Authors:
Lu Liu Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Search for other papers by Lu Liu in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5461-6706
,
Yuan-Wei Yao Department of Psychology, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
State Key Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

Search for other papers by Yuan-Wei Yao in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9635-7826
,
Xiao-Yi Fang Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Xiao-Yi Fang in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9494-0997
,
Lin-Xuan Xu State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Lin-Xuan Xu in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Meng-Jian Hu Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Search for other papers by Meng-Jian Hu in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jin-Tao Zhang State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

Search for other papers by Jin-Tao Zhang in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4807-1196
, and
Marc N. Potenza Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Child Study Center, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA
Department of Neuroscience, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA

Search for other papers by Marc N. Potenza in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6323-1354
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Compulsivity contributes to the development and maintenance of multiple addictive disorders. However, the relationship between compulsivity-related cognitive features and problematic usage of the internet (PUI), an umbrella term for various internet use disorders/interfering behaviors, remains largely unclear, partly due to the multidimensional nature of compulsivity. This scoping review utilized a four-domain framework of compulsivity to consider this topic and aimed to summarize available evidence on compulsivity-related neuropsychological characteristics in PUI based on this framework.

Methods

A systematic literature search was conducted by applying the combination of search term to the search engines of PubMed, PsycINFO and Web of Science. A four-domain framework of compulsivity, involving cognitive flexibility, set-shifting, attentional bias, and habit learning, was used to consider its complex structure and frequently used tasks. Main findings in related PUI studies were summarized based on this framework. Our secondary aim was to compare compulsivity-related features between different PUI subtypes.

Results

Thirty-four empirical studies were retained, comprising 41 task-results and 35 independent data sets. Overall, individuals with PUI showed more consistent deficits in attentional biases and were relatively intact in set-shifting. Few studies have examined cognitive flexibility and habit learning, and more evidence is thus needed to establish reliable conclusions. Moreover, most studies focused on internet gaming disorder, whereas other PUI sub-types were not sufficiently examined.

Conclusion

This systematic review highlights the use of the four-domain framework for advancing understanding of mechanisms underlying compulsivity in PUI. Related therapeutic implications and future directions are discussed.

Abstract

Background and aims

Compulsivity contributes to the development and maintenance of multiple addictive disorders. However, the relationship between compulsivity-related cognitive features and problematic usage of the internet (PUI), an umbrella term for various internet use disorders/interfering behaviors, remains largely unclear, partly due to the multidimensional nature of compulsivity. This scoping review utilized a four-domain framework of compulsivity to consider this topic and aimed to summarize available evidence on compulsivity-related neuropsychological characteristics in PUI based on this framework.

Methods

A systematic literature search was conducted by applying the combination of search term to the search engines of PubMed, PsycINFO and Web of Science. A four-domain framework of compulsivity, involving cognitive flexibility, set-shifting, attentional bias, and habit learning, was used to consider its complex structure and frequently used tasks. Main findings in related PUI studies were summarized based on this framework. Our secondary aim was to compare compulsivity-related features between different PUI subtypes.

Results

Thirty-four empirical studies were retained, comprising 41 task-results and 35 independent data sets. Overall, individuals with PUI showed more consistent deficits in attentional biases and were relatively intact in set-shifting. Few studies have examined cognitive flexibility and habit learning, and more evidence is thus needed to establish reliable conclusions. Moreover, most studies focused on internet gaming disorder, whereas other PUI sub-types were not sufficiently examined.

Conclusion

This systematic review highlights the use of the four-domain framework for advancing understanding of mechanisms underlying compulsivity in PUI. Related therapeutic implications and future directions are discussed.

Introduction

Problematic usage of the internet (PUI), conceptualized as excessive online activity associated with marked functional impairment and addictive, impulsive and/or compulsive features, has been recognized as a global public health problem (Fineberg et al., 2018, 2022). The umbrella term PUI encompasses multiple potential problematic internet-related behaviors, such as gaming, pornography viewing, and social networking (Brand et al., 2019). In light of increasing incidence and research, the DSM-5 and ICD-11 have recognized internet gaming disorder (IGD), a major subtype of PUI, as a non-substance (behavioral) addiction. Recently, PUI has received more attention during the COVID-19 pandemic, as individuals utilized the internet for more aspects of daily life during this period (Fineberg et al., 2022). Because of the high frequency of internet use, increasingly more people may be automatically approaching internet activities, even without clear goals in some situations (van Deursen, Bolle, Hegner, & Kommers, 2015). However, the relationships between these automatic and repetitive actions and PUI remain poorly understood (Kim et al., 2017).

Excessive and habitual internet use may be closely related to compulsivity, which may be defined as the tendency to engage in “repetitive and functionally impairing overt or covert behavior without [apparent] adaptive function, performed in a habitual or stereotyped fashion, according to rigid rules or as a means of avoiding perceived negative consequences” (Fineberg et al., 2014; van Timmeren, Daams, van Holst, & Goudriaan, 2018). Importantly, compulsivity has been included in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework as a transdiagnostic construct that is common to multiple psychiatric disorders (Insel et al., 2010). For both substance use disorders (SUDs) and behavioral addictions, addictive disorders have been proposed as the endpoint of a behavioral transition from initial goal-directed approach to eventual automatic and compulsive addictive behaviors (Everitt & Robbins, 2016; Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit, & Ersche, 2012). Previous cue-reactivity fMRI studies have identified the brain regions involved in transitions across addictions (Liu et al., 2017), suggesting the involvement of compulsivity. Furthermore, questionnaires assessing internet addiction have identified internet-related compulsive behaviors in PUI populations (Demetrovics, van den Brink, Paksi, Horvath, & Maraz, 2022; Fineberg et al., 2022), echoing the syndrome of persistent and poorly controlled online activity regardless of negative consequences (Kuss, Griffiths, Karila, & Billieux, 2014; Petry et al., 2014). Thus, elucidating the cognitive mechanisms underlying compulsivity may not only advance our understanding of PUI as a set of internet-related addictions or other psychiatric conditions (e.g., related to obsessive-compulsive disorder), but also provide crucial information for the development of tailored interventions for PUI.

Compulsivity as a neuropsychological construct, with its behavioral underpinnings of inflexibility, rigidity and poor self-control, has been described in individuals with SUDs (Leeman & Potenza, 2012) and gambling disorder (van Timmeren et al., 2018). Although an increasing number of studies have investigated compulsive behaviors in individuals with PUI, most previous research has explicitly assessed compulsivity using self-report questionnaires, and this situation largely limits a mechanistic explanation of the cognitive processes underlying compulsive behaviors. On the other hand, some behavioral studies have used experimental tasks (e.g., probability learning task, set-shifting task) to compare the performance between individuals with and without PUI; however, these studies have rarely discussed their findings in the context of compulsivity. A major barrier to progress in this area is the complex construct of compulsivity and its ambiguous definition in most theoretical models of PUI (Yücel & Fontenelle, 2012). Indeed, previous studies have demonstrated that compulsivity is not a unidimensional construct. Rather, compulsivity may include at least four major components, including cognitive flexibility, set-shifting, habitual behaviors, and attentional bias (Carr, Wiedemann, Macdonald-Gagnon, & Potenza, 2021; Fineberg et al., 2014; Lee, Hoppenbrouwers, & Franken, 2019; van Timmeren et al., 2018). Thus, this four-component framework provides a suitable approach to systematically classify and summarize compulsivity-related findings in PUI.

Specifically, the four-component framework proposes that different aspects of compulsivity can be operationalized by different neurocognitive tasks (Table 1). For example, cognitive flexibility is usually measured by tasks that require participants to adapt to changing rules or environments. A representative example is reversal learning task in which the best option is not fixed, and participants need to learn related information by trial and error (Izquierdo & Jentsch, 2012). Set shifting is mainly related to attentional switching between (neutral) stimuli and can be measured by tasks that rely on switching ability (e.g., Wisconsin card sorting task) (van Timmeren et al., 2018). Attentional bias, on the other hand, focuses on the hypersensitivity to salient (often addiction-related) stimuli (Field & Cox, 2008). Therefore, this process can be evaluated by tasks involving the inhibition of prepotent and automatic responses (e.g., addiction-related Stroop task). Finally, habit learning refers to the establishment of engrained/automatic over goal-directed behaviors (Vandaele & Ahmed, 2021). A classic paradigm that has been used to disentangle habitual and goal-directed behaviors during learning is the sequential two-step task (Daw, Gershman, Seymour, Dayan, & Dolan, 2011). While these tasks may assess features of compulsivity, they may also overlap with other constructs (e.g., impulsivity), and this may in part explain why some groups score high on measures of both compulsivity and impulsivity (Fineberg et al., 2014; van Timmeren et al., 2018).

Table 1.

Domains of compulsivity in PUI

Neurocognitive domainsDefinitionTasksNumber of studies
Contingency-related cognitive flexibilityDifficulties adaptating behavior after negative feedback (devaluation or degradation of the outcome)Probabilistic reversal learning task3
Deterministic reversal learning task
Contingency learning task
Contingency degradation task
Set-shiftingDifficulties frequently switching among a set of rules or response modesWisconsin card sorting task13
Intra-extra dimensional set shift
Trail Making Task
Switch task
Attentional bias/attentional disengagementDifficulties with attentional engagement or disengagement to prepotent disorder-relevant stimuliDot probe task17
Addiction Stroop task
Addiction Go/No-Go task
Habit learningLimited sensitivity to goals or outcomes of actions (an imbalance between goal-directed and habitual behavior)Two-step decision task3
Fabulous fruit game
Devaluation task

In the current study, we aimed to comprehensively review previous behavioral studies that investigated compulsivity in individuals with PUI. As a wide range of tasks may be used to explore distinct parts of compulsivity, we employed the four-domain framework from the compulsivity literature to facilitate the classification of related tasks (Fineberg et al., 2014). Therefore, rather than treating compulsivity as a unidimensional construct, we examined the relationship between the four defined neurocognitive components and PUI. Finally, although the current study was not restricted to specific subtypes of PUI, most studies have focused on IGD. Since different subtypes of PUI may involve different psychopathological mechanisms, if applicable, we additionally explored the compulsivity-related differences between IGD and other subtypes of PUI.

Methods

Information sources and search strategy

A scoping review was conducted to clarify the framework of compulsivity and to map available evidence (Munn et al., 2018; Peters et al., 2015). This review was performed in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., 2015). The literature search was conducted to compile existing evidence regarding the four neuropsychological components of compulsivity in PUI. Different types of PUI were considered based on their documented prevalence and psychosocial correlates (Castro-Calvo et al., 2021; Fineberg et al., 2022). Specifically, problematic online activities were selected with the restriction that the problematic usage of the particular online activity (1) was well defined and studied; (2) was identified with addictive, impulsive or compulsive features; (3) was identified with considerable prevalence estimates; (4) was mainly for entertainment and relief, largely consistent with prior descriptions of possible internet use disorders(Müller et al., 2022). However, gambling was omitted given that most studies did not distinguish between online and offline betting and smartphone use was included given that it appeared to fit the four criteria. Therefore, five subtypes of PUI were included: (1) internet gaming, (2) social media use, (3) smartphone use, (4) online shopping, and (5) internet pornography.

Relevant peer-reviewed studies (January 2000 – February 2024) published in English were identified via search engines of PubMed, PsycINFO and Web of Science by using the search terms in the Title and Abstract: (addict* OR excessive OR problematic OR pathologic* OR disorder) AND (internet OR computer OR ‘internet use’ OR ‘online gaming’ OR game OR gaming OR ‘smartphone use’ OR ‘social media use’ OR social network* OR ‘online shopping’ OR ‘online buying’ OR ‘internet pornography’) AND (compulsive OR compulsion OR habit* OR ‘cognitive flexibility’ OR ‘reversal learning’ OR ‘card playing task’ OR ‘probabilistic learning’ OR ‘set shifting’ OR ‘intra-extra dimensional’ OR ‘Wisconsin card sorting’ OR ‘trail making’ OR ‘rule shift card’ OR ‘dot-probe’ OR ‘Stroop task’ OR ‘go/no-go task’ OR ‘attentional bias’ OR ‘attentional disengagement’ OR fruit game OR ‘slips of action’ OR ‘instrumental learning’ OR ‘habit learning’ OR ‘reinforcement learning’ OR ‘two-step decision’ OR ‘two-stage task’ OR ‘model-based’ OR ‘model-free’ OR ‘goal-directed’ OR ‘sequential decision’ OR ‘sequential learning’ OR ‘contingency learning’ OR perseveration OR devaluation OR ‘contingency degradation’).

Studies were eligible if they: (1) were original peer-reviewed articles in humans; (2) included at least one PUI group screened according to predefined criteria (e.g., DSM-5-defined IGD, Young Internet Addiction Test (YIAT), Chen Internet Addiction Scale (CIAS), or other PUI scales); (3) included a healthy control group; (4) used at least one experimental task related to one of the four components of compulsivity (Table 1; Fig. 1); and, (5) compared compulsive behaviors between PUI and control groups. In addition, as the current study focused on the neuropsychological/behavioral features of compulsivity, studies that only used questionnaires to assess compulsivity in PUI were not included.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) flow diagram. PUI = problematic usage of the internet; HC = healthy control

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 13, 2; 10.1556/2006.2024.00023

Study selection

The titles and abstracts of all identified studies were independently screened by two researchers (L.L. and Y.W.Y.). The selected studies were read in full to ensure all inclusion criteria were met. Discrepancies in these steps were resolved by consensus among the authors.

Data extraction and study quality

Data extraction included: (1) demographic and clinical characteristics (e.g., sample sizes, age, and diagnostic tools); (2) neuropsychological measurements and primary related dependent variables, and (3) main results. The selected variables for the included studies are listed in Tables 25.

Table 2.

Overview of included studies within the contingency-related cognitive flexibility domain

TasksAuthor(s)Population (PUI/HC)Mean age (SD)DiagnosisDVsMajor findingsSummaryComorbidity control
PRLTBanca et al. (2016)IGD = 26; HC = 52IGD = 24.69 (5.90); HC = 22.91 (4.73)DSM-IVatrials to criterion; lose-shift/win-stay
  1. IGD group took more trials to reach reversal criterion in the reversal phase;

  2. No significant group difference of trials to criterion in the acquisition phase;

  3. No significant group difference in lose-shift or win-stay.

IGD < HCNo
PRLTRaj et al. (2023)PUI = 52; HC = 38;PUI = 22.80 (4.25); HC = 22.07 (3.61)IAT-10b ≥ 30perseverative errors
  1. The PIU group showed more perseverative errors compared to HCs;

  2. The perseverative errors significantly statistically predicted the likelihood of an individual experiencing PUI

PIU < HCYes (MINI)
Probability learningLei et al. (2022)IGD = 45; HC = 42IGD = 20.82 (1.37); HC = 21.29 (1.52)DSM-5 ≥ 5;discrimination accuracyNo significant group difference in accuracy; 2. IGD showed blunted RPE signals in the caudate, OFC and DLPFCIGD = HCNo

Note. DLFPC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; DSM = diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; DV = dependent variable; HC = healthy controls; IGD = internet gaming disorder; MINI = the mini international psychiatric interview; OFC = orbitofrontal cortex; PUI = problematic usage of the internet; RPE = reward prediction error; PRLT = probabilistic reversal learning task; SD = standard deviation.

Note: a, adapted criteria of DSM-IV Pathological Gambling; b, the 10-item abbreviated version of Young's Internet Addiction Test (IAT-10).

Table 3.

Overview of included studies within the attentional/set-shifting domain

TasksAuthor(s)Population (PUI/HC)Mean age (SD)DiagnosisDVsMajor findingsSummaryComorbidity control
Wisconsin card sorting test
WCSTAydin et al. (2020)PSNSU = 100; HC = 140PSNSU = 22.16 (3.50); HC = 23.42 (5.19)BSMAS ≥19perseverative errors; number of category completedNo significant group differences in both the perseverative errors and the numbers of categories completedPSNSU = HCNo
WCSTFiroozabadi, Razavian, Saleh, and Hosseini (2023)PUI at-risk = 30; PUI = 12; HC = 117PUI at-risk = 22.59 (3.83); PUI = 24.17 (4.38); HC = 26.17 (9.03)IA at-risk: YIAT 50–79; IA: YIAT 80–100perseverative errorsNo significant group differences in perseverative errors between IA and HC groups, nor between IA at-risk and HC groups.IA = HC; IA at-risk = HCYes (self-report)
WCSTHan et al. (2016)IGD-only = 60; IGD-MDD = 35; HC = 42IGD-only = 20.20 (3.20); IGD-MDD = 20.50 (3.30); HC = 20.2 (2.90)YIAT >50perseverative responses; perseverative errorsIGD + MDD group had more perseverative responses and perseverative errors compared to the IGD-only and HC groups;

2. No significant group difference in both perseverative responses and perseverative errors
IGD-MDD < HC; IGD-MDD < IGD-only; IGD-only = HCYes (DSM-IV; BDI, BAI)
WCSTKuo et al. (2018)IA child = 35; HC = 39IA = 11.60 (0.5); HC = 11.3(0.7)CIAS> 85%total errors; preservative responses; preservative errorsThe IA group had more total errors, preservative responses, and preservative errorsIA < HCNo
WCSTZhou et al. (2014)IA = 22; HC = 22IA = 28 (7); HC = 28 (7)YDQtotal errors; perseverative errors; number of categories completed
  1. The IAD group showed more total errors, perseverative errors;

  2. The IAD group completed fewer categories than the HC group

IA < HCYes (DSM-IV)
WCSTZhou et al. (2016)IAD = 23; HC = 23IA = 29 (7); HC = 28 (6)YDQtotal errors; perseverative errors; number of categories completed
  1. The IAD group showed more total errors, perseverative errors;

  2. The IAD group showed decreased number of categories completed than HC group

IA < HCYes (DSM-IV)
Intra-extra dimensional set shift
IEDBanca et al. (2016)IGD = 24; HC = 36N/ADSM-IVaextra-dimensional shift errorsNo significant group difference in extra-dimensional shift errorsIGD = HCNo
IEDChamberlain, Ioannidis, and Grant (2018)PUI-only = 18; comp PUI = 37; HC = 67PUI-only = 22.8 (3.0); comp PUI = 24.1 (3.7); HC = 22.4 (3.7)YDQ ≥4adjusted IED total errors
  1. No significant group difference in adjusted IED total errors

PUI-only = comp PIU = HCYes (MINI)
IEDChoi, Kim et al. (2014)IGD = 15; HC = 15IGD = 20.80 (5.09); HC = 25.33 (5.30)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥70IED total errors; IED total trials completedNo significant group differences in IED total errors and IED total trials completedIGD = HC
IEDChoi, Park et al. (2014)IA = 23; HC = 24IA = 23.22 (3.44); HC = 22.42 (2.47)YIAT ≥70IED total errorsNo significant group difference in IED total errorsIA = HCYes (DSM-IV)
IEDKim et al. (2017)IGD = 86; HC = 77IGD = 21.54 (6.91); HC = 22.84 (5.67)DSM-5 ≥ 5;IED total errors; IED total trials completedThe IGD group showed worse performance than the AUD group in IED total trials completed;

2. No significant group difference in IED total trials completed
IGD < AUD; IGD = HCYes (DSM-IV)
IEDLim et al. (2016)IGD = 44; HC = 40IGD = 19.16 (5.22); HC = 21.38 (6.31)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥70IED total errors; IED total trials completedNo significant group differences in IED total errors and total trials completedIGD = HCYes (DSM-IV, BDI, BAI)
Trail making task
TMTChoi, Kim et al. (2014)IGD = 15; HC = 15IGD = 20.80 (5.09); HC = 25.33 (5.30)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥70TMT-B completion timeNo significant group differences in TMT-B completion timeIGD = HC
TMTChoi, Park et al. (2014)IA = 23; HC = 24IA = 23.22 (3.44); HC = 22.42 (2.47)YIAT ≥70TMT-B completion timeNo significant group differences in TMT-B completion timeIA = HCYes (DSM-IV)
TMTTekin et al. (2018)IA = 30; HC = 29IA = 26.93; HC = 33.17YIAT ≥81TMT-B error number; TMT-B completion timeThe IA group showed higher TMT-B form time than the HC group;

2. The IA group performed worse on TMT-B error numbers than HCs
IA > HCYes (self-report)
TMTLim et al. (2016)IGD = 44; HC = 40IGD = 19.16 (5.22); HC = 21.38 (6.31)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥70TMT-B completion timeNo group differences in TMT-B completion time;

2. A short TMT-B completion time at baseline statistically predicted good prognosis for treatment of IGD
IGD = HCYes (DSM-IV, BDI, BAI)

Note: AUD = alcohol use disorder; BAI = Beck Anxiety Inventory; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; BSMAS = Bergen social media addiction scale; CIAS = Chen internet addiction scale; IA = internet addiction; IAD = internet addiction disorder; IED = intra-extra dimensional set shift; MDD = major depressive disorder; PUI = problematic usage of the internet; PSNSU = problematic social networking sites use; TMT = trail making task; YDQ = Young's diagnostic questionnaire; WCST = Wisconsin card sorting test; YIAT = Young's internet addiction test.

Note: a, criteria adapted from DSM-IV pathological gambling criteria.

Table 4.

Overview of included studies within the attentional bias/disengagement domain

TasksAuthor(s)Population (PUI/HC)Mean age (SD)DiagnosisDVsMajor findingsSummaryComorbidity control
Dot probe task
DPTHe, Zheng, Nie, and Zhou (2018)IA = 15; HC = 15IA = 20.80 (1.01); HC = 20.87 (1.06)YIAT ≥80RT; accuracyaThe IA group had shorter RTs in congruent conditions and longer RTs in incongruent conditions;

2. The IA group showed significantly longer RTs in incongruent vs congruent conditions
AB (RT): IA > HCNo
DPTJeromin, Nyenhuis, et al. (2016)IGD = 21; HC = 30IGD = 22.9 (2.1); HC = 24.5(3.2)CIUSRT; accuracyaSignificant interaction of group by picture-type in accuracy: IGD made more errors with computer-related pictures than with neutral pictures;

2. The IGD group had shorter RTs in general;

3. No significant group by picture-type interaction effect in RTs
AB (accuracy): IGD < HCNo
DPTJiang et al. (2017)OSA high = 27; OSA low = 28N/AOSA-high >62; OSA-low <46RTbNo significant main effects of group and congruent condition, nor interaction effect of group by congruent conditionOSA high = OSA lowYes (self-report)
DPTLiu et al. (2023)PSU = 22; HC = 25PSU = 20.72 (1.48); HC = 21.32 (2.28)SAS-SV > 33 for females; SAS-SV > 31 for malesfixation time; RTb
  1. First fixation duration on smartphone icon stimuli was significantly longer than that on neutral stimuli in the PSU group;

  2. no significant interaction effect of congruency by group.

PSU > HCNo
DPTLorenz et al. (2013)IGD = 8; HC = 9IGD = 25 (7.4); HC = 24.8 (6.9)ICD-10cRTbSignificant interaction effect of congruency by group: PCGPs, but not HCs, showed longer RTs in incongruent trails than congruent trailsAB: IGD > HC (RT)Yes (DSM-IV)
DPTNikolaidou, Fraser, and Hinvest (2019)PSNSU = 16; HC = 24PSNSU = 19.25 (1.39); HC = 20.92 (2.90)AEQ ≥4gazing dell timeThe PSNSU group spent more time looking at SNS pictures than control pictures;

2. The PSNSU group spent less time looking at control pictures compared to the HC group
AB: PSNSU > HCNo
DPTWang and Huang (2022)PIPU high = 40; PIPU low = 40PIPU = 19.80 (1.83); HC = 19.58 (1.72)Median of PIPUSengagement RT; disengagement RTdThe PIPU group showed enhanced engagement RTs for pornographic stimuli in short SOA;

2. HCs showed longer disengagement RTs for pornographic pictures than neutral ones in long SOA
engagement RT: PIPU > HC; disengagement RT: PIPU < HCYes (self-report)
DPTZhao et al. (2022)PSMU = 30; HC = 30PSMU = 20.07 (1.70); HC = 19.33 (1.40)BSMAS ≥24RTbA significant interaction effect of group by word type: the PSMU group displayed AB toward social media-related cues; the PSMU group reacted faster to the congruent condition than to the incongruent condition; the PSMU group responded faster to congruent trials than the HC group;

Whole sample, AB toward SM-related stimuli positively correlated with the severity of PSMU
AB: PSMU > HCYes (clinical interview)
DPTZhou, Zhou, Zhou, Shen, and Zhang (2022)IGD = 30; HC = 32IGD = 19.17 (1.26); HC = 19.03 (0.86)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥50RTd; accuracyThe IGD group showed higher attentional disengagement bias compared to HCs;

No significant group difference in terms of attentional engagement bias;

3. No significant group differences in terms of accuracy.
IGD > HC (RT disengage)No
Addiction Stroop task
StroopJeromin, Nyenhuis, et al. (2016)IGD = 21; HC = 30IGD = 22.90 (2.10); HC = 24.50 (3.20)CIUSRTe; accuracyeThe IGD group showed longer reaction times to computer-related words;

2. The IGD group made more errors with computer-related pictures than with neutral pictures
IGD > HC (RT)No
StroopJeromin, Rief, et al. (2016)IGD = 27; HC = 27IGD = 24.90 (7.40); HC = 31.20 (7.70)CIUS ≥29RTe; accuracyeNo significant group nor group-by-word-type interaction effects in RT and accuracy on the StroopIGD = HCNo
StroopJeromin, Rief, et al. (2016)IGD = 29; HC = 29IGD = 25.20 (5.30); HC = 23.50 (4.90)IGDQ ≥5;RTe; accuracyeAddiction Stroop: no significant group main effect nor group-by-congruence-condition interaction effects in RT and accuracy;

Classical Stroop: no significant group main effect nor group-by-congruence-condition interaction effects in RT and accuracy
IGD = HCNo
StroopMetcalf and Pammer (2011)IGD = 20; HC = 19IGD = 21.25 (2.53); HC = 22.53 (2.93)AEQ ≥4RTe; accuracyeA significant interaction between word type and group: (1) the IGD group had significant longer RTs to MMORPG and negative words compared to neutral words; (2) the HC group showed no difference in RTs between word typesIGD > HCNo
StroopZhang et al. (2016)IGD = 19; HC = 21IGD = 22.2 (3.1); HC = 22.8 (2.4)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥50RTe; accuracyeNo significant group main effect nor interaction effect were found in accuracy and RT;

2. Greater brain activity in the IPL, dlPFC and MOG for AB in IGD compared to HC groups
IGD = HCYes (self-report)
StroopZhao et al. (2022)PSMU = 30; HC = 30PSMU = 20.07 (1.70); HC = 19.33 (1.40)BSMAS ≥24RTeNo significant group main effect nor interaction effect in RTsPSMU = HCNo
StroopWang et al. (2018)IGD = 18; HC = 19IGD = 21.50 (2.01); HC = 22.26 (1.82)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT ≥50RTeNo significant interaction effect in group by word-type;

IGD subjects showed increased FC in the temporal gyrus, and reduced FC in the PCC and MFG
IGD = HCYes (MINI)
Addiction go/no-go task
Go/no-goLiu et al. (2014)IGD = 11; HC = 11IGD = 23.45 (2.34); HC = 22.45 (1.70)DCIA-Cerror rate of go trials; commission errorsfThe IGD group made more commission errors than the HC group in the game-cue distraction condition;

No significant group differences in the no distraction condition (with black background)
IGD > HC (errors)Yes (MINI)
Go/no-goGou, Yuan, Zhang, Tang, and Zhang (2023)PSMUS = 37; HC = 41PSMUS = 19.16 (0.83); HC = 19.34 (1.09)PSMUS >50error rate of go trials; commission errorsf
  1. The PSMU group made more commission errors than the HC group in addiction-relevant go/no-go task;

  2. The PSMU group made more commission errors in addiction-relevant go/no-go task than neutral go/no-go task;

  3. No significant group differences in neutral go/no-go task

PSMU > HC (errors)Yes (self-report)
Go/no-goYao et al. (2015)IGD = 34; HC = 32IGD = 22.29 (2.07); HC = 22.47 (2.08)CIAS ≥67error rate of go trials; commission errorsf; RTIGD subjects made more commission errors compared to HCsIGD > HC (errors)Yes (self-report)

Note. AB = attentional bias; AEQ = addiction engagement questionnaire; AICA = assessment of internet and computer game addiction; BSMAS = Bergen social media addiction scale; CIUS = compulsive internet use scale; DCIA-C = diagnostic criteria for internet addiction for college students; DPT = dot probe task; IPL = inferior parietal lobule; MFG = middle frontal gyrus; MMORPG = massively multiplayer online role-playing gamers; MOG = middle occipital gyrus; OSAS = online shopping addiction scale; OSVe-S = self-report questionnaire for internet addiction-related behavior (skala zum onlinesuchtverhalten bei erwachsenen); PCC = posterior cingulate cortex; PIPUS = problematic internet pornography use scale; PSMU = problematic social media use; PSMUS = Problematic Social Media Use Scale; PSU = Problematic Smartphone Use; RT = reaction time; SAS-SV = Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version; SM = social media; YIAT = Young's internet addiction test.

a, accuracy in congruent vs incongruent trials; b, RTs of incongruent trials – RTs of congruent trials; c, a criteria adapted from ICD-10 the diagnostic criteria of substance addiction; d, engagement RT = [RTs neutral – pornographic stimuli] in congruent trials, disengagement RT = [RTs pornographic – neutral stimuli] in incongruent trials; e, RT and accuracy in addiction vs control trials; f, commission errors, error rate of No-Go trials.

Table 5.

Overview of included studies within the habit learning domain

TasksAuthor(s)Population (PUI/HC)Mean age (SD)DiagnosisDVsMajor findingsSummaryComorbidity control
Fabulous fruit task
FFTZhou et al. (2018)IA = 21; HC = 23all = 18.73 (range 18–22)CIAS ≥64accuracy in the learning phase; accuracy in devalue testsThe IA group showed lower accuracy in the learning phase;

The IA group showed lower accuracy in both outcome devaluation and S–R habit tests compared to HCs
IA < HCYes (clinical interview)
FFTZhou et al. (2021)IGD = 25; HC = 25IGD = 22.03 (2.24); HC = 21.51 (2.10)DSM-5 ≥ 5; YIAT >50accuracy in the learning phase; accuracy in devalue testsThe IGD group showed lower accuracy rate in the incongruent condition (IC score) in the learning phase;

2. In the IGD group, the IC score was positively correlated with the rsFC of PN-MFG
IGD < HCYes (MINI)
Feedback learning task
FLTKim et al. (2017)IGD = 18; HC = 20IGD = 22.17 (2.00); HC = 21.20 (2.20)IGADS >20%; YIAT ≥50correct to stay rateIGD showed decreased correct-stay rate with symbolic feedbacks in loss conditionIGD < HCYes (self-report)

Note: FFT = Fabulous fruit task; IGADS = internet game addiction diagnostic scale; PN = pulvinar nucleus; rsFC = resting-state functional connectivity.

Study quality and risk of bias were assessed using the Newcastle-Ottawa-Scale (NOS; http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp) checklist for case-control studies (Wells et al., 2000; Zeng et al., 2015). The NOS makes judgement of identified studies on three criteria: the selection (max. 4 points), the comparability (max. 2 points), and the exposure ascertainment (max. 3 points), resulting in a quality score ranging from 0 to 9. Two raters (the first and second authors) assessed each study independently and then discussed together to resolve discrepancies. Most studies performed well on the checklist items, except for the item of blinded exposure assessment, as almost all studies omitted to report this. Moreover, blinding is not applicable for most case-control studies. Thirty-one of the thirty-four assessed studies had scores between 7 and 9, and the rest three studies scored 5–6 (Table S1 in the Supplementary Materials).

Ethics

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Psychology, Sun Yat-sen University.

Results

Identified studies

The initial search identified 2,114 unique studies, of which 34 studies representing 35 unique samples met inclusion criteria. The PRISMA flow diagram illustrates the study selection process (Fig. 1). Study descriptions and behavioral findings are summarized in Tables 25. The 34 studies included 41 task results (n = 1,161 individuals with PUI and n = 1,267 healthy control participants). In the following sections, we describe commonly used tasks and main findings related to each within the four-component framework for compulsivity (Tables 25; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

Structure of compulsivity and compulsive behaviors in PUI. Schematic representation of compulsive online behaviors in PUI and the corresponding relationships with the four-domain compulsivity framework. Individuals with PUI have consistently shown a greater attentional bias to addition-related stimuli, whereas set-shifting seems to be relatively intact. As few studies have shown deficits in contingency-related cognitive flexibility and habit learning, more studies are still required. The red, yellow, or green frame indicates that the corresponding domain is altered, ambiguous, or intact in PUI, respectively. The total number of results from the studies and the percentage of significant results for each domain are shown in the margin

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 13, 2; 10.1556/2006.2024.00023

Neurocognition of compulsivity

Cognitive flexibility

Contingency-related cognitive flexibility describes the learning process of a rule and the adaptation of behavior after changes of the rule according to the feedback (e.g., devaluation or degradation of an outcome) (Fineberg et al., 2014). If individuals are not sensitive to outcomes associated with stimulus-response or to stimulus-outcome contingencies, they would show rigid adherence (perseveration) to rules or strategies when they change in these tasks. Two tasks were frequently used to measure this component: the probabilistic reversal learning task and contingency learning task.

Probabilistic reversal learning task

The probabilistic reversal learning task (PRLT) involves rule acquisition and rule reversal, which usually refers to reward (and loss) schedules changes (Banca, Harrison, & Voon, 2016; Fineberg et al., 2014). This task typically includes at least two phases with opposite rules, which are separated by a reversal point (see Box 1A in the Supplementary Information). For example, one stimulus in the learning phase is more advantageous (e.g., 80% winning probability) than the other one (e.g., 20% winning probability). Participants need to learn which stimulus is better by trial and error. In the reversal phase, the stimulus-reward mapping is switched. Participants should therefore adjust their behavior according to rule/environmental changes. Perseveration can be reflected in the number/ratio of correct choices after a rule change. Persevering with a response that was once rewarded but is later associated with negative consequences may reflect a lack of flexibility in learning and may thus reflect compulsive behavior (Fineberg et al., 2014).

Only two studies were identified that investigated cognitive flexibility with the PRLT in individuals with IGD (Table 2). One study found that the IGD group required more trials to learn the changed rule in the reversal phase, suggesting that individuals with IGD were likely to stick to a previously established strategy in pursuit of a reward, even when such strategy is no longer optimal due to the environmental changes (Banca et al., 2016). Importantly, the IGD group showed a similar performance to the HC group in the acquisition phase, suggesting that the perseveration observed in the reversal phase may not be driven by reward-related learning dysfunction. The other recent study also reported higher perseverative errors in the PUI group, and perseverative errors and self-reported compulsivity may significantly predict the likelihood of experiencing PUI (Raj, Segrave, Verdéjo-Garcia, & Yücel, 2023).

A related study applied a reward-related learning task with the rule change in between (Lei et al., 2022). However, instead of directly switching the rule, this task included a deterministic learning phase and a probabilistic test (contingency degradation) phase to create and measure prediction errors. The study reported no significant group differences in response accuracy (the number of correct choices) in the rule-changed phase. Despite null results at the behavioral level, this study involved fMRI scanning and identified blunted reward prediction-error signals in the caudate, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), brain regions involved in reward processing and decision-making (Lei et al., 2022).

Task/attentional set-shifting

Task set-shifting involves the ability to frequently switch among settings of a task. This requires discrimination, attention maintenance and shifting on various dimensions of stimuli, and thus compulsivity may be linked to set-shifting. Three tasks were included in this domain: the Wisconsin card sorting, intra-extra dimensional set-shifting, and trail making tasks.

Wisconsin card sorting test

The Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST) is widely applied to assess set-shifting depending on rule changes (Heaton, 1993). In this task (see Box 1B in the Supplementary Materials), individuals sort cards according to one of the categorical rules (color, number, or form), with correct responses learned from the feedback provided after each response. After a certain number of correct responses, the rule changes and participants need to shift to a new approach for correctly sorting. The indices related to compulsivity include the number of classifications completed and the number of perseverative errors.

Six studies have assessed set-shifting in individuals with PUI using the WCST (Table 3). Three studies reported more perseverative errors on the WCST in the PUI group (Kuo et al., 2018; Zhou et al., 2014, 2016). One study in IGD participants with and without major depressive disorder (MDD) found that only the IGD-MDD group showed more perseverative errors compared to pure IGD and HC groups (Han, Kim, Bae, Renshaw, & Anderson, 2016). Another study involving people with problematic use of social media did not show significant between-group differences (Aydin, Obuća, Boz, & Ünal-Aydın, 2020).

Intra-extra dimensional set-shift

The intra-extra dimensional set-shifting (IED) task has also been used to examine set-shifting (Robbins et al., 1998). In the task, two stimuli (a correct and an incorrect one) are presented, and participants are asked to discriminate the correct one through the feedback. After a fixed number of correct trials, the rule is changed: the stimuli initially are composed of one dimension and the changes are intra-dimensional (i.e., from one shape to another shape). After a certain number of trials, the stimuli are composed of two dimensions (i.e., shapes and lines) and changes are extra-dimensional that requires shifting of attention to a previously irrelevant dimension (i.e., from shapes to lines). Test indices of interest include the number of stages completed and the total number of errors (the number of intra-dimensional errors and extra-dimensional errors). Most studies reported here consistently used the total number of errors. In addition, the IED task could distinguish between problems with set-shifting due to distraction (intra-dimensional set-shift) and problems with shifting from a previously learned rule (extra-dimensional set-shift) (Jazbec et al., 2007).

Six studies have examined set-shifting using the IED. None have found significant group differences between individuals with and without PUI (Table 3).

Trail making task

The trail making task (TMT) measures set-shifting between letters and numbers (Reitan & Reitan, 1992). This task instructs participants to connect a sequence of consecutive targets (e.g., letters or numbers) as quickly and accurately as possible. It includes two parts: in the first part (TMT-A), all targets are numbers, and participants need to connect the numbers in sequential order; in the second part (TMT-B), both letters and numbers are targets, and participants are asked to sequentially connect those in alternating order (e.g., 1, A, 2, B, etc.). This requires inhibition of the automatic inclination to order numbers or letters separately (e.g., 1, 2, 3, or A, B, C). Test indices include errors and completion time of the TMT-B after controlling for individual differences in completion the TMT-A.

Only one of the four studies that applied the TMT found that the PUI group took more time to complete the TMT-B trials and showed more TMT-B errors compared to the healthy control (HC) group (Tekin, Yetkin, Adiguzel, & Akman, 2018). In addition, one study that examined intervention effects for IGD reported that shorter TMT-B completion time at baseline statistically predicted better treatment response, although no baseline TMT-B between-group differences were observed (Table 3).

Attentional bias/disengagement

Attentional bias refers to individuals' inclination to preferentially engage with disorder-relevant versus disorder-irrelevant stimuli. Attentional disengagement relates to difficulties shifting focus away from disorder-relevant stimuli (Fineberg et al., 2014). Compulsivity in this domain is thus defined by participants' difficulties with inhibiting prepotent or rigid responses related to a specific disorder, which may reflect hyper-sensitivity to disorder-relevant stimuli and difficulties in mental set-shifting in such situations. Features of attentional bias, set-shifting and cognitive flexibility may overlap (Izquierdo, Brigman, Radke, Rudebeck, & Holmes, 2017; van Timmeren et al., 2018). To further clarify boundaries between these components, we focused attentional bias only in disorder-relevant situations to emphasize the context-specific aspect of compulsivity (Fineberg et al., 2014). Therefore, paradigms related to attentional bias include the dot-probe, addiction-Stroop, and addiction go/no-go tasks.

Dot-probe task

The dot-probe task, which measures preferential attention toward disorder-relevant stimuli, is commonly used in addiction studies. This task simultaneously presents an addiction-related stimulus and a non-addiction stimulus (e.g., pictures or words) on both sides of a fixation (see Box 1C in the Supplementary Information). The stimuli disappear after a fixed duration and are followed by a probe item (e.g., a dot, an asterisk, etc.) presenting either on the addiction-stimulus side (congruent condition) or on the control-stimulus side (incongruent condition). Participants are asked to indicate the probe location as quickly as possible, and the reaction time (RT) is measured. The response latency to the probe will be reduced when it appears in an attended rather than unattended location (Mogg & Bradley, 2005). Accordingly, individual's prepotent attention allocation to one of the two lateralized cues will facilitate the response to the probe at the congruent location. By comparing the averaged reaction times for the congruent and incongruent conditions, the relatively faster responses to the former are interpreted as an attentional bias. Two types of intervals (i.e., stimulus onset asynchrony; SOA) between the presentation of the stimulus and the probe-dot have been used in previous studies. With short exposure time (short SOA), participants' attention orients to certain stimuli unconsciously, while at longer exposure time (long SOA), this orientation may be linked to conscious processing.

A total of nine PUI studies using this task were found, eight of which reported significant attention bias towards internet-related stimuli in individuals with PUI (Table 4). The only study that found no significant attention-bias effect focused on online shopping addiction (Jiang, Zhao, & Li, 2017). Besides, another two studies conceptualized PUI as a continuum and reported significantly positive correlations between internet use symptom severity scores and attention bias (Pekal, Laier, Snagowski, Stark, & Brand, 2018; van Holst et al., 2012).

Addiction-Stroop task

The Stroop task (Stroop, 1935) is a classic neuropsychological task that requires selective attention, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control. Since the performance on the classic Stroop task depends on the meaning of stimuli, some researchers have modified the task by using addiction-related words and neutral words to assess addiction-specific concerns activated by the meaning of the words (Faunce & Job, 2000). In the addiction Stroop task, individuals with addictive disorders tend to name the colors of addiction-related words more slowly (i.e., larger interference effect). The interference score is reflected by the increased RT difference between the incongruent and congruent trials, as attention bias to addiction-related stimuli may lead to failures to inhibit prepotent responses.

Of the six articles (seven independent results) that used the addiction Stroop task (Table 4), two found significant impairments in individuals with PUI compared to healthy controls (HCs), whereas the rest five studies did not (Jeromin, Rief, & Barke, 2016; Wang et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2016; Zhao et al., 2022). Regarding the two studies with significant results, longer reaction times to game-relevant words in individuals with IGD were identified (Jeromin, Nyenhuis, & Barke, 2016; Metcalf & Pammer, 2011). In addition, one other study found longer reaction times in individuals with higher tendency of online shopping addiction (OSA) compared to participants with low tendency of OSA (Jiang et al., 2017). Additionally, one other study reported significant correlations between participants' game addiction scores and numbers of errors for game-related words (van Holst et al., 2012). Taken together, these findings suggest an attentional bias relating to automatic addiction-related meaning processing in people with IGD.

Other tasks of attentional bias

The addiction go/no-go task has also been used to investigate attentional bias (Table 4). Differing from the classic go/no-go task, the modified versions include addiction-related and neutral stimuli either as targets or distractors. Both versions use response accuracy in go and no-go conditions and reaction times in go condition as main outcomes. One identified study used gaming pictures as go targets and neutral pictures as no-go targets and identified more errors on no-go trials in the IGD group when compared with the HC group (Yao et al., 2015). One study used social media icons as no-go targets and neutral pictures as go targets and reported more errors on no-go trials in the group with problematic use of social media compared to that without. Another study used game-related pictures as background distractors and neutral polygons as go/no-go targets. This study showed that the IGD group made more commission errors than the control group in the gaming distracting version but not in the classic go/no-go version (Liu et al., 2014). The results suggest a preponderance of addictive cues in individuals with PUI, leading to greater difficulties with attentional disengagement and inhibitory control.

Habit learning

Habit learning refers to the decreased sensitivity to outcomes and the automation of actions through over-training. According to the double-system theory, an instrumental learning process is jointly controlled by goal-directed and habitual systems (Balleine & Dickinson, 1998). The goal-directed system guides action performance according to the context and outcomes, whereas the habitual system renders behavior automatic and actions insensitive to the outcome, over-relying on stimulus-response contingencies. Therefore, compulsive behaviors may be attributed to either impaired goal-directed control or an over-reliance on the habit system. The fabulous fruit task and two-stage task are commonly used to assess habit control in instrumental learning processes (Daw et al., 2011; de Wit, Corlett, Aitken, Dickinson, & Fletcher, 2009).

Fabulous fruit task

The fabulous fruit task measures the balance between goal-directed and habitual learning via the manipulation of outcome devaluation. The task consists of a discrimination learning phase and two outcome devaluation test phases (see Box 1D in the Supplementary Information). In the learning phase, when a particular fruit stimulus (S) is presented, the correct response (R) will be rewarded with a particular outcome (O), so that a S-R-O association is established. This phase relies on both the goal-directed and habit learning systems.

The learning phase is followed by two phases of devalue tests. In the first testing phase, outcomes are presented pair-wised with one outcome devalued (marked with a red cross) to indicate that the specific outcome is no longer worth points, and only the valued outcome requires a correct response. The outcome-devaluation test is used to measure goal-directed control by assessing the participant's response to action-outcome associations. The second testing phase is the slips-of-action test session, where subjects are asked to press the correct key when a stimulus signals the still-valuable outcome and to refrain from responding when its outcome has been devalued. Therefore, the slips-of-action test can be used to assess the habitual system (Gillan et al., 2011).

Two studies were identified using the fabulous fruit task to assess habit learning among individuals with PUI (Table 5). One showed that individuals with PUI performed worse in learning S-R-O associations (Zhou, Wang, Zhang, Li, & Nie, 2018). Importantly, the study identified that PUI participants had lower accuracy in the outcome-devaluation test (i.e., first testing phase) and higher response frequency to devalue-paired stimuli in the slips-of-action test (i.e., second testing phase), which reflected the greater reliance on the habitual system and impaired goal-directed control in people with PUI (Zhou et al., 2018). Similarly, the other study found that the IGD group showed deficits in the incongruent condition during the learning phase (Zhou et al., 2021). However, the results of the two post-learning tests were not reported. These findings suggest that individuals with IGD may over-rely on habitual system and overlooked outcomes (deficits in goal-directed control). In addition, the IGD group showed significantly increased functional connectivity between the pulvinar nucleus and medial frontal gyrus, the connectivity of which was positively correlated with worse performance in the incongruent S-R-O learning condition (Zhou et al., 2021).

Other tasks of habit learning

Except for the above-mentioned two tasks, researchers have also used the feedback learning task (deterministic learning task) to investigate S-R-O instrumental association learning processes in individuals with IGD (Kim & Kang, 2018). This task requires participants to learn stimulus-response associations depending on the feedback in a trial-and-error fashion. There are three learning conditions (i.e., gain, loss, and neutral conditions) with each assigned to a positive and negative feedback (e.g., a monetary reward, non-monetary reward, a monetary penalty, non-monetary penalty). A significant group difference was found in the loss condition, such that the IGD patients showed lower correct-stay rates after the symbolic positive feedback, which suggested impaired S-R-O learning from abstract feedback in the loss condition. In addition, participants performed the learning task during MRI scanning: reduced correct-to-stay rate in the gain condition in IGD individuals was associated with stronger connectivity between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens.

Compulsivity-related alterations between IGD and other subtypes of PUI

Among the 41 task results, 23 results focused on IGD participants and 11 showed significant group differences. The 11 remaining task results did not provide the information related to sub-types of PUI, and 7 reported significant results. There were 8 task results focusing on sub-types of PUI apart from IGD (e.g., problematic use of social media), and 5 reported significant group differences between people with sub-types of PUI and HCs.

Discussion

We systematically reviewed the literature that tested compulsivity-related neuropsychological functioning and behaviors in PUI based on a four-components framework. We found that most studies focused on IGD, and other types of PUI have received considerably less attention. Regarding different components of compulsivity, individuals with PUI showed more consistent deficits on tasks measuring attentional bias to addiction-related stimuli, whereas findings are rather mixed for those measuring set-shifting in a neutral context. We also found some supporting evidence for deficits in cognitive flexibility and habit learning in PUI, although the number of studies for these two components may be insufficient to draw reliable conclusions. We will discuss the findings for each compulsivity component and related perspectives below.

Studies examining contingency-related cognitive flexibility are scarce and have shown mixed results. Results from the studies using the PRLT reveal significant behavioral inflexibility (perseveration) in individuals with PUI (Banca et al., 2016; Raj et al., 2023). However, such differences seem to be specific to the adaptation after the reversal instead of the rule change in general, since another study using a probability learning task with the contingency degradation (e.g., from a deterministic learning phase to a probabilistic test phase) showed similarly high accuracy in both IGD and HC groups (Lei et al., 2022). Thus, it remains an open question whether PUI is characterized by impaired inhibition to learned rules prior to reversal. One factor that possibly obscures these results is the diversity in paradigm and outcome parameters between the studies. In addition to the behavioral findings, individuals with IGD also showed blunted positive reward-prediction-error signal in brain regions involved in the reward system (e.g., caudate, OFC) (Lei et al., 2022), which may reflect a link between cognitive inflexibility and impairment in reward value updating for unexpected rewards, as often reported in drug addictions (Parvaz et al., 2015; Tanabe et al., 2013).

Similar to the contingency-related cognitive flexibility domain, few experimental studies to date have examined habit learning in PUI (Kim & Kang, 2018; Zhou et al., 2018, 2021), despite its crucial role in establishing the compulsive behavior and its close association with addictions (Everitt & Robbins, 2016). Specifically, two studies using the fabulous fruit task revealed deficits in learning S-R-O associations in individuals with PUI, especially in the incongruent condition. These findings may reflect a greater reliance on habitual learning over goal-directed control in PUI populations (Zhou et al., 2018, 2021), which are consistent with previous evidence in people with alcohol (Sjoerds et al., 2013) and cocaine use disorders (Ersche et al., 2016). Importantly, in line with theoretical proposal that addictions (including PUI) may evolve from impulsive to compulsive processes (Brand et al., 2019), the devaluation effect in the S–R habit test also negatively correlated with the severity of PUI, indicating that participants with more severe PUI were less sensitive to value changes related to their actions (Zhou et al., 2018). Besides the fabulous fruit task, previous studies using the two-stage reinforcement learning task also showed decreased goal-directed control across various addictions (e.g., methamphetamine use, alcohol use, and gambling disorders) (Sebold et al., 2014; Voon et al., 2015; Wyckmans et al., 2019). By distinguishing model-based (goal-directed) and model-free (habitual) learning processes using a computational modeling approach, analyses of data from the task can provide insight into the balance between the two learning process systems. However, to the best of our knowledge, the two-stage task has not been used in PUI studies yet, and more evidence is needed to understand relationships between habit learning and PUI.

Unlike cognitive flexibility and habit learning, set-shifting has been widely examined in individuals with PUI, yet only a small proportion of related studies (5 out of 15 studies) showed significant between-group differences. The WCST reported relatively consistent findings of poor performance for individuals with PUI (Han et al., 2016; Kuo et al., 2018; Zhou et al., 2014, 2016). In contrast, among the IED and TMT studies, only one using the TMT identified group differences in task performance (Tekin et al., 2018). The unambiguous but opposite results suggest task specificity in assessing PUI individual's set-shifting. Similar task-dependent deficits have been also reported in systematic reviews of gambling and binge eating disorders (Carr et al., 2021; van Timmeren et al., 2018). A key difference between these tasks is that the WCST includes multiple dimensions (including differences in color, shape, and quantity) that participants needed to keep in working memory (Nyhus & Barceló, 2009), whereas the IED and TMT only require participants to operate in 1–2 dimensions. Therefore, a possible explanation is that the between-group difference observed in the WCST may relate to task difficulty (e.g., working memory load) that reflect cognitive capacity rather than set shifting (Choi, Kim et al., 2014; Choi, Park et al., 2014; Roberts, Tchanturia, Stahl, Southgate, & Treasure, 2007). However, this hypothesis remains to be tested by a new paradigm that experimentally manipulates working memory load during set-shifting.

Finally, regarding the attentional bias/disengagement domain, almost all studies using dot-probe or addiction go/no-go tasks identified poor task performance in individuals with PUI compared to HCs. The results on the addiction Stroop task are more mixed (Jeromin, Nyenhuis, et al., 2016; Jeromin, Rief, et al., 2016; Metcalf & Pammer, 2011). These findings are consistent with research in gambling disorder (van Timmeren et al., 2018). A possibility may be that most of the dot-probe and addiction go/no-go tasks used addiction-related images, which may induce larger attentional biases compared to the addiction-related words commonly used in the addiction Stroop task (Fineberg et al., 2014; van Holst et al., 2012). Furthermore, the go/no-go paradigm with addiction-related cues, as an approach to measure stimulus-specific inhibitory control, is widely used in studying behavioral addiction disorders (Antons, Müller, Neumann, Müller, & Steins-Loeber, 2023). The increased commission errors identified in this review consistently confirmed the reduction of inhibitory control and the automatic approach tendency cued by internet stimuli, which may reflect the underlying mechanism of repetitive online behaviors in PUI subtypes. The enhanced attentional bias/disengagement to internet-related cues may indicate the underlying mechanisms of repetitive and continuous online activities use, suggesting hooked attention to addictive stimuli and reduced inhibitory control in the online environment (He, Pan, Nie, Zheng, & Chen, 2021; Ioannidis et al., 2019). Overall, the comparable compulsive attention tendency in individuals with behavioral addictions (e.g., IGD, gambling disorder) may explain the repetitive addictive behavior that is engaged by disorder-relevant information.

Taken together, the four-component framework provides insights into the multifaceted feature of compulsivity and helps to explain seemingly inconsistent findings in previous studies of PUI. Overall, previous evidence suggests that individuals with PUI are impaired in addiction-related attentional bias but not set shifting in the non-addictive context. The number of studies examining cognitive flexibility and habit learning are limited, and more evidence is needed to establish more reliable conclusions. Attentional bias and habit learning rely more on stimuli and established, automatic responses (Fineberg et al., 2014). These two components may serve as early components of compulsivity, reflecting a relatively automatic and less goal-directed reaction. Online activities that are replete with entertaining and self-referential information may trigger individuals to click and refresh habitually and continuously with less intention, especially for individuals with PUI (Kuss & Griffiths, 2017). Individuals with PUI may exhibit a cruise mode of online behavior, automatically and stereotypically using internet activity without definite goals. On the other hand, contingency-related cognitive flexibility and set-shifting typically require more reflection and algorithms (Fineberg et al., 2014; Izquierdo et al., 2017), and thus may serve as late components of compulsivity depending on the cognitive control. Repeated online behaviors despite negative consequences in individuals with PUI may reflect rigid and inflexible tendencies associated with deficits in executive functioning. Specifically, cognitive inflexibility may be reflected in the repeated and rigid user-device interaction (e.g., notification-touching, posting-liking) despite diminishing positive feedback or even negative consequences. The susceptibility of the early and late components to PUI may differ and may be a focus of further study.

The findings also shed new light on the development of treatments. For example, since PUI is consistently associated with attentional bias towards addiction-relevant stimuli, approach bias modification treatments could be effective in alleviating PUI. Such hypothesis has already received some supportive evidence in preliminary studies, with approach bias modification significantly decreasing IGD severity and related characteristics (e.g., gaming intention and craving level) (He et al., 2021; Rabinovitz & Nagar, 2015). As approach biases have been linked to other online behaviors (e.g., severity of problematic pornography use in both young men and women) (Sklenarik et al., 2019, 2020), such approaches warrant investigation in a broad range of PUI types. Moreover, a systematic review on neuropsychological interventions in other addictive disorders highlighted the efficacy of goal-management training and contingency management, which are closely related to attentional bias inhibition training and goal monitoring (Verdejo-García, Alcázar-Córcoles, & Albein-Urios, 2019). Thus, specific neuropsychological modification approaches may also be applied to PUI intervention studies in the future. Based on the neuropsychological and neuroanatomical and neurochemical mechanisms that contribute to compulsive responses, a four-dimensional structure has been proposed (Fineberg et al., 2014). Previous systematic reviews and meta-analytic studies have demonstrated the ability of this framework to capture different aspects of compulsive impairments in gambling disorder (van Timmeren et al., 2018), binge eating disorder (Carr et al., 2021), obsessive compulsive disorder and other conditions (Chamberlain, Solly, Hook, Vaghi, & Robbins, 2021; Fineberg et al., 2018). Moreover, in the current study, we found that individuals with PUI seemed to be impaired in attentional bias but relatively intact in set shifting, suggesting these two domains may reflect distinct aspects of compulsivity. Taken together, these studies suggest that the using four-domain framework may help promote understanding of the multidimensionality of compulsivity and provide a more precise picture of compulsivity-related concerns regarding PUI.

