Author:
Ralf C. Buckley International Chair in Ecotourism Research, Griffith University, Gold Coast, Australia

Search for other papers by Ralf C. Buckley in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Individuals can display characteristics of behavioral addictions to nature and the outdoors as well as adventure activities. Research on mental health effects of nature exposure is relevant to research on nature and adventure addictions.

Abstract

Individuals can display characteristics of behavioral addictions to nature and the outdoors as well as adventure activities. Research on mental health effects of nature exposure is relevant to research on nature and adventure addictions.

At least some individual practitioners of adventure sports, recreation, and tourism display all the characteristic criteria of powerful behavioral addiction (Buckley, 2012, 2015a; Heirene, Shearer, Roderique-Davies, & Mellalieu, 2016). There are also many individuals, however, who take part in the same activities without exhibiting symptoms of addiction. In addition, I suggest that there are individuals who do exhibit symptoms of behavioral addiction, but where the addiction they display is not to a specific activity, but to the natural setting, nature and the outdoors. I, therefore, suggest that there is an opportunity to link research on behavioral addictions and research on the mental health effects of exposure to nature.

Most published research on adventure tourism and recreation makes no mention of addictions. At least 14 different distinct motivations for participation in these activities have been identified (Buckley, 2012). Broadly, these may be divided into external social motivations, and internal individual motivations. Social motivations include making friends, building image, and acquiring social capital. Internal motivations include thrill, nature appreciation, and various physical, mental, or emotional challenges. Social motivations predominate for the more numerous participants at lower skill levels, and internal motivations for the less numerous participants at higher skill levels (Buckley, 2012).

Individuals can develop behavioral addictions at any level of skill, but the most powerful addictions are shown by long-term expert practitioners, as exemplified for the climbers interviewed by Heirene et al. (2016). There are also many practitioners who have converted their skills to a career as guides or instructors, and still continue to practice the same activities when not at work, typically at higher skill levels.

Not everyone involved in adventure activities, however, becomes addicted. Some may try a particular activity but decide it is not for them. Others continue to take part occasionally, but without addiction, as for some of the climbers interviewed by Heirene et al. (2016). Some individuals are addicted to different activities at different times. From an autoethnographic perspective, for example (Buckley, 2015b), over the past four decades I myself have been addicted to hang gliding, sailboarding, whitewater kayaking, surfing, snowboarding, and kiteboarding at various times and intensities.

This raises three questions for further research. The first is about the activities: what is it that makes some activities more addictive than others? The second is about individual personalities: what is it that makes some people addicted and others not? The third is about process and mechanism: how exactly do individual people become addicted to particular activities, and what determines whether those addictions increase, maintain, or fade over time? For each of these questions, adventure sports, recreation, and tourism can provide ethically straightforward experimental opportunities that are relevant to research on behavioral addictions more broadly (Buckley, 2012). Equally, laboratory and clinical work on both substance and behavioral addictions is relevant in understanding these issues for adventure addictions specifically.

One possibility, for example, is that the addictive component is rush, a combination of thrill and flow, or thrill with skill (Buckley, 2012). If correct, this hypothesis might explain why high thrill but low skill activities such as bungy jumping or scary theme-park rides do not seem to generate the same behavioral addiction as skilled adventure activities. This cannot be the whole picture; however, since there are many low skill activities, notably including automated gaming-machine gambling, which do indeed generate behavioral addictions.

If thrill is important, then we can consider what personality characteristics and emotions may contribute to thrill experiences. There is extensive research on personality factors, most of it employing the thrill-seeking subscale of the sensation-seeking personality scale (Zuckerman, 2007). Sensation-seeking personalities are prevalent among practitioners of many outdoor adventure activities (Buckley, 2016), but the pattern is not universal. Thrill is driven at least partly by fear (Buckley, 2016), but with differences between individuals and differences between specific events and activities for the same individuals. Fear boosts thrill up to a certain threshold, but beyond that, fear increases and thrill disappears. More complex links between different types of fear, and between triumph and relief as well as thrill, remain to be investigated.

One aspect that does not yet seem to have been investigated at all within the adventure addiction literature, is the importance of the setting, the environment in which the activity takes place. In adventure psychology research, this aspect has only been considered as a contributing factor to risk, difficulty, and skill, for example, different grades of climb, different grades of whitewater rapids, or different types of surfing wave. Research on nature exposure and mental health, however, draws very fine distinctions between the types and degrees of exposure to either built or natural environments, and their effects on cognition, attention, memory, attitude, self-perceived quality of life, and similar factors (Berman et al., 2012; Bratman, Hamilton, Hahn, Daily, & Gross, 2015; Pearson & Craig, 2014).

We may legitimately ask, therefore, whether individuals can become addicted to nature, either jointly or independently of outdoor adventure activities. The evidence suggests that this is indeed possible, and merits research attention. Criteria for behavioral addictions (Lichtenstein, Larsen, Christiansen, Støving, & Bredahl, 2014; Marks, 1990) include: preoccupation, prioritization, cyclical craving and satisfaction, and withdrawal symptoms such as moodiness and inability to pay attention. The “videophilia” hypothesis of Pergams and Zaradic (2008) suggests that in urbanized developed nations, younger generations are losing interest in the outdoors, and instead paying attention to digital multimedia communication and entertainment devices. This hypothesis has been questioned (Balmford et al., 2009; Buckley, 2009); but even if it is indeed correct for some individuals, it does not apply universally.

There are many individuals who do indeed: plan and use their resources so as to spend time in nature, experience emotional distress and powerful nature cravings if confined indoors, experience strong withdrawal symptoms if they do not spend time in nature, and whose attention, memory, and cognition are all enhanced in nature, and depleted as time passes without a nature fix. All of this is entirely consistent with research on nature exposure and mental health, but has not previously been framed as a behavioral prediction. I propose that this would indeed be a valuable perspective, and one worth pursuing in conjunction with continuing research on behavioral addictions to adventure tourism, recreation, and sport.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • Balmford, A. , Beresford, J. , Green, J. , Naidoo, R. , Walpole, M. , & Manica, A. (2009). A global perspective on trends in nature-based tourism. PLoS Biology, 7(6), e1000144. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000144

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berman, M. G. , Kross, E. , Krpan, K. M. , Askren, M. K. , Burson, A. , Deldin, P. J. , Kaplan, S. , Sherdell, L. , Gotlib, I. H. , & Jonides, J. (2012). Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 140(3), 300305. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.012

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bratman, G. N. , Hamilton, J. P. , Hahn, K. S. , Daily, G. C. , & Gross, J. J. (2015). Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(28), 85678572. doi:10.1073/pnas.1510459112

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buckley, R. C. (2009). Parks and tourism. PLoS Biology, