Author:
Rory C. Reid Department of Psychiatry, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USAand Global Clinical Scholars Research Training Program, Harvard University, Boston, MA, USA

Search for other papers by Rory C. Reid in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and Aims

The concept of severity among providers working with hypersexual behavior is frequently used despite a lack of consensus about how severity should be operationalized. The paucity of dialogue about severity for hypersexual behavior is disconcerting given its relevance in determining level of care, risk, allocation of resources, and measuring treatment outcomes in clinical practice and research trials. The aim of the current article is to highlight several considerations for assessing severity based on the proposed DSM-5 criteria for hypersexual disorder.

Methods

A review of current conceptualizations for severity among substance-use disorders and gambling disorder in the DSM-5 were considered and challenged as lacking applicability or clinical utility for hypersexual behavior.

Results and conclusions

The current research in the field of hypersexual behavior is in its infancy. No concrete approach currently exists to assess severity in hypersexual populations. Several factors in operationalizing severity are discussed and alternative approaches to defining severity are offered for readers to consider.

Abstract

Background and Aims

The concept of severity among providers working with hypersexual behavior is frequently used despite a lack of consensus about how severity should be operationalized. The paucity of dialogue about severity for hypersexual behavior is disconcerting given its relevance in determining level of care, risk, allocation of resources, and measuring treatment outcomes in clinical practice and research trials. The aim of the current article is to highlight several considerations for assessing severity based on the proposed DSM-5 criteria for hypersexual disorder.

Methods

A review of current conceptualizations for severity among substance-use disorders and gambling disorder in the DSM-5 were considered and challenged as lacking applicability or clinical utility for hypersexual behavior.

Results and conclusions

The current research in the field of hypersexual behavior is in its infancy. No concrete approach currently exists to assess severity in hypersexual populations. Several factors in operationalizing severity are discussed and alternative approaches to defining severity are offered for readers to consider.

Introduction

The proposed criteria for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013) characterize Hypersexual Disorder (HD) as a repetitive and intense preoccupation with sexual fantasies, urges, and behaviors, leading to adverse consequences and clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. One defining feature of the proposed disorder includes multiple unsuccessful attempts to control or diminish the amount of time an individual engages in sexual fantasies, urges, and behavior in response to dysphoric mood states or stressful life events (Kafka, 2010). While some have suggested HD should be considered a behavioral addiction the empirical literature appears to fall short of supporting this conceptualization at the present time (Kor, Fogel, Reid & Potenza, 2013). Nevertheless, there are many parallels concerning HD and addictive-related disorders and this appears especially true for shared commonalities between HD and a gambling disorder (Farre et al., 2015). However, it is unclear how severity for hypersexual behavior should be conceptualized and this is the first paper in the literature to explore how HD severity might be operationalized. This article is intended to be a catalyst to advancing some perspectives and discussion about assessing severity related to HD. As a caveat, the current article uses the term HD or hypersexual behavior for purposes of referring to the HD criteria proposed for DSM-5 while recognizing that ultimately the proposed disorder was excluded in the final publication for a number of reasons discussed elsewhere (Reid & Kafka, 2014). However, given the results of the DSM-5 field trial (Reid et al., 2012), the HD criteria are commonly used as the standard for measuring this phenomenon in clinical and research applications.

Methods

Defining severity

The Cambridge Online Dictionary defines severity as something serious or causing great pain, difficulty, or damage. Severity in the DSM-5 has typically been operationalized from mild to severe and assigned to a diagnosis based on the number of symptom criteria endorsed. In the section on Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders, severity across time is also measured by reductions or increases in the frequency and/or doses of substances used. Likewise, severity for Gambling Disorder is assigned by the number of symptoms endorsed at the time of evaluation. However, the DSM-5 operationalization of severity for addictive-related disorders is debatable and this is certainly true for HD for a vast array of reasons articulated below.

