011; Schacht, Anton, & Myrick, 2013; Sjoerds et al., 2014) and GD (Goudriaan et al., 2010; Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2017), both groups showed increased activity in the presence of addiction-related compared to neutral cues in a number of regions previously implicated in cue-reactivity (Schacht et al., 2013), including the ACC, precuneus (Courtney, Ghahremani, London, & Ray, 2014), insula (Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2017) and visual areas (Hanlon, Dowdle, Naselaris, Canterberry, & Cortese, 2014). However, these neural patterns were not significantly different from the responses observed in HCs. There are several potential explanations for the lack of group differences in cue-elicited striatal activation. First, our findings are in line with results from a meta-analysis of alcohol cue-reactivity studies, which found that striatal activity in response to alcohol cues does not differentiate cases from controls (Schacht et al., 2013). However, a more recent voxel-wise meta-analysis of studies in alcohol use disorder showed increased medial PFC and ACC (Zeng et al., 2021), and a meta-analysis of behavioural addictions revealed hyperactivity in several regions including the striatum and cingulate (Starcke et al., 2018). Suggested reasons for ambiguous findings across cue-reactivity paradigms are (among others) drug availability and treatment status (Jasinska, Stein, Kaiser, Naumer, & Yalachkov, 2014). Second, the monetary rewards in our design may have interfered with the cue-processing and thereby abolished the cue-reactivity effect. This interference could have occurred in two ways. On the one hand, the fact that the addiction cues were shown in the background could have led to attentional distraction (further discussed under limitations). On the other hand, the cues signaling monetary rewards may already have addiction-like incentive properties. This is true for alcohol use disorder, where it may serve as a cue predicting the purchase of alcohol and thus to engagement in alcohol use. But especially for gambling disorder, where monetary rewards are the very outcomes of the addictive behavior itself, the inclusion of coins may have dissipated the relative intensity of the cue-reactivity effect. Thus, the appearance of coins may have led to some form of craving even in the neutral condition. In sum, our findings are in line with previous work suggesting that striatal cue-reactivity is not as robust as might be assumed, and our specific task-design may have further abolished an already difficult to induce effect. A promising way forward may be to use virtual reality technology, which researchers have recently started to exploit to create naturalistic settings in cue-reactivity studies (Bruder, Scharer, & Peters, 2021; Ghiţă & Gutiérrez-Maldonado, 2018).

Behaviorally, an interaction was seen in GD patients and HCs between gambling-related cues and rewards, such that gambling cues amplified the effect of reward magnitude in both directions. Thus, participants performed better (lowest reaction time, highest accuracy) in the presence of gambling cues when playing for big rewards, but performed worse when playing for small rewards. One explanation could be that gambling cues boost participants' impulsivity specifically when playing for bigger rewards (Miedl, Büchel, & Peters, 2014). Alternatively, this may be interpreted as an increased motivational effect of gambling cues on performance (Genauck et al., 2019), an effect also known as Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (Dickinson & Balleine, 1994), which is often considered as an important factor for the development of and relapse to addictive behavior in associative-learning models of addiction (Everitt & Robbins, 2015; Hogarth, Balleine, Corbit, & Killcross, 2013). However, no interaction between cue-reactivity and reward anticipation or any relation with craving was seen on a neural level.

Abnormal striatal processing may constitute a biomarker of addiction severity and risk for relapse (Courtney, Schacht, Hutchison, Roche, & Ray, 2016; Jasinska et al., 2014). Several studies have previously reported correlations between various addiction-related factors and striatal activity during MIDT (reviewed by Balodis & Potenza, 2015) or cue-reactivity tasks (reviewed by Schacht et al., 2013; Starcke et al., 2018), although often using small samples (n < 15). We tested these relations in our exploratory analyses, and, despite our relatively large AUD sample, we did not replicate associations between striatal activity and addiction severity (Sjoerds et al., 2014), abstinence duration (Li et al., 2014) or relapse (Courtney et al., 2016). Notably, however, AUDs who reported having more obsessive alcohol-related thoughts (higher score on OCDS) did show lower reward anticipation levels in several areas of the reward circuitry including the dorsal striatum, replicating previous findings (Wrase et al., 2007).

Our design also enabled us to directly test the relationship between striatal ‘ups’ (related to addiction cues) and ‘downs’ (related to monetary reward anticipation). However, we did not find a significant association between ventral striatal activity during monetary reward processing and dorsal (caudate or putamen) striatal activity during cue-reactivity. Moreover, Bayes Factors provided moderate evidence against such a relationship in both AUD and GD groups (with BFs ranging between 3.3 and 5.5). These results may be interpreted to suggest that neural reward processing and cue reactivity in addiction are not related, but rather independent factors. This interpretation is somewhat limited by the fact that we did not see robust striatal cue-reactivity effects compared to HCs on a group-level. Our findings contrast with Wrase et al. (2007), who let the same participants undergo a cue-reactivity and a MIDT on separate days and found that individual differences in the relative hyper- and hypoactivity in AUD patients correlated. Interestingly, a recent study in non-addicted humans found that participants who reported greater motivation (i.e., wanting) to consume more alcohol after a single moderate dose of alcohol also exhibited increased striatal reward processing on a subsequent fMRI session (Radoman et al., 2021). Hence, in non-addicted populations, there seems to be a relation between striatal sensitivity to drug rewards and striatal monetary rewards. Our results suggest that this relation may get dissociated during the development of an addiction.

Several limitations need to be considered when interpreting these results. First, our patients were all abstinent and (being) treated with cognitive behavioral therapy in which techniques against craving are trained (e.g., craving surfing). This may have abolished the cue-elicited striatal activation in our clinical group, as suggested by a voxel-wise fMRI meta-analysis (Zeng et al., 2021). Second, our fMRI task was designed to test the simultaneous processing of addiction-related cues and monetary rewards, but this might be problematic for the GD group as monetary rewards are the very outcomes reinforcing gambling and money may become a conditioned ‘cue’ itself. This difference between GD and AUD was one of the reasons we did not directly do comparisons between the clinical groups but rather have two separate experiments. The task-context in which monetary cues and reward anticipation are presented, may have largely differential effects in GD (van Holst, Veltman, Büchel, van den Brink, & Goudriaan, 2012; Wagner, Mathar, & Peters, 2021), but also in AUD: for instance, in an earlier study from our group, reward expectation during a gambling task induced higher activity in the striatum in AUD compared to healthy controls (van Holst, Clark, Veltman, van den Brink, & Goudriaan, 2014). Future research could combine cue-reactivity paradigms with natural rewards (e.g. erotic cues, Sescousse et al., 2013) to investigate the imbalance in reward sensitivity. Third, the clinical and control groups showed significant differences in several demographic variables. However, as we will outline below, we believe that these discrepancies do not significantly impede the interpretation of the results. In study 1, there were more smokers in the AUD group than in the control group, which is unsurprising given the high comorbidity (Grant, Hasin, Chou, Stinson, & Dawson, 2004). An additional analysis of neural reward anticipation between smoking and non-smoking AUD patients did not reveal any significant differences (see Supplement), indicating that the observed striatal hypoactivation may not be attributed to their smoking status. Furthermore, IQ was significantly lower in GD patients than the controls Because there is evidence for a (positive) association between IQ and ventral striatal reward anticipation (Kaminski et al., 2018). we conducted a sensitivity analysis controlling for IQ. Although the overall pattern of results remained unchanged, activity in the left angular and temporal superior gyrus was no longer significantly lower in GD patients compared to healthy controls. Finally, the control group exhibited a significantly higher proportion of left-handers compared to the GD group. However, we believe that the observed discrepancies handedness did not significantly influence the results as brain lateralization does not appear to play a major role in cue-reactivity and monetary reward anticipation (in contrast to e.g., language-related processes). In summary, while the control groups were not perfectly matched to the clinical groups, we believe that any potential impact on the results would be minimal.

In addition to money being a complex cue, a potential caveat of our task is that striatal activity has also been associated with increased attention irrespective of the presence of a current or prospective reward (Boehler et al., 2011; Breckel, Giessing, & Thiel, 2011; Fan, Hof, Guise, Fossella, & Posner, 2008). Hence, reduced striatal recruitment by reward anticipatory cues in our task may stem from inattention to the cues rather than any intrinsic ambivalence to rewards. That said, the behavioral effect of monetary rewards on reaction time (faster responses to bigger vs smaller monetary rewards), mitigates this idea. The addiction cues in our task were shown as task-irrelevant background distractors and may have captured attention away from an actual reward elicited instrumental behavior signal or vice versa, reducing the impact of the addiction cue to elicit cue-reactivity, which seems more likely given our results. Moreover, previous studies have found that drug cues can evoke activations evens when participants are unaware of having seen the cues (Childress et al., 2008). For the above-mentioned reasons and the fact that drug availability is important to generate cue-reactivity, future studies aiming at understanding the influence of addiction-related cues on reward anticipation could benefit from including abstract stimuli for big or small monetary outcomes (as is often done in the MIDT) and also manipulate the intensity of the addiction-relevant cues (i.e. high/low alcohol amounts) to predict these outcomes.

In conclusion, this study aimed to directly investigate striatal ups and downs in addictive disorders. The results suggest that, relative to controls, patients in treatment for AUD (but not GD) show striatal hypoactivations during reward anticipation in a MIDT task, while both groups did not show hyperactivation during cue-reactivity. The finding that decreased activity during reward anticipation was seen in alcohol use but not gambling disorder is in line with the idea that repeated drug use leads to reward deficiency, while we did not find evidence for a sensitized ‘hyperdopaminergic’ striatal response to addiction cues. Finally, despite large individual variance in ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation and dorsal striatal cue-reactivity, there was no correlation between the two (and in fact moderate Bayesian evidence against such a relationship), suggesting that these processes independently contribute to addiction.

Funding sources

This work is supported by a NWO-ZonMw grant VIDI [grant number 91713354] to Anna E. Goudriaan.

Authors' contribution

TvT, RJvH and AEG conceived and designed the study. TvT acquired, analyzed and interpreted the data, with support of RJvH and AEG. TvT prepared the manuscript; all other authors provided critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors read, corrected and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

Ruth J. van Holst is an associate editor of the Journal of Behavioral Addictions. The other authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank prof. Wim van den Brink for insightful comments on this manuscript.

Supplementary material

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2023.00015.

References

  • Balodis, I. M., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). Anticipatory reward processing in addicted populations: A focus on the monetary incentive delay task. Biological Psychiatry, 77(5), 434444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A., Schlagenhauf, F., Wüstenberg, T., Hein, J., Kienast, T., Kahnt, T., … Wrase, J. (2009). Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 734742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Billieux, J., Khazaal, Y., Oliveira, S., De Timary, P., Edel, Y., ZebouniF., … Van Der Linden, M. (2011). The Geneva appetitive alcohol pictures (GAAP): Development and preliminary validation. European Addiction Research, 17(5), 225230. https://doi.org/10.1159/000328046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2012). Mesolimbic recruitment by nondrug rewards in detoxified alcoholics: Effort anticipation, reward anticipation, and reward delivery. Human Brain Mapping, 33(9), 21742188. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21351.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blaszczynski, Alex, & Nower, Lia (2010). Instrumental tool or drug: Relationship between attitudes to money and problem gambling. Addiction Research & Theory, 18(6), 681691. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066351003786752.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blum, K., Braverman, E. R., Holder, J. M., Lubar, J. F., Monastra, V. J., Miller, D., … Comings, D. E. (2000). Reward deficicency syndrome: A biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32(Suppl 1), 1112. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2000.10736099.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boehler, C. N., Hopf, J. M., Krebs, R. M., Stoppel, C. M., Schoenfeld, M. A., Heinze, H. J., & Noesselt, T. (2011). Task-load-dependent activation of dopaminergic midbrain areas in the absence of reward. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(13), 4955. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4845-10.2011.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Breckel, T. P. K., Giessing, C., & Thiel, C. M. (2011). Impact of brain networks involved in vigilance on processing irrelevant visual motion. NeuroImage, 55(4), 17541762. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.01.025.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bruder, L. R., Scharer, L., & Peters, J. (2021). Reliability assessment of temporal discounting measures in virtual reality environments. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86388-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühler, M., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Kobiella, A., Budde, H., Reed, L. J., Braus, D. F., … Smolka, M. N. (2010). Nicotine dependence is characterized by disordered reward processing in a network driving motivation. Biological Psychiatry, 67(8), 745752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.029.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 94(3), 327340. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chase, H. W., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., & Hogarth, L. (2011). The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 70(8), 785793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.025.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Childress, A. R., Ehrman, R. N., Wang, Z., Li, Y., Sciortino, N., Hakun, J., … O'Brien, C. P. (2008). Prelude to passion: Limbic activation by “unseen” drug and sexual cues. PloS One, 3(1), e1506. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001506.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, L., Boileau, I., & Zack, M. (2019). Neuroimaging of reward mechanisms in gambling disorder: An integrative review. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(5), 674693. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0230-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Claus, E. D., Ewing, S. W. F., Filbey, F. M., Sabbineni, A., & Hutchison, K. E. (2011). Identifying neurobiological phenotypes associated with alcohol use disorder severity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(10), 20862096. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.99.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Courtney, K. E., Ghahremani, D. G., London, E. D., & Ray, L. A. (2014). The association between cue-reactivity in the precuneus and level of dependence on nicotine and alcohol. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 141, 2126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.026.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Courtney, K. E., Schacht, J. P., Hutchison, K., Roche, D. J. O., & RayL. A. (2016). Neural substrates of cue reactivity: Association with treatment outcomes and relapse. Addiction Biology, 21(1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12314.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • De Wildt, W. A. J. M., Lehert, P., Schippers, G. M., Nakovics, H., Mann, K., & Van Den Brink, W. (2005). Investigating the structure of craving using structural equation modeling in analysis of the obsessive-compulsive drinking scale: A multinational study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(4), 509516. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.ALC.0000158844.35608.48.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22(Issue 1), 118. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199951.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2015). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 150807174122003. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fan, J., Hof, P. R., Guise, K. G., Fossella, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2008). The functional integration of the anterior cingulate cortex during conflict processing. Cerebral Cortex, 18(4), 796805. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm125.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fauth-Bühler, M., Zois, E., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Lemenager, T., Beutel, M., & Mann, K. (2014). Insula and striatum activity in effort-related monetary reward processing in gambling disorder: The role of depressive symptomatology. NeuroImage: Clinical, 6, 243251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. In Canadian centre on substance abuse. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9224-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, M., Munafò, M. R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 589607. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0015843.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Filbey, F. M., Claus, E., Audette, A. R., Niculescu, M., Banich, M. T., Tanabe, J., … Hutchison, K. E. (2008). Exposure to the taste of alcohol elicits activation of the mesocorticolimbic neurocircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(6), 13911401. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301513.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Freeman, T. P., Morgan, C. J. A., Beesley, T., & Curran, H. v. (2012). Drug cue induced overshadowing: Selective disruption of natural reward processing by cigarette cues amongst abstinent but not satiated smokers. Psychological Medicine, 42(1), 161171. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001139.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2013). The garden of forking paths: Why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no “fishing expedition” or “p-hacking” and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. In Department of Statistics, Columbia University, New York, US (Issue 1). https://doi.org/10.7135/upo9781843313649.011.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Genauck, A., Andrejevic, M., Brehm, K., Matthis, C., Heinz, A., Weinreich, A., … Romanczuk-Seiferth, N. (2019). Cue‐induced effects on decision‐making distinguish subjects with gambling disorder from healthy controls. Addiction Biology, July, 564781. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12841.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ghiţă, A., & Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J. (2018). Applications of virtual reality in individuals with alcohol misuse: A systematic review. Addictive Behaviors, 81(January), 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.036.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 12(11), 652669. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goudriaan, A. E., de Ruiter, M. B., van den Brink, W., Oosterlaan, J., & Veltman, D. J. (2010). Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: An fMRI study. Addiction Biology, 15(4), 491503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00242.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grant, B. F., Hasin, D. S., Chou, S. P., Stinson, F. S., & Dawson, D. A. (2004). Nicotine dependence and psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and RelatedConditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(11), 11071115. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.61.11.1107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., Naselaris, T., Canterberry, M., & Cortese, B. M. (2014). Visual cortex activation to drug cues: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging papers in addiction and substance abuse literature. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 143, 206212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.07.028.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hogarth, L., Balleine, B. W., Corbit, L. H., & Killcross, S. (2013). Associative learning mechanisms underpinning the transition from recreational drug use to addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1282, 1224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06768.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ihssen, N., Cox, W. M., Wiggett, A., Fadardi, J. S., & Linden, D. E. J. (2011). Differentiating heavy from light drinkers by neural responses to visual alcohol cues and other motivational stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 21(6), 14081415. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq220.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jasinska, A. J., Stein, E. A., Kaiser, J., Naumer, M. J., & Yalachkov, Y. (2014). Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: A survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 38(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.8.6). [Computer Software].

  • Kaminski, J. A., Schlagenhauf, F., Rapp, M., Awasthi, S., Ruggeri, B., Deserno, L., … Heinz, A. (2018). Epigenetic variance in dopamine D2 receptor: A marker of IQ malleability? Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0222-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., & Hommer, D. W. (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage, 12(1), 2027. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0593.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2005). Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the “dark side” of drug addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 14421444. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1105-1442.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2008). Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 2953. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leyton, M., & Vezina, P. (2013). Striatal ups and downs: Their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9 Pt A), 19992014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.018.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leyton, M., & Vezina, P. (2014). Dopamine ups and downs in vulnerability to addictions: A neurodevelopmental model. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 35(6), 268276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tips.2014.04.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, P., Wu, P., Xin, X., Fan, Y. L., Wang, G. B., Wang, F., … Lu, L. (2014). Incubation of alcohol craving during abstinence in patients with alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology, 513522. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Limbrick-Oldfield, E. H., Mick, I., Cocks, R. E., McGonigle, J., Sharman, S. P., Goldstone, A. P., … Clark, L. (2017). Neural substrates of cue reactivity and craving in gambling disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 7(1), e992. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.256.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Limbrick-Oldfield, E. H., Van Holst, R. J., & Clark, L. (2013). Fronto-striatal dysregulation in drug addiction and pathological gambling: Consistent inconsistencies? NeuroImage: Clinical, 2, 385393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.02.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luijten, M., Schellekens, A. F., Kühn, S., Machielse, M. W. J., & Sescousse, G. (2017). Disruption of reward processing in addiction. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(4), 387. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miedl, S. F., Büchel, C., & Peters, J. (2014). Cue-induced craving increases impulsivity via changes in striatal value signals in problem gamblers. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(13), 47504755. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5020-13.2014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Poser, B. A., Versluis, M. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Norris, D. G. (2006). BOLD contrast sensitivity enhancement and artifact reduction with multiecho EPI: Parallel-acquired inhomogeneity-desensitized fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 55(6), 12271235. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20900.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Radoman, M., Crane, N. A., Gorka, S. M., Weafer, J., Langenecker, S. A., de Wit, H., & Phan, K. L. (2021). Striatal activation to monetary reward is associated with alcohol reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(2), 343350. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0728-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 31373146. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0093.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption--II. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 88(6), 791804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schacht, J. P., Anton, R. F., & Myrick, H. (2013). Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: A quantitative meta-analysis and systematic review. Addiction Biology, 18(1), 121133. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sescousse, G., Barbalat, G., Domenech, P., & Dreher, J.-C. (2013). Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 136(Pt 8), 25272538. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt126.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sheehan, D., Lecrubier, Y., & Sheehan, K. (1998). Diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 2233.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sjoerds, Z., van den Brink, W., Beekman, A. T. F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., & Veltman, D. J. (2014). Cue reactivity is associated with duration and severity of alcohol dependence: An FMRI study. PloS One, 9(1), e84560. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084560.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Skinner, H. A., & Sheu, W. J. (1982). Reliability of alcohol use indices. The lifetime drinking history and the MAST. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 43(11), 11571170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., & Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 112. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.39.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., … Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15(1), 273289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Clark, L., Veltman, D. J., van den Brink, W., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2014). Enhanced striatal responses during expectancy coding in alcohol dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 142, 204208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Veltman, D. J., Büchel, C., van den Brink, W., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Distorted expectancy coding in problem gambling: Is the addictive in the anticipation? Biological Psychiatry, 71(8), 741748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.030.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Piray, P., Goudriaan, A. E., & van Holst, R. J. (2023). Goal-directed and habitual decision making under stress in gambling disorder: An fMRI study. Addictive Behaviors, 140(107628). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2023.107628.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Quail, S. L., Balleine, B. W., Geurts, D. E. M., Goudriaan, A. E., & van Holst, R. J. (2020). Intact corticostriatal control of goal-directed action in alcohol use disorder : A pavlovian-to- instrumental transfer and outcome- devaluation study. Scientific Reports, 10(4949), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-61892-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Zhutovsky, P., van Holst, R. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2018). Connectivity networks in gambling disorder: A resting-state fMRI study. International Gambling Studies, 18(2), 242258. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2018.1449884.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vezina, P., & Leyton, M. (2009). Conditioned cues and the expression of stimulant sensitization in animals and humans. Neuropharmacology, 56(Suppl. 1), 160168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.070.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., Koob, G. F., & McLellan, A. T. (2016). Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. The New England Journal of Medicine, 374(4), 363371. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1511480.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., & Morales, M. (2015). The brain on drugs: From reward to addiction. Cell, 162(4), 712725. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G., … Mann, K. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 105(10), 17411749. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagenmakers, E. J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(1), 3557. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagner, B., Mathar, D., & Peters, J. (2021). Gambling environment exposure increases temporal discounting but improves model-based control in regular slot-machine gamblers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 291298. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406923.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wiers, R. W., Bartholow, B. D., van den Wildenberg, E., Thush, C., Engels, R. C. M. E., Sher, K. J., … Stacy, A. W. (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 86(2), 263283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wrase, J., Schlagenhauf, F., Kienast, T., Wüstenberg, T., Bermpohl, F., Kahnt, T., … Heinz, A. (2007). Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. NeuroImage, 35(2), 787794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zeng, J., Yu, S., Cao, H., Su, Y., Dong, Z., & Yang, X. (2021). Neurobiological correlates of cue-reactivity in alcohol-use disorders: A voxel-wise meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 128, 294310. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.06.031.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zilberman, N., Lavidor, M., Yadid, G., & Rassovsky, Y. (2019). Qualitative review and quantitative effect size meta-analyses in brain regions identified by cue-reactivity addiction studies. Neuropsychology, 33(3), 319334. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000526.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Supplementary Materials

  • Balodis, I. M., & Potenza, M. N. (2015). Anticipatory reward processing in addicted populations: A focus on the monetary incentive delay task. Biological Psychiatry, 77(5), 434444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A., Schlagenhauf, F., Wüstenberg, T., Hein, J., Kienast, T., Kahnt, T., … Wrase, J. (2009). Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 734742. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.035.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Billieux, J., Khazaal, Y., Oliveira, S., De Timary, P., Edel, Y., ZebouniF., … Van Der Linden, M. (2011). The Geneva appetitive alcohol pictures (GAAP): Development and preliminary validation. European Addiction Research, 17(5), 225230. https://doi.org/10.1159/000328046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bjork, J. M., Smith, A. R., Chen, G., & Hommer, D. W. (2012). Mesolimbic recruitment by nondrug rewards in detoxified alcoholics: Effort anticipation, reward anticipation, and reward delivery. Human Brain Mapping, 33(9), 21742188. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21351.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blaszczynski, Alex, & Nower, Lia (2010). Instrumental tool or drug: Relationship between attitudes to money and problem gambling. Addiction Research & Theory, 18(6), 681691. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066351003786752.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blum, K., Braverman, E. R., Holder, J. M., Lubar, J. F., Monastra, V. J., Miller, D., … Comings, D. E. (2000). Reward deficicency syndrome: A biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32(Suppl 1), 1112. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2000.10736099.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boehler, C. N., Hopf, J. M., Krebs, R. M., Stoppel, C. M., Schoenfeld, M. A., Heinze, H. J., & Noesselt, T. (2011). Task-load-dependent activation of dopaminergic midbrain areas in the absence of reward. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(13), 4955. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4845-10.2011.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Breckel, T. P. K., Giessing, C., & Thiel, C. M. (2011). Impact of brain networks involved in vigilance on processing irrelevant visual motion. NeuroImage, 55(4), 17541762. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.01.025.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bruder, L. R., Scharer, L., & Peters, J. (2021). Reliability assessment of temporal discounting measures in virtual reality environments. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-86388-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühler, M., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Kobiella, A., Budde, H., Reed, L. J., Braus, D. F., … Smolka, M. N. (2010). Nicotine dependence is characterized by disordered reward processing in a network driving motivation. Biological Psychiatry, 67(8), 745752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.029.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 94(3), 327340. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chase, H. W., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., & Hogarth, L. (2011). The neural basis of drug stimulus processing and craving: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 70(8), 785793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.025.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Childress, A. R., Ehrman, R. N., Wang, Z., Li, Y., Sciortino, N., Hakun, J., … O'Brien, C. P. (2008). Prelude to passion: Limbic activation by “unseen” drug and sexual cues. PloS One, 3(1), e1506. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001506.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, L., Boileau, I., & Zack, M. (2019). Neuroimaging of reward mechanisms in gambling disorder: An integrative review. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(5), 674693. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0230-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Claus, E. D., Ewing, S. W. F., Filbey, F. M., Sabbineni, A., & Hutchison, K. E. (2011). Identifying neurobiological phenotypes associated with alcohol use disorder severity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(10), 20862096. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.99.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Courtney, K. E., Ghahremani, D. G., London, E. D., & Ray, L. A. (2014). The association between cue-reactivity in the precuneus and level of dependence on nicotine and alcohol. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 141, 2126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.026.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Courtney, K. E., Schacht, J. P., Hutchison, K., Roche, D. J. O., & RayL. A. (2016). Neural substrates of cue reactivity: Association with treatment outcomes and relapse. Addiction Biology, 21(1), 322. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12314.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • De Wildt, W. A. J. M., Lehert, P., Schippers, G. M., Nakovics, H., Mann, K., & Van Den Brink, W. (2005). Investigating the structure of craving using structural equation modeling in analysis of the obsessive-compulsive drinking scale: A multinational study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29(4), 509516. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.ALC.0000158844.35608.48.