The current study has some limitations that should be considered in future studies. First, compulsivity is a multifaceted construct (Fineberg et al., 2014). However, most studies only focused on one specific domain, making it difficult to integrate seemingly inconsistent findings from different studies. To provide a more comprehensive picture of the role of compulsivity in PUI, studies examining the different components in the same sample are highly recommended. Furthermore, the ability of the neurocognitive tests to capture fully compulsivity was not verified. On the one hand, few empirical studies identified in the review provided evidence about the relationship between task performance and self-reported compulsivity or related neural underpinnings. On the other hand, as compulsivity was the primary interest of the review, only the convergent effect of the tasks in relation to compulsivity was assessed. We did not assess the non-compulsive deficits in PUI that may be captured by tasks (e.g., assessing executive functioning, impulsivity, or distress). Thus, future studies may investigate relationships between task-assessed compulsivity and with neurocognition, neural circuitry and self-reported compulsivity in both PUI and healthy populations. Second, different parameters (e.g., stimulus types) and dependent variables (e.g., reaction times and accuracy) of a specific paradigm were used in previous studies, making it challenging to compare findings across studies as well as between different PUI subtypes. Moreover, false positive evidence may emerge because of selective reporting. Thus, studies that report all core indexes related to compulsive components regardless of the significance and that apply multiple comparison correction should be encouraged. Third, studies have used different scales or criteria to diagnose PUI and some studies have used assessment criteria without validation provided. Such usage may impact the findings related to samples recruited as cases and controls and influence prevalence estimates, especially for generalized internet addiction (Pan, Chiu, & Lin, 2020). Taken together, this situation may increase the risk of study bias. One example involves the DSM-5 9-item criteria for IGD which has shown sound psychometric properties and has been widely used for making IGD diagnoses (Pan et al., 2020; Przybylski, Weinstein, & Murayama, 2017). Future studies should further validate different assessment approaches for other subtypes of PUI and investigate relationships with neurocognitive performance based on agreed-upon threshold, particularly threshold based on diagnostic criteria.

Conclusions

To summarize, we examined four neuropsychological domains of compulsivity in PUI. Behavioral tasks referring to contingency-related cognitive flexibility, set-shifting, attentional bias and habit learning were retained. People with PUI demonstrated impairments in attentional bias but were largely intact in set-shifting. Relatively consistent deficits in the domains of cognitive flexibility and habit learning were identified, but more evidence is still required. The results confirmed that individuals with PUI, to some extent, are characterized by compulsivity-related cognitive deficits. In addition, the evidence in different PUI sub-types was not evenly distributed. Therefore, further investigation is needed to distinguish domain-related compulsivity deficits in sub-types of PUI, which may elaborate our understanding of compulsive behaviors between PUI sub-types and help to develop specific cognitive modification treatments.

Funding sources

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China [No. 32200910, No. 32171083 and No. 32371142] and the Humanities and Social Sciences Research Project from the Ministry of Education of China (No. 22YJC190016). Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (No. CNLZD1903) and 111 Project [BP0719032]. Marc Potenza discloses that he has consulted for Opiant Therapeutics, Game Day Data, and Baria-Tek; been involved in a patent application with Yale University and Novartis; received research support from the Mohegan Sun Casino, Children and Screens and the Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling; and consulted for legal, non-profit and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control, internet use and addictions. The other authors have no conflicts of interest with respect to the content of this work.

Authors' contribution

All authors contributed to all parts of the manuscript. L.L.: Conceptualization; data curation; writing – original draft; investigation; project administration; visualization. Y.W.Y.: Conceptualization; data curation; writing – review & editing; investigation; validation. X.Y.F.: Supervision; writing – review & editing. L.X.X.: Data curation; writing – review & editing. M.J.H.: Data curation. J.T.Z.: Conceptualization; supervision; funding acquisition; writing – review & editing. M.N.P.: Conceptualization; supervision; writing – review & editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest with respect to the content of this work. Marc Potenza discloses that he has consulted for Opiant Therapeutics, Game Day Data, Baria-Tek, and the Addiction Policy Forum; been involved in a patent application with Yale University and Novartis; received research support from the Mohegan Sun Casino, Children and Screens and the Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling; and consulted for legal and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control, internet use and addictions. Marc Potenza is an associate editor to the Journal of Behavioral Addictions. The other authors report no disclosures.

Supplementary material

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at doi: https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2024.00023.

References

  • Antons, S., Müller, S. M., Neumann, P., Müller, A., & Steins-Loeber, S. (2023). Stimuli-specific inhibitory control in disorders due to addictive behaviours: A review of current evidence and discussion of methodological challenges. Current Addiction Reports, 10(4), 749769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-023-00517-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aydin, O., Obuća, F., Boz, C., & Ünal-Aydın, P. (2020). Associations between executive functions and problematic social networking sites use. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 42(6), 634645. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2020.1798358.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Balleine, B. W., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental action: Contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology, 37(4–5), 407419. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Banca, P., Harrison, N. A., & Voon, V. (2016). Compulsivity across the pathological misuse of drug and non-drug rewards. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carr, M. M., Wiedemann, A. A., Macdonald-Gagnon, G., & Potenza, M. N. (2021). Impulsivity and compulsivity in binge eating disorder: A systematic review of behavioral studies. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 110, 110318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110318.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Castro-Calvo, J., King, D. L., Stein, D. J., Brand, M., Carmi, L., Chamberlain, S. R., … Billieux, J. (2021). Expert appraisal of criteria for assessing gaming disorder: An international Delphi study. Addiction, 116(9), 24632475. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15411.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chamberlain, S. R., Ioannidis, K., & Grant, J. E. (2018). The impact of comorbid impulsive/compulsive disorders in problematic Internet use. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 269275. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.30.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chamberlain, S. R., Solly, J. E., Hook, R. W., Vaghi, M. M., & Robbins, T. W. (2021). Cognitive inflexibility in OCD and related disorders. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 49, 125145. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2020_198.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, S. -W., Kim, H. S., Kim, G. -Y., Jeon, Y., Park, S. M., Lee, J. -Y., … Kim, D. -J. (2014a). Similarities and differences among internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3(4), 246253. https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.3.2014.4.6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, J. -S., Park, S. M., Roh, M. -S., Lee, J. -Y., Park, C. -B., Hwang, J. Y., … Jung, H. Y. (2014b). Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in Internet addiction. Psychiatry Research, 215(2), 424428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.001.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron, 69(6), 12041215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de Wit, S., Corlett, P. R., Aitken, M. R., Dickinson, A., & Fletcher, P. C. (2009). Differential engagement of the ventromedial prefrontal Ccortex by goal-directed and habitual behavior toward food pictures in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(36), 1133011338. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1639-09.2009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demetrovics, Z., van den Brink, W., Paksi, B., Horvath, Z., & Maraz, A. (2022). Relating compulsivity and impulsivity with severity of behavioral addictions: A dynamic interpretation of large-scale cross-sectional findings. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.831992.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ersche, K. D., Gillan, C. M., Jones, P. S., Williams, G. B., Ward, L. H. E., Luijten, M., … Robbins, T. W. (2016). Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science, 352(6292), 14681471. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf3700.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2016). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 2350. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faunce, G. J., & Job, R. F. (2000). The Stroop colour-naming task and addictive behaviour: Some recommendations. Addiction, 95(9), 14381442. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.959143818.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97, 120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Chamberlain, S. R., Goudriaan, A. E., Stein, D. J., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Gillan, C. M., … Potenza, M. N. (2014). New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums, 19(1), 6989. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852913000801.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Demetrovics, Z., Stein, D. J., Ioannidis, K., Potenza, M. N., Grünblatt, E., … Chamberlain, S. R. (2018). Manifesto for a European research network into problematic usage of the internet. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 28(11), 12321246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.08.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Menchón, J. M., Hall, N., Dell’Osso, B., Brand, M., Potenza, M. N., … Zohar, J. (2022). Advances in problematic usage of the internet research – A narrative review by experts from the European network for problematic usage of the internet. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 118, 152346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2022.152346.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Firoozabadi, A., Razavian, Y., Saleh, S., & Hosseini, S. R. (2023). The comparison of neurocognitive functions between internet-addicted, methamphetamine users, and healthy participants. Applied Neuropsychology-Adult. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2023.2224478.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & de Wit, S. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gou, S. -Y., Yuan, R. -J., Zhang, W. -Q., Tang, Y. -L., & Zhang, W. -Z. (2023). Problematic social media use in youths cause response inhibition impairment. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05425-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Han, D. H., Kim, S. M., Bae, S., Renshaw, P. F., & Anderson, J. S. (2016). A failure of suppression within the default mode network in depressed adolescents with compulsive internet game play. Journal of Affective Disorders, 194, 5764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.01.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • He, J., Pan, T., Nie, Y., Zheng, Y., & Chen, S. (2021). Behavioral modification decreases approach bias in young adults with internet gaming disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 113, 106686. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106686.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • He, J., Zheng, Y., Nie, Y. F., & Zhou, Z. -K. (2018). Automatic detection advantage of network information among Internet addicts: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Scientific Reports, 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25442-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heaton, R. K. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test: computer version 2 (pp. 14). Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

  • Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ioannidis, K., Hook, R., Goudriaan, A. E., Vlies, S., Fineberg, N. A., Grant, J. E., & Chamberlain, S. R. (2019). Cognitive deficits in problematic internet use: Meta-analysis of 40 studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 215(5), 18. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Izquierdo, A., Brigman, J. L., Radke, A. K., Rudebeck, P. H., & Holmes, A. (2017). The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective. Neuroscience, 345, 1226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Izquierdo, A., & Jentsch, J. D. (2012). Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 607620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jazbec, S., Pantelis, C., Robbins, T., Weickert, T., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2007). Intra-dimensional/extra-dimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia: Impact of distractors. Schizophrenia Research, 89, 339349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeromin, F., Nyenhuis, N., & Barke, A. (2016). Attentional bias in excessive Internet gamers: Experimental investigations using an addiction Stroop and a visual probe. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(1), 3240. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.012.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeromin, F., Rief, W., & Barke, A. (2016). Using two web-based addiction Stroops to measure the attentional bias in adults with Internet Gaming Disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(4), 666673. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.075.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jiang, Z. -C., Zhao, X. -X., & Li, C. -C. (2017). Self-control predicts attentional bias assessed by online shopping-related Stroop in high online shopping addiction tendency college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 75, 1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.02.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, J., & Kang, E. (2018). Internet game overuse is associated with an alteration of fronto-striatal functional connectivity during reward feedback processing. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00371.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, Y. J., Lim, J. A., Lee, J. Y., Oh, S., Kim, S. N., Kim, D. J., … Choi, J. S. (2017). Impulsivity and compulsivity in internet gaming disorder: A comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 545553. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.069.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuo, S. Y., Chen, Y. T., Chang, Y. K., Lee, P. H., Liu, M. J., & Chen, S. R. (2018). Influence of internet addiction on executive function and learning attention in Taiwanese school-aged children. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 54(4), 495500. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppc.12254.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons learned. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(3). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14030311.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 40264052. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990617.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, R. S. C., Hoppenbrouwers, S., & Franken, I. (2019). A systematic meta-review of impulsivity and compulsivity in addictive behaviors. Neuropsychology Review, 29(1), 1426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-019-09402-x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leeman, R. F., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 469490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lei, W., Liu, K., Chen, G., Tolomeo, S., Liu, C., Peng, Z., … Chen, J. (2022). Blunted reward prediction error signals in internet gaming disorder. Psychological Medicine, 52(11), 21242133. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000402X.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lim, J. -A., Lee, J. -Y., Jung, H. Y., Sohn, B. K., Choi, S. -W., Kim, Y. J., … Choi, J. -S. (2016). Changes of quality of life and cognitive function in individuals with internet gaming disorder: A 6-month follow-up. Medicine, 95(50), e5695. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000005695.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, D. -Y., & Yang, H. -B. (2023). The improvement of attentional bias in individuals with problematic smartphone use through cognitive reappraisal: An eye-tracking study. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05356-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, G. -C., Yen, J. -Y., Chen, C. -Y., Yen, C. -F., Chen, C. -S., Lin, W. -C., & Ko, C. -H. (2014). Brain activation for response inhibition under gaming cue distraction in internet gaming disorder. The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 30(1), 4351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.kjms.2013.08.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, L., Yip, S. W., Zhang, J. -T., Wang, L. -J., Shen, Z. -J., Liu, B., … Fang, X. -Y. (2017). Activation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during cue reactivity in Internet gaming disorder. Addiction Biology, 22(3), 791801. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12338.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorenz, R. C., Kruger, J. K., Neumann, B., Schott, B. H., Kaufmann, C., Heinz, A., & Wustenberg, T. (2013). Cue reactivity and its inhibition in pathological computer game players. Addiction Biology, 18(1), 134146. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00491.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metcalf, O., & Pammer, K. (2011). Attentional bias in excessive massively multiplayer online role-playing gamers using a modified Stroop task. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(5), 19421947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.05.001.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 2945. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1646-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., … Stewart, L. A. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Müller, S. M., Wegmann, E., Oelker, A., Stark, R., Müller, A., Montag, C., … Brand, M. (2022). Assessment of Criteria for Specific Internet-use Disorders (ACSID-11): Introduction of a new screening instrument capturing ICD-11 criteria for gaming disorder and other potential Internet-use disorders. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(2), 427450. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Munn, Z., Peters, M. D. J., Stern, C., Tufanaru, C., McArthur, A., & Aromataris, E. (2018). Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 18(1), 143. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0611-x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nikolaidou, M., Fraser, D. S., & Hinvest, N. (2019). Attentional bias in Internet users with problematic use of social networking sites. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(4), 733742. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.60.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nyhus, E., & Barceló, F. (2009). The Wisconsin card sorting test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: A critical update. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 437451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.03.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pan, Y. -C., Chiu, Y. -C., & Lin, Y. -H. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiology of internet addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 612622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parvaz, M. A., Konova, A. B., Proudfit, G. H., Dunning, J. P., Malaker, P., Moeller, S. J., … Goldstein, R. Z. (2015). Impaired neural response to negative prediction errors in cocaine addiction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(5), 18721879. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2777-14.2015.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pekal, J., Laier, C., Snagowski, J., Stark, R., & Brand, M. (2018). Tendencies toward Internet-pornography-use disorder: Differences in men and women regarding attentional biases to pornographic stimuli. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(3), 574583. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.70.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Peters, M. D. J., Godfrey, C. M., Khalil, H., McInerney, P., Parker, D., & Soares, C. B. (2015). Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare, 13(3), 141146. https://doi.org/10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Gentile, D. A., Lemmens, J. S., Rumpf, H. -J., Mößle, T., … O’Brien, C. P. (2014). An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction, 109(9), 13991406. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., & Murayama, K. (2017). Internet gaming disorder: Investigating the clinical relevance of a new phenomenon. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(3), 230236. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020224.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rabinovitz, S., & Nagar, M. (2015). Possible end to an endless quest? Cognitive bias modification for excessive multiplayer online gamers. Cyberpsychology Behavior and Social Networking, 18(10), 581587. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0173.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raj, K., Segrave, R., Verdéjo-Garcia, A., & Yücel, M. (2023). Cognitive inflexibility and repetitive habitual actions are associated with problematic use of the internet. Addictive Behaviors, 139, 107600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107600.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reitan, R. M., & Reitan, R. M. (1992). Trail making test: Manual for administration and scoring. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:141448957.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., & Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: Towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 8191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Lawrence, A. D., McInnes, L., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Batt. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 474490. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798455073.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roberts, M. E., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., Southgate, L., & Treasure, J. (2007). A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 37(8), 10751084. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707009877.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sebold, M., Deserno, L., Nebe, S., Schad, D. J., Garbusow, M., Haegele, C., … Huys, Q. J. M. (2014). Model-based and model-free decisions in alcohol dependence. Neuropsychobiology, 70(2), 122131. https://doi.org/10.1159/000362840.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sjoerds, Z., de Wit, S., van den Brink, W., Robbins, T. W., Beekman, A. T. F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., & Veltman, D. J. (2013). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Translational Psychiatry, 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sklenarik, S., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., & Astur, R. S. (2020). Approach bias for erotic stimuli among heterosexual female college students who use pornography. Addictive Behaviors, 108, 106438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106438.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sklenarik, S., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Kor, A., Kraus, S. W., & Astur, R. S. (2019). Approach bias for erotic stimuli in heterosexual male college students who use pornography. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(2), 234241. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.31.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643662. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tanabe, J., Reynolds, J., Krmpotich, T., Claus, E., Thompson, L. L., Du, Y. P., & Banich, M. T. (2013). Reduced neural tracking of prediction error in substance-dependent individuals. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(11), 13561363. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12091257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tekin, A., Yetkin, A., Adiguzel, S., & Akman, H. (2018). Evaluation of Stroop and Trail-Making Tests performance in university students with internet addiction. Anadolu Psikiyatri Dergisi-Anatolian Journal of Psychiatry, 19(6), 593598. https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.292389.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Deursen, A., Bolle, C. L., Hegner, S. M., & Kommers, P. A. M. (2015). Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior: The role of smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress, self-regulation, age, and gender. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 411420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.039.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Attentional bias and disinhibition toward gaming cues are related to problem gaming in male adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50(6), 541546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.07.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Daams, J. G., van Holst, R. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2018). Compulsivity-related neurocognitive performance deficits in gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 204217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.022.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vandaele, Y., & Ahmed, S. H. (2021). Habit, choice, and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(4), 689698. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00899-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verdejo-García, A., Alcázar-Córcoles, M. A., & Albein-Urios, N. (2019). Neuropsychological interventions for decision-making in addiction: A systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 29(1), 7992. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9384-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Ruck, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., … Bullmore, E. T. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: A common bias towards learning habits. Molecular Psychiatry, 20(3), 345352. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.44.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, J. -F., & Huang, Y. -L. (2022). Approach-avoidance pattern of attentional bias in individuals with high tendencies toward problematic Internet pornography use. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.988435.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, L. -X., Zhang, Y. -F., Lin, X., Zhou, H. -L., Du, X. -X., & Dong, G. -H. (2018). Group independent component analysis reveals alternation of right executive control network in Internet gaming disorder. CNS Spectrums, 23(5), 300310. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852917000360.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wells, G. A., Shea, B., O’Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., & Tugwell, P. (2000). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Available from: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.htm.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wyckmans, F., Otto, A. R., Sebold, M., Daw, N., Bechara, A., Saeremans, M., … Noël, X. (2019). Reduced model-based decision-making in gambling disorder. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 19625. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-56161-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yao, Y. -W., Wang, L. -J., Yip, S. W., Chen, P. -R., Li, S., Xu, J., … Fang, X. -Y. (2015). Impaired decision-making under risk is associated with gaming-specific inhibition deficits among college students with Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Research, 229, 302309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yücel, M., & Fontenelle, L. F. (2012). Compulsivity as an endophenotype: The search for a hazy moving target. Addiction, 107(10), 17351736. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03663.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zeng, X., Zhang, Y., Kwong, J. S. W., Zhang, C., Li, S., Sun, F., … Du, L. (2015). The methodological quality assessment tools for preclinical and clinical studies, systematic review and meta-analysis, and clinical practice guideline: A systematic review. Journal of Evidence-Based Medicine, 8(1), 210. https://doi.org/10.1111/jebm.12141.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, Y. -F., Lin, X., Zhou, H. -L., Xu, J. -J., Du, X. -X., & Dong, G. -H. (2016). Brain activity toward gaming-related cues in internet gaming disorder during an addiction Stroop task. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00714.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhao, J., Zhou, Z., Sun, B., Zhang, X., Zhang, L., & Fu, S. (2022). Attentional bias is associated with negative emotions in problematic users of social media as measured by a dot-probe task. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(24). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416938.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, B., Wang, W., Zhang, W., Li, Y., & Nie, J. (2018). Succumb to habit: Behavioral evidence for overreliance on habit learning in Internet addicts. Computers in Human Behavior, 89, 230236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.030.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, W., Zheng, H., Wang, M., Zheng, Y., Chen, S., Wang, M., & Dong, G. -H. (2021). The imbalance between goal-directed and habitual systems in internet gaming disorder: Results from the disturbed thalamocortical communications. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 134, 121128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.12.058.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, Y., Zhou, Y., Zhou, J., Shen, M., & Zhang, M. (2022). Attentional biases and daily game craving dynamics: An ecological momentary assessment study. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(4), 10441054. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00085.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, Z. -H., Zhou, H. -L., & Zhu, H. -M. (2016). Working memory, executive function and impulsivity in internet-addictive disorders: A comparison with pathological gambling. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 28(2), 92100. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2015.54.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, Z., Zhu, H., Li, C., & Wang, J. (2014). Internet addictive individuals share impulsivity and executive dysfunction with alcohol-dependent patients. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00288.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Supplementary Materials

  • Antons, S., Müller, S. M., Neumann, P., Müller, A., & Steins-Loeber, S. (2023). Stimuli-specific inhibitory control in disorders due to addictive behaviours: A review of current evidence and discussion of methodological challenges. Current Addiction Reports, 10(4), 749769. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-023-00517-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aydin, O., Obuća, F., Boz, C., & Ünal-Aydın, P. (2020). Associations between executive functions and problematic social networking sites use. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 42(6), 634645. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2020.1798358.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Balleine, B. W., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental action: Contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology, 37(4–5), 407419. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Banca, P., Harrison, N. A., & Voon, V. (2016). Compulsivity across the pathological misuse of drug and non-drug rewards. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 154. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carr, M. M., Wiedemann, A. A., Macdonald-Gagnon, G., & Potenza, M. N. (2021). Impulsivity and compulsivity in binge eating disorder: A systematic review of behavioral studies. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 110, 110318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2021.110318.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Castro-Calvo, J., King, D. L., Stein, D. J., Brand, M., Carmi, L., Chamberlain, S. R., … Billieux, J. (2021). Expert appraisal of criteria for assessing gaming disorder: An international Delphi study. Addiction, 116(9), 24632475. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.15411.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chamberlain, S. R., Ioannidis, K., & Grant, J. E. (2018). The impact of comorbid impulsive/compulsive disorders in problematic Internet use. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(2), 269275. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.30.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chamberlain, S. R., Solly, J. E., Hook, R. W., Vaghi, M. M., & Robbins, T. W. (2021). Cognitive inflexibility in OCD and related disorders. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 49, 125145. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2020_198.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, S. -W., Kim, H. S., Kim, G. -Y., Jeon, Y., Park, S. M., Lee, J. -Y., … Kim, D. -J. (2014a). Similarities and differences among internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3(4), 246253. https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.3.2014.4.6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, J. -S., Park, S. M., Roh, M. -S., Lee, J. -Y., Park, C. -B., Hwang, J. Y., … Jung, H. Y. (2014b). Dysfunctional inhibitory control and impulsivity in Internet addiction. Psychiatry Research, 215(2), 424428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.001.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron, 69(6), 12041215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de Wit, S., Corlett, P. R., Aitken, M. R., Dickinson, A., & Fletcher, P. C. (2009). Differential engagement of the ventromedial prefrontal Ccortex by goal-directed and habitual behavior toward food pictures in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(36), 1133011338. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1639-09.2009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demetrovics, Z., van den Brink, W., Paksi, B., Horvath, Z., & Maraz, A. (2022). Relating compulsivity and impulsivity with severity of behavioral addictions: A dynamic interpretation of large-scale cross-sectional findings. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.831992.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ersche, K. D., Gillan, C. M., Jones, P. S., Williams, G. B., Ward, L. H. E., Luijten, M., … Robbins, T. W. (2016). Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction. Science, 352(6292), 14681471. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf3700.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2016). Drug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 2350. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faunce, G. J., & Job, R. F. (2000). The Stroop colour-naming task and addictive behaviour: Some recommendations. Addiction, 95(9), 14381442. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.959143818.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, M., & Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 97, 120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.030.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Chamberlain, S. R., Goudriaan, A. E., Stein, D. J., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Gillan, C. M., … Potenza, M. N. (2014). New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectrums, 19(1), 6989. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852913000801.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Demetrovics, Z., Stein, D. J., Ioannidis, K., Potenza, M. N., Grünblatt, E., … Chamberlain, S. R. (2018). Manifesto for a European research network into problematic usage of the internet. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 28(11), 12321246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.08.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fineberg, N. A., Menchón, J. M., Hall, N., Dell’Osso, B., Brand, M., Potenza, M. N., … Zohar, J. (2022). Advances in problematic usage of the internet research – A narrative review by experts from the European network for problematic usage of the internet. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 118, 152346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2022.152346.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Firoozabadi, A., Razavian, Y., Saleh, S., & Hosseini, S. R. (2023). The comparison of neurocognitive functions between internet-addicted, methamphetamine users, and healthy participants. Applied Neuropsychology-Adult. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2023.2224478.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., & de Wit, S. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(7), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gou, S. -Y., Yuan, R. -J., Zhang, W. -Q., Tang, Y. -L., & Zhang, W. -Z. (2023). Problematic social media use in youths cause response inhibition impairment. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05425-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Han, D. H., Kim, S. M., Bae, S., Renshaw, P. F., & Anderson, J. S. (2016). A failure of suppression within the default mode network in depressed adolescents with compulsive internet game play. Journal of Affective Disorders, 194, 5764. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.01.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • He, J., Pan, T., Nie, Y., Zheng, Y., & Chen, S. (2021). Behavioral modification decreases approach bias in young adults with internet gaming disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 113, 106686. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106686.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • He, J., Zheng, Y., Nie, Y. F., & Zhou, Z. -K. (2018). Automatic detection advantage of network information among Internet addicts: Behavioral and ERP evidence. Scientific Reports, 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25442-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heaton, R. K. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test: computer version 2 (pp. 14). Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

  • Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., … Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ioannidis, K., Hook, R., Goudriaan, A. E., Vlies, S., Fineberg, N. A., Grant, J. E., & Chamberlain, S. R. (2019). Cognitive deficits in problematic internet use: Meta-analysis of 40 studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 215(5), 18. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2019.3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Izquierdo, A., Brigman, J. L., Radke, A. K., Rudebeck, P. H., & Holmes, A. (2017). The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective. Neuroscience, 345, 1226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.03.021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Izquierdo, A., & Jentsch, J. D. (2012). Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 607620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jazbec, S., Pantelis, C., Robbins, T., Weickert, T., Weinberger, D. R., & Goldberg, T. E. (2007). Intra-dimensional/extra-dimensional set-shifting performance in schizophrenia: Impact of distractors. Schizophrenia Research, 89, 339349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.08.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeromin, F., Nyenhuis, N., & Barke, A. (2016). Attentional bias in excessive Internet gamers: Experimental investigations using an addiction Stroop and a visual probe. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(1), 3240. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.012.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeromin, F., Rief, W., & Barke, A. (2016). Using two web-based addiction Stroops to measure the attentional bias in adults with Internet Gaming Disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(4), 666673. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.075.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jiang, Z. -C., Zhao, X. -X., & Li, C. -C. (2017). Self-control predicts attentional bias assessed by online shopping-related Stroop in high online shopping addiction tendency college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 75, 1421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.02.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, J., & Kang, E. (2018). Internet game overuse is associated with an alteration of fronto-striatal functional connectivity during reward feedback processing. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00371.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, Y. J., Lim, J. A., Lee, J. Y., Oh, S., Kim, S. N., Kim, D. J., … Choi, J. S. (2017). Impulsivity and compulsivity in internet gaming disorder: A comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 545553. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.069.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuo, S. Y., Chen, Y. T., Chang, Y. K., Lee, P. H., Liu, M. J., & Chen, S. R. (2018). Influence of internet addiction on executive function and learning attention in Taiwanese school-aged children. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 54(4), 495500. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppc.12254.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Social networking sites and addiction: Ten lessons learned. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(3). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14030311.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 40264052. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990617.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, R. S. C., Hoppenbrouwers, S., & Franken, I. (2019). A systematic meta-review of impulsivity and compulsivity in addictive behaviors. Neuropsychology Review, 29(1), 1426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-019-09402-x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leeman, R. F., & Potenza, M. N. (2012). Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 219(2), 469490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lei, W., Liu, K., Chen, G., Tolomeo, S., Liu, C., Peng, Z., … Chen, J. (2022). Blunted reward prediction error signals in internet gaming disorder. Psychological Medicine, 52(11), 21242133. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000402X.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lim, J. -A., Lee, J. -Y., Jung, H. Y., Sohn, B. K., Choi, S. -W., Kim, Y. J., … Choi, J. -S. (2016). Changes of quality of life and cognitive function in individuals with internet gaming disorder: A 6-month follow-up. Medicine, 95(50), e5695. https://doi.org/10.1097/MD.0000000000005695.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, D. -Y., & Yang, H. -B. (2023). The improvement of attentional bias in individuals with problematic smartphone use through cognitive reappraisal: An eye-tracking study. Current Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-05356-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, G. -C., Yen, J. -Y., Chen, C. -Y., Yen, C. -F., Chen, C. -S., Lin, W. -C., & Ko, C. -H. (2014). Brain activation for response inhibition under gaming cue distraction in internet gaming disorder. The Kaohsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 30(1), 4351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.kjms.2013.08.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liu, L., Yip, S. W., Zhang, J. -T., Wang, L. -J., Shen, Z. -J., Liu, B., … Fang, X. -Y. (2017). Activation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during cue reactivity in Internet gaming disorder. Addiction Biology, 22(3), 791801. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12338.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorenz, R. C., Kruger, J. K., Neumann, B., Schott, B. H., Kaufmann, C., Heinz, A., & Wustenberg, T. (2013). Cue reactivity and its inhibition in pathological computer game players. Addiction Biology, 18(1), 134146. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00491.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metcalf, O., & Pammer, K. (2011). Attentional bias in excessive massively multiplayer online role-playing gamers using a modified Stroop task. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(5), 19421947. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2011.05.001.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 2945. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-005-1646-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., … Stewart, L. A. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Müller, S. M., Wegmann, E., Oelker, A., Stark, R., Müller, A., Montag, C., … Brand, M. (2022). Assessment of Criteria for Specific Internet-use Disorders (ACSID-11): Introduction of a new screening instrument capturing ICD-11 criteria for gaming disorder and other potential Internet-use disorders. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(2), 427450. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Munn, Z., Peters, M. D. J., Stern, C., Tufanaru, C., McArthur, A., & Aromataris, E. (2018). Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 18(1), 143. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-018-0611-x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nikolaidou, M., Fraser, D. S., & Hinvest, N. (2019). Attentional bias in Internet users with problematic use of social networking sites. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(4), 733742. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.60.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nyhus, E., & Barceló, F. (2009). The Wisconsin card sorting test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: A critical update. Brain and Cognition, 71(3), 437451. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.03.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pan, Y. -C., Chiu, Y. -C., & Lin, Y. -H. (2020). Systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiology of internet addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 612622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Parvaz, M. A., Konova, A. B., Proudfit, G. H., Dunning, J. P., Malaker, P., Moeller, S. J., … Goldstein, R. Z. (2015). Impaired neural response to negative prediction errors in cocaine addiction. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(5), 18721879. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2777-14.2015.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pekal, J., Laier, C., Snagowski, J., Stark, R., & Brand, M. (2018). Tendencies toward Internet-pornography-use disorder: Differences in men and women regarding attentional biases to pornographic stimuli. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(3), 574583. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.70.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Peters, M. D. J., Godfrey, C. M., Khalil, H., McInerney, P., Parker, D., & Soares, C. B. (2015). Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. International Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare, 13(3), 141146. https://doi.org/10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Gentile, D. A., Lemmens, J. S., Rumpf, H. -J., Mößle, T., … O’Brien, C. P. (2014). An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction, 109(9), 13991406. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12457.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., & Murayama, K. (2017). Internet gaming disorder: Investigating the clinical relevance of a new phenomenon. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174(3), 230236. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020224.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rabinovitz, S., & Nagar, M. (2015). Possible end to an endless quest? Cognitive bias modification for excessive multiplayer online gamers. Cyberpsychology Behavior and Social Networking, 18(10), 581587. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0173.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raj, K., Segrave, R., Verdéjo-Garcia, A., & Yücel, M. (2023). Cognitive inflexibility and repetitive habitual actions are associated with problematic use of the internet. Addictive Behaviors, 139, 107600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107600.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reitan, R. M., & Reitan, R. M. (1992). Trail making test: Manual for administration and scoring. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:141448957.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., & Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: Towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 8191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Lawrence, A. D., McInnes, L., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: Implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Batt. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(5), 474490. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798455073.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roberts, M. E., Tchanturia, K., Stahl, D., Southgate, L., & Treasure, J. (2007). A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 37(8), 10751084. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291707009877.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sebold, M., Deserno, L., Nebe, S., Schad, D. J., Garbusow, M., Haegele, C., … Huys, Q. J. M. (2014). Model-based and model-free decisions in alcohol dependence. Neuropsychobiology, 70(2), 122131. https://doi.org/10.1159/000362840.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sjoerds, Z., de Wit, S., van den Brink, W., Robbins, T. W., Beekman, A. T. F., Penninx, B. W. J. H., & Veltman, D. J. (2013). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Translational Psychiatry, 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.107.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sklenarik, S., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., & Astur, R. S. (2020). Approach bias for erotic stimuli among heterosexual female college students who use pornography. Addictive Behaviors, 108, 106438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106438.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sklenarik, S., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Kor, A., Kraus, S. W., & Astur, R. S. (2019). Approach bias for erotic stimuli in heterosexual male college students who use pornography. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(2), 234241. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.31.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643662. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tanabe, J., Reynolds, J., Krmpotich, T., Claus, E., Thompson, L. L., Du, Y. P., & Banich, M. T. (2013). Reduced neural tracking of prediction error in substance-dependent individuals. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(11), 13561363. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12091257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tekin, A., Yetkin, A., Adiguzel, S., & Akman, H. (2018). Evaluation of Stroop and Trail-Making Tests performance in university students with internet addiction. Anadolu Psikiyatri Dergisi-Anatolian Journal of Psychiatry, 19(6), 593598. https://doi.org/10.5455/apd.292389.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Deursen, A., Bolle, C. L., Hegner, S. M., & Kommers, P. A. M. (2015). Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior: The role of smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress, self-regulation, age, and gender. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 411420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.039.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Attentional bias and disinhibition toward gaming cues are related to problem gaming in male adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50(6), 541546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.07.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Timmeren, T., Daams, J. G., van Holst, R. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2018). Compulsivity-related neurocognitive performance deficits in gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84, 204217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.022.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vandaele, Y., & Ahmed, S. H. (2021). Habit, choice, and addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(4), 689698. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00899-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verdejo-García, A., Alcázar-Córcoles, M. A., & Albein-Urios, N. (2019). Neuropsychological interventions for decision-making in addiction: A systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 29(1), 79