Severity in the DSM-5 by symptom endorsement

Symptom count is a gross measure of severity and makes an assumption that all symptoms for a given disorder are equal. During the DSM-5 field trial for HD, some patients barely met the diagnostic threshold for HD that would have likely been considered more “serious” or “severe” cases but such impressions were based on an examination of the broader scope of their overall clinical presentation that extended beyond the DSM-5 proposed criteria. This observation has also been noted elsewhere in substance-related disorders (Moss, 2011). Complicating matters, symptom endorsement for HD has a much higher threshold required compared to substance-related and gambling disorders. For example, several substance-use disorders can be diagnosed with as few as 2 of 10 or more symptoms. A gambling disorder requires 4 of 9 symptoms. An HD diagnosis, however, required 4 out of 5 of the “A” criteria, evidence of impairment, and symptoms had to occur independent of a substance-related disorder, medical condition, or manic episode (see Figure 1). Collectively, the current structure of the HD proposed criteria makes severity by symptom endorsement difficult insofar as someone meeting the threshold for the diagnosis endorses 80% of the symptoms (e.g., a patient meeting criteria for HD would have either endorsed 4 or 5 of the ‘A’ criteria, which does not allow for the current tri-categorized DSM-5 severity designations — mild, moderate, and severe). The threshold for meeting the HD criteria would need to be lowered to 3 out of 5 of the ‘A’ criteria to accommodate severity categorizations of mild, moderate, and severe, however, such a change would potentially introduce the problem of false-positive HD diagnosis. Finally, symptom count as an index of severity also ignores the magnitude of how someone might experience a given symptom. For example patients may report significant clinical distress but the magnitude of the distress may vary across individuals. This is why someone with a greater magnitude across a few symptoms might be considered a more severe case than someone with more symptoms but of a lesser magnitude. Given the limitations of the symptom-count approach to operationalizing severity, studies are needed to compare this method with alternative approaches. The field of hypersexuality research might consider some of the work challenging substance-use disorder severity designations as a model for research that could be applied to the HD criteria (Fazzino, Rose, Burt & Helzer, 2014).

Figure 1.
Figure 1.

DSM-5 Proposed criteria for hypersexual disorder

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 4, 4; 10.1556/2006.4.2015.041

Excessiveness, frequency, and duration

One approach to assess severity might consider how much time is spent engaging in sexual fantasies, urges, and behavior (e.g. time spent planning for and recovering from sexual activities). Generally time — as measured by the frequency and duration of sexual fantasies, urges, and behaviors — can vary and may influence whether an activity is considered excessive or even problematic. For example, masturbation once a week for 15 minutes might not be considered excessive or problematic whereas sex with an extra-dyadic partner outside a monogamous committed relationship once a week for 15 minutes is likely both problematic and excessive. These examples raise questions about whether severity should consider the specific manifestation of sexual behavior. While no attempt was made to pathologize the expression of sexual behaviors with the DSM-5 proposal for HD, it is important to ask how the type of sexual behavior linked to hypersexuality might play a role in assessing levels of severity. For example, do solo-sex behaviors carry more or less potential to contribute to a severity index than relational sexual activities? As evident above, considering severity based exclusively on level of excessiveness or frequency of a sexual activity has several limitations.

Level of impairment, diminished control, and consequences

Another approach might consider severity based on the extent that hypersexual behavior causes significant impairment to one’s social, occupational or other important areas of functioning. Closely related to impairment are the concepts of diminished control and consequences. Certainly, these three concepts have some overlap and interaction. Diminished control contributes to repetitive hypersexual behaviors that create consequences which impair one’s ability to function. These concepts could be considered with respect to operationalizing severity. For example, multiple unsuccessful attempts to control or significantly reduce sexual fantasies, urges and behaviors might suggest a greater level of severity. Perhaps diminished control in one context might have greater consequences than others (e.g. inability to refrain from pornography on a work vs. a home computer). Of course, the notion of diminished control makes an assumption that attempts to control the frequency or manifestations of hypersexual behavior have occurred. In cases where a patient may be pre-contemplative about change, it is possible attempts to control or change their behavior have never been made, and thus patients would deny experiencing multiple unsuccessful attempts to control their sexual behavior. Further, the presence or absence of such efforts may occur, independent of the severity of their condition.

The number or types of consequences might also influence how severity is assigned. Consequences however, vary depending on several other factors and some consequences might be considered more severe based on the implications, frequency, or subjective values. In attempt to understand consequences, researchers developed the Hypersexual Behavior Consequences Scale which offered several insights about the types of consequences encountered by hypersexual patients (Reid, Garos & Fong, 2012). For example, consequences leading to interference with friendships typically have different ramifications than those leading to divorce. A consequence such as job loss might also vary in severity depending on other variables. For example, job loss might be associated with greater severity if one is financially struggling for money or if it is highly publicized in the media as with some political scandals, celebrities, or well-known sports figures. The related legal problems that can be associated with hypersexual behavior can also heighten the seriousness of a case presentation and by extension, how severity is conceptualized. A patient’s moral values might also influence whether a specific consequence is considered severe. One patient marginalized the consequence of an unintended pregnancy resulting from hypersexual behavior stating “it wasn’t an issue, she just got an abortion.” However, upon further investigation, it was discovered the woman felt pressured into having the abortion and afterward experienced significant depression, shame, and guilt as it contradicted her religious values. Subsequently, it might be important to consider the extent to which others may be physically or emotionally harmed by hypersexual behavior and such factors weighted in assessing severity. Further, a way of quantifying consequences is necessary to determine what types of consequences should be given more attention in assessing severity.

An extension of the vast array of consequences encountered by hypersexual patients might consider whether hypersexual behavior contributes to comorbid psychological conditions such as depression, anxiety, substance abuse, suicidality or personality traits such as shame (Raymond, Coleman & Minor, 2003; Reid, Stein & Carpenter, 2011). Such cases require special consideration and their complexity can influence the seriousness, pain, or suffering leading to a greater level of severity.

Level of risk taking

One symptom of the HD proposal involves risk taking: “Repetitively engaging in sexual behavior while disregarding the risk for physical or emotional harm to self or others.” Arguably, not all risk taking behavior is equal and can vastly influence how severity might be operationalized. The word severe can also imply damage and some risk-taking behavior has significantly higher potential for greater damage. Some examples might include risks associated with sexually transmitted infections, unintended pregnancies, physical harm to self or others, legal problems, job loss, divorce, and so forth. During the DSM-5 field trial, patients reported masturbating to the extent of incurring genital lesions requiring medical attention. Some described watching pornography on their digital devices while driving placing them at risk for automobile accidents. A few patients meeting criteria for HD also had a comorbid paraphilic disorder of autoerotic asphyxiation, which has also been linked to unintended deaths. Several patients described pursuing sex in neighborhoods that were known to have high crime rates that compromised their physical safety. While not part of the DSM-5 criteria for a gambling disorder, several studies show individuals with a gambling disorder and co-occurring illegal activities associated with gambling exhibited greater levels of gambling severity in comparison to gamblers without criminal histories (Ledgerwood, Weinstock, Morasco & Petry, 2007; Potenza, Steinberg, McLaughlin, Rounsaville & O’Malley, 2000; Strong & Kahler, 2007). Subsequently, if gambling research generalized to hypersexual patients, those who commit illegal acts in order to engage in sexual activities may represent more severe cases. Collectively, the level of risk a patient is willing to disregard to pursue sexual fantasies, urges, and behaviors should be considered in assessing levels of severity.

Cravings and urges

Understanding severity for HD based on sexual cravings and urges creates several challenges. First, unlike substance-use disorders where abstinence is often a goal, clinicians try to help hypersexual patients reorganize their relationship with sexual cravings in order to cultivate healthy expressions of sexuality. Thus, a sexual craving leading to sexual behavior may not always be problematic whereas a craving to use cocaine is likely always considered problematic.

Another challenge is the lack of research specifically focused on the role of cravings in the etiology or maintenance of hypersexual behavior. The field is in its infancy and only recently began developing measures of craving (Kraus & Rosenberg, 2014). However, more investigations are needed to understand the role of craving and its relationship to the concept of severity. For example, ecological momentary assessment could be used to further understand how cravings manifest among hypersexual individuals (Shiffman, Stone & Hufford, 2008). Such research might help resolve existing controversy where some have argued HD falls short of a pathological condition and is merely a manifestation of high sexual desire (e.g. craving) of behaviors and not an addictive process per se (Carvalho, Stulhofer, Vieira & Jurin, 2015; Steele, Staley, Fong & Prause, 2013; Winters, Christoff & Gorzalka, 2010).

As the field evolves, researchers might consider drawing on the work of those in the field of gambling disorders who have found some evidence between self-reported cravings and relapse (Oei & Gordon, 2008), risk-taking propensity in wagering (Ashrafioun, Kostek & Ziegelmeyer, 2013), and gambling severity as measured by the Problem-Gambling Severity Index (Ferris & Wynne, 2001) among gambling populations (Young & Wohl, 2009). However, other researchers in the field of behavioral addictions have considered alternative models to help explain the construct of cravings such as those often reported in food addiction (Rogers & Hendrik, 2000). Collectively, further research is needed to understand the construct of cravings among hypersexual populations in order to consider how sexual cravings might be linked (if at all) in characterizing the severity of HD. Regardless, cravings, as an index of severity, relapse, treatment response, or etiology, is likely to pose a difficult and challenging area of scientific inquiry as noted by other prominent craving researchers in the field of substance-use disorders (Tiffany & Wray, 2009).

Onset and clinical course

During the DSM-5 field trial for HD, the onset and clinical course of HD among participants was explored (Reid, Carpenter et al., 2012). While the proposal limited an HD diagnosis to adults over the age of 18, surprisingly, 54% of participants stated they began to experience difficulties regulating their sexual behavior as adolescents and 30% during college-age years. Most (82.6%) reported a gradual onset lasting several months or years with 17.4% indicating a rapid-acute onset > 90 days. The clinical course was split with 48.6% reporting a continuous pattern of hypersexual behavior and 51.4% stating episodic patterns. The substantial number of participants reported patterns of escalation including time (83.5%), frequency or intensity (81.7%), types of manifestations (62.4%), and increased risk (60.6%). There is no consensus about how these data might be associated with severity of HD, but similar data have been considered as surrogates for other indicators of severity in alcohol use disorders (e.g. Hingson, Heeren & Winter, 2006). At present, further research is needed to investigate what implications the onset and clinical course of HD symptoms may have on attrition in therapy, treatment adherence, relapse, refractoriness to treatment – all which might be markers of severity – and the overall prognosis for hypersexual patients. Such research might consider naturalistic longitudinal studies assessing the trajectory of sexual behaviors and HD across the lifespan in relation to different conceptualizations of severity.

Statistical approaches

Recently, item response theory (IRT) has been used to evaluate severity (Saha, Chou & Grant, 2006) based on the assumption that symptoms less commonly endorsed reflect outliers with greater problems or severity. A key advantage of IRT is the ability to select items based on the information they contribute which can inform the development of a severity index (Conway et al., 2010). For example, an IRT approach has been used to assess the continuum of gambling problems using DSM criteria in exploring an index of problem severity (Strong & Kahler, 2007). Alternatively, cluster analysis could be used to examine latent dimensions of HD and used to assess how best to threshold cases among populations of patients seeking help for hypersexual behavior. Survival analysis is a plausible solution to evaluate the relationship between time and symptom remission in hypersexual patients allowing insights about treatment profiles and resistance to change. This “resistance” could subsequently be utilized as part of classifying a severity index. For example, if a particular patient profile is predictive of greater resistance to change and requires more treatment resources, such a profile might be classified as having a greater level of severity.

Results and Conclusions

The concept of severity among providers working with hypersexual behavior is frequently used, despite any lack of consensus about how severity should be operationalized. This article has highlighted several challenges to operationalizing severity for the DSM-5 proposed criteria for HD. Regardless of what approach to classifying severity is ultimately adopted, the field should start dialogue about this concept given its relevance to determining level of patient care, risk, allocation of resources, and measuring treatment outcomes in clinical trials.

Authors’ contribution

The solo-author is responsible for all of the content and contributions to this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The author was the principle investigator for the DSM-5 field trial for hypersexual disorder. He has no other conflicts of interest to declare.

REFERENCES

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ashrafioun, L. , Kostek, J. & Ziegelmeyer, E. (2013). Assessing post-cue exposure craving and its association with amount wagered in an optional betting task. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(3), 133137.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carvalho, J. , Stulhofer, A. , Vieira, A. L. & Jurin, T. (2015). Hypersexuality and high sexual desire: Exploring the structure of problematic sexuality. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 12(6), 13561367.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Conway, K. P. , Levy, J. , Vanyukov, M. , Chandler, R. , Rutter, J. , Swan, G. E. & Neale, M. (2010). Measuring addiction propensity and severity: The need for a new instrument. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 111, 412.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farre, J. M. , Fernandez-Aranda, F. , Granero, R. , Aragay, N. , Mallorquí-Bague, N. , Ferrer, V. , More, A. , Bouman, W. P. , Arcelus, J. , Savvidou, L. G. , Penelo, E. , Aymamí, M. N. , Gómez-Peña, M. , Gunnard, K. , Romaguera, A. , Menchón, J. M. , Vallès, V. & Jiménez-Murcia, S. (2015). Sex addiction and gambling disorder: Similarities and differences. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 5968.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fazzino, T. L. , Rose, G. L. , Burt, K. B. & Helzer, J. E. (2014). A test of the DSM-5 severity scale for alcohol use disorder. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 141, 3943.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final report. Ottawa, Ontario: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Hingson, R. W. , Heeren, T. & Winter, M. R. (2006). Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence: Age at onset, duration, and severity. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160(7), 739746.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kafka, M. P. (2010). Hypersexual Disorder: A proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(2), 377400.

  • Kor, A. , Fogel, Y. A. , Reid, R. C. & Potenza, M. N. (2013). Should hypersexual disorder be classified as an addiction? Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 20(1–2), 2747.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. & Rosenberg, H. (2014). The Pornography Craving Questionnaire: Psychometric properties. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 451462.

  • Ledgerwood, D. M. , Weinstock, J. , Morasco, B. J. & Petry, N. M. (2007). Clinical features and treatment prognosis of pathological gamblers with and without recent gambling-related illegal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 35(3), 294301.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moss, H. B. (2011). How do addiction psychiatrists think about “severity”? American Academy of Addiction Psychiatry Summer Newsletter. AAAP News, 7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Oei, T. P. & Gordon, L. M. (2008). Psychosocial factors related to gambling abstinence and relapse in members of gamblers anonymous. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(1), 91105.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Potenza, M. N. , Steinberg, M. A. , McLaughlin, S. D. , Rounsaville, B. J. & O’Malley, S. S. (2000). Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28(4), 389403.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raymond, N. C. , Coleman, E. & Miner, M. H. (2003). Psychiatric comorbidity and compulsive/impulsive traits in compulsive sexual behavior. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44(5), 370380.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. , Carpenter, B. N. , Hook, J. N. , Garos, S. , Manning, J. C. , Gilliland, R. , Cooper, E. B. , McKittrick, H. , Davtian, M. & Fong, T. W. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 Field Trial for Hypersexual Disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(11), 28682877.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. , Garos, S. & Fong, T. (2012). Psychometric development of the Hypersexual Behavior Consequences Scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(3), 115122.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. & Kafka, M. P. (2014). Controversies about Hypersexual Disorder and the DSM–5. Current Sexual Health Reports, 6, 259264.

  • Reid, R. C. , Stein, J. A. & Carpenter, B. N. (2011). Understanding the roles of shame and neuroticism in a patient sample of hypersexual men. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(4), 263267.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rogers, P. J. & Hendrik, J. S. (2000). Food craving and food “addiction”: A critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 66(1), 314.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Saha, T. D. , Chou, S. P. & Grant, B. F. (2006). Toward an alcohol use disorder continuum using item response theory: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychological Medicine, 36(7), 931941.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shiffman, S. , Stone, A. A. & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 132.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steele, V. , Staley, C. , Fong, T. & Prause, N. (2013) Sexual desire, not hypersexuality, is related to neurophysiological responses elicited by sexual images. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, 3, 20770.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Strong, D. R. & Kahler, C. W. (2007). Evaluation of the continuum of gambling problems using the DSM-IV. Addiction, 102(4), 713721.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tiffany, S. T. & Wray, J. (2009). The continuing conundrum of craving. Addiction, 104, 16181619.

  • Winters, J. , Christoff, K. & Gorzalka, B. B. (2010). Dysregulated sexuality and high sexual desire: Distinct constructs? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(5), 10291043.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, M. M. & Wohl, M. J. A. (2009). The Gambling Craving Scale: Psychometric validation and behavioral outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23(3), 512522.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ashrafioun, L. , Kostek, J. & Ziegelmeyer, E. (2013). Assessing post-cue exposure craving and its association with amount wagered in an optional betting task. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(3), 133137.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carvalho, J. , Stulhofer, A. , Vieira, A. L. & Jurin, T. (2015). Hypersexuality and high sexual desire: Exploring the structure of problematic sexuality. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 12(6), 13561367.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Conway, K. P. , Levy, J. , Vanyukov, M. , Chandler, R. , Rutter, J. , Swan, G. E. & Neale, M. (2010). Measuring addiction propensity and severity: The need for a new instrument. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 111, 412.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farre, J. M. , Fernandez-Aranda, F. , Granero, R. , Aragay, N. , Mallorquí-Bague, N. , Ferrer, V. , More, A. , Bouman, W. P. , Arcelus, J. , Savvidou, L. G. , Penelo, E. , Aymamí, M. N. , Gómez-Peña, M. , Gunnard, K. , Romaguera, A. , Menchón, J. M. , Vallès, V. & Jiménez-Murcia, S. (2015). Sex addiction and gambling disorder: Similarities and differences. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 5968.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fazzino, T. L. , Rose, G. L. , Burt, K. B. & Helzer, J. E. (2014). A test of the DSM-5 severity scale for alcohol use disorder. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 141, 3943.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final report. Ottawa, Ontario: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Hingson, R. W. , Heeren, T. & Winter, M. R. (2006). Age at drinking onset and alcohol dependence: Age at onset, duration, and severity. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160(7), 739746.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kafka, M. P. (2010). Hypersexual Disorder: A proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(2), 377400.

  • Kor, A. , Fogel, Y. A. , Reid, R. C. & Potenza, M. N. (2013). Should hypersexual disorder be classified as an addiction? Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 20(1–2), 2747.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. & Rosenberg, H. (2014). The Pornography Craving Questionnaire: Psychometric properties. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 451462.

  • Ledgerwood, D. M. , Weinstock, J. , Morasco, B. J. & Petry, N. M. (2007). Clinical features and treatment prognosis of pathological gamblers with and without recent gambling-related illegal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 35(3), 294301.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moss, H. B. (2011). How do addiction psychiatrists think about “severity”? American Academy of Addiction Psychiatry Summer Newsletter. AAAP News, 7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Oei, T. P. & Gordon, L. M. (2008). Psychosocial factors related to gambling abstinence and relapse in members of gamblers anonymous. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(1), 91105.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Potenza, M. N. , Steinberg, M. A. , McLaughlin, S. D. , Rounsaville, B. J. & O’Malley, S. S. (2000). Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 28(4), 389403.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raymond, N. C. , Coleman, E. & Miner, M. H. (2003). Psychiatric comorbidity and compulsive/impulsive traits in compulsive sexual behavior. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44(5), 370380.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. , Carpenter, B. N. , Hook, J. N. , Garos, S. , Manning, J. C. , Gilliland, R. , Cooper, E. B. , McKittrick, H. , Davtian, M. & Fong, T. W. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 Field Trial for Hypersexual Disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9(11), 28682877.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. , Garos, S. & Fong, T. (2012). Psychometric development of the Hypersexual Behavior Consequences Scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(3), 115122.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C. & Kafka, M. P. (2014). Controversies about Hypersexual Disorder and the DSM–5. Current Sexual Health Reports, 6, 259264.

  • Reid, R. C. , Stein, J. A. & Carpenter, B. N. (2011). Understanding the roles of shame and neuroticism in a patient sample of hypersexual men. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 199(4), 263267.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rogers, P. J. & Hendrik, J. S. (2000). Food craving and food “addiction”: A critical review of the evidence from a biopsychosocial perspective. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 66(1), 314.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Saha, T. D. , Chou, S. P. & Grant, B. F. (2006). Toward an alcohol use disorder continuum using item response theory: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychological Medicine, 36(7), 931941.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shiffman, S. , Stone, A. A. & Hufford, M. R. (2008). Ecological momentary assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 132.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steele, V. , Staley, C. , Fong, T. & Prause, N. (2013) Sexual desire, not hypersexuality, is related to neurophysiological responses elicited by sexual images. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, 3, 20770.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Strong, D. R. & Kahler, C. W. (2007). Evaluation of the continuum of gambling problems using the DSM-IV. Addiction, 102(4), 713721.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tiffany, S. T. & Wray, J. (2009). The continuing conundrum of craving. Addiction, 104, 16181619.

  • Winters, J. , Christoff, K. & Gorzalka, B. B. (2010). Dysregulated sexuality and high sexual desire: Distinct constructs? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(5), 10291043.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, M. M. & Wohl, M. J. A. (2009). The Gambling Craving Scale: Psychometric validation and behavioral outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23(3), 512522.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2024  
Scopus  
CiteScore  
CiteScore rank  
SNIP  
Scimago  
SJR index 2.26
SJR Q rank Q1

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Nov 2024 0 99 9
Dec 2024 0 109 12
Jan 2025 0 120 12
Feb 2025 0 142 10
Mar 2025 0 135 11
Apr 2025 0 45 17
May 2025 0 9 14