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. W. (1994). Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22(Issue 1), 118. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199951.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2015). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67(1), 150807174122003. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fan, J., Hof, P. R., Guise, K. G., Fossella, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2008). The functional integration of the anterior cingulate cortex during conflict processing. Cerebral Cortex, 18(4), 796805. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm125.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fauth-Bühler, M., Zois, E., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Lemenager, T., Beutel, M., & Mann, K. (2014). Insula and striatum activity in effort-related monetary reward processing in gambling disorder: The role of depressive symptomatology. NeuroImage: Clinical, 6, 243251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2014.09.008.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index: Final report. In Canadian centre on substance abuse. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-010-9224-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, M., Munafò, M. R., & Franken, I. H. A. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 589607. https://doi.org/10.1037/A0015843.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Filbey, F. M., Claus, E., Audette, A. R., Niculescu, M., Banich, M. T., Tanabe, J., … Hutchison, K. E. (2008). Exposure to the taste of alcohol elicits activation of the mesocorticolimbic neurocircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(6), 13911401. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1301513.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Freeman, T. P., Morgan, C. J. A., Beesley, T., & Curran, H. v. (2012). Drug cue induced overshadowing: Selective disruption of natural reward processing by cigarette cues amongst abstinent but not satiated smokers. Psychological Medicine, 42(1), 161171. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711001139.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2013). The garden of forking paths: Why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no “fishing expedition” or “p-hacking” and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. In Department of Statistics, Columbia University, New York, US (Issue 1). https://doi.org/10.7135/upo9781843313649.011.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Genauck, A., Andrejevic, M., Brehm, K., Matthis, C., Heinz, A., Weinreich, A., … Romanczuk-Seiferth, N. (2019). Cue‐induced effects on decision‐making distinguish subjects with gambling disorder from healthy controls. Addiction Biology, July, 564781. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12841.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ghiţă, A., & Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J. (2018). Applications of virtual reality in individuals with alcohol misuse: A systematic review. Addictive Behaviors, 81(January), 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.01.036.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldstein, R. Z., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 12(11), 652669. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3119.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goudriaan, A. E., de Ruiter, M. B., van den Brink, W., Oosterlaan, J., & Veltman, D. J. (2010). Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: An fMRI study. Addiction Biology, 15(4), 491503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00242.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grant, B. F., Hasin, D. S., Chou, S. P., Stinson, F. S., & Dawson, D. A. (2004). Nicotine dependence and psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and RelatedConditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(11), 11071115. https://doi.org/10.1001/ARCHPSYC.61.11.1107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., Naselaris, T., Canterberry, M., & Cortese, B. M. (2014). Visual cortex activation to drug cues: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging papers in addiction and substance abuse literature. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 143, 206212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.07.028.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hogarth, L., Balleine, B. W., Corbit, L. H., & Killcross, S. (2013). Associative learning mechanisms underpinning the transition from recreational drug use to addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1282, 1224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06768.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ihssen, N., Cox, W. M., Wiggett, A., Fadardi, J. S., & Linden, D. E. J. (2011). Differentiating heavy from light drinkers by neural responses to visual alcohol cues and other motivational stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 21(6), 14081415. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq220.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jasinska, A. J., Stein, E. A., Kaiser, J., Naumer, M. J., & Yalachkov, Y. (2014). Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: A survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 38(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.8.6). [Computer Software].

  • Kaminski, J. A., Schlagenhauf, F., Rapp, M., Awasthi, S., Ruggeri, B., Deserno, L., … Heinz, A. (2018). Epigenetic variance in dopamine D2 receptor: A marker of IQ malleability? Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0222-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Knutson, B., Westdorp, A., Kaiser, E., & Hommer, D. W. (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage, 12(1), 2027. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0593.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2005). Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the “dark side” of drug addiction. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 14421444. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1105-1442.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2008). Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 2953. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leyton, M., & Vezina, P. (2013). Striatal ups and downs: Their roles in vulnerability to addictions in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9 Pt A), 19992014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.018.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leyton, M., & Vezina, P. (2014). Dopamine ups and downs in vulnerability to addictions: A neurodevelopmental model. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 35(6), 268276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tips.2014.04.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, P., Wu, P., Xin, X., Fan, Y. L., Wang, G. B., Wang, F., … Lu, L. (2014). Incubation of alcohol craving during abstinence in patients with alcohol dependence. Addiction Biology, 513522. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Limbrick-Oldfield, E. H., Mick, I., Cocks, R. E., McGonigle, J., Sharman, S. P., Goldstone, A. P., … Clark, L. (2017). Neural substrates of cue reactivity and craving in gambling disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 7(1), e992. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.256.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Limbrick-Oldfield, E. H., Van Holst, R. J., & Clark, L. (2013). Fronto-striatal dysregulation in drug addiction and pathological gambling: Consistent inconsistencies? NeuroImage: Clinical, 2, 385393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2013.02.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luijten, M., Schellekens, A. F., Kühn, S., Machielse, M. W. J., & Sescousse, G. (2017). Disruption of reward processing in addiction. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(4), 387. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miedl, S. F., Büchel, C., & Peters, J. (2014). Cue-induced craving increases impulsivity via changes in striatal value signals in problem gamblers. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(13), 47504755. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5020-13.2014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Poser, B. A., Versluis, M. J., Hoogduin, J. M., & Norris, D. G. (2006). BOLD contrast sensitivity enhancement and artifact reduction with multiecho EPI: Parallel-acquired inhomogeneity-desensitized fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 55(6), 12271235. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20900.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Radoman, M., Crane, N. A., Gorka, S. M., Weafer, J., Langenecker, S. A., de Wit, H., & Phan, K. L. (2021). Striatal activation to monetary reward is associated with alcohol reward sensitivity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(2), 343350. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0728-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 31373146. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0093.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption--II. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 88(6), 791804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schacht, J. P., Anton, R. F., & Myrick, H. (2013). Functional neuroimaging studies of alcohol cue reactivity: A quantitative meta-analysis and systematic review. Addiction Biology, 18(1), 121133. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00464.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sescousse, G., Barbalat, G., Domenech, P., & Dreher, J.-C. (2013). Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 136(Pt 8), 25272538. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt126.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sheehan, D., Lecrubier, Y., & Sheehan, K. (1998). Diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 2233.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sjoerds, Z., van den Brink, W., Beekman, A. T. F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., & Veltman, D. J. (2014). Cue reactivity is associated with duration and severity of alcohol dependence: An FMRI study. PloS One, 9(1), e84560. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084560.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Skinner, H. A., & Sheu, W. J. (1982). Reliability of alcohol use indices. The lifetime drinking history and the MAST. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 43(11), 11571170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., & Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 112. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.39.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., … Joliot, M. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15(1), 273289. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Clark, L., Veltman, D. J., van den Brink, W., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2014). Enhanced striatal responses during expectancy coding in alcohol dependence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 142, 204208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.06.019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Veltman, D. J., Büchel, C., van den Brink, W., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Distorted expectancy coding in problem gambling: Is the addictive in the anticipation? Biological Psychiatry, 71(8), 741748. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.030.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Piray, P., Goudriaan, A. E., & van Holst, R. J. (2023). Goal-directed and habitual decision making under stress in gambling disorder: An fMRI study. Addictive Behaviors, 140(107628). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2023.107628.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Quail, S. L., Balleine, B. W., Geurts, D. E. M., Goudriaan, A. E., & van Holst, R. J. (2020). Intact corticostriatal control of goal-directed action in alcohol use disorder : A pavlovian-to- instrumental transfer and outcome- devaluation study. Scientific Reports, 10(4949), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-61892-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Zhutovsky, P., van Holst, R. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2018). Connectivity networks in gambling disorder: A resting-state fMRI study. International Gambling Studies, 18(2), 242258. https://doi.org/10.1080/14459795.2018.1449884.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vezina, P., & Leyton, M. (2009). Conditioned cues and the expression of stimulant sensitization in animals and humans. Neuropharmacology, 56(Suppl. 1), 160168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.070.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., Koob, G. F., & McLellan, A. T. (2016). Neurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction. The New England Journal of Medicine, 374(4), 363371. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1511480.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., & Morales, M. (2015). The brain on drugs: From reward to addiction. Cell, 162(4), 712725. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G., … Mann, K. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 105(10), 17411749. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagenmakers, E. J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, J., Love, J., … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(1), 3557. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagner, B., Mathar, D., & Peters, J. (2021). Gambling environment exposure increases temporal discounting but improves model-based control in regular slot-machine gamblers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wetzels, R., Matzke, D., Lee, M. D., Rouder, J. N., Iverson, G. J., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2011). Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 291298. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611406923.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wiers, R. W., Bartholow, B. D., van den Wildenberg, E., Thush, C., Engels, R. C. M. E., Sher, K. J., … Stacy, A. W. (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 86(2), 263283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wrase, J., Schlagenhauf, F., Kienast, T., Wüstenberg, T., Bermpohl, F., Kahnt, T., … Heinz, A. (2007). Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics. NeuroImage, 35(2), 787794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zeng, J., Yu, S., Cao, H., Su, Y., Dong, Z., & Yang, X. (2021). Neurobiological correlates of cue-reactivity in alcohol-use disorders: A voxel-wise meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 128, 294310. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.06.031.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zilberman, N., Lavidor, M., Yadid, G., & Rassovsky, Y. (2019). Qualitative review and quantitative effect size meta-analyses in brain regions identified by cue-reactivity addiction studies. Neuropsychology, 33(3), 319334. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000526.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Aug 2024 0 108 33
Sep 2024 0 82 28
Oct 2024 0 319 29
Nov 2024 0 458 41
Dec 2024 0 521 30
Jan 2025 0 159 16
Feb 2025 0 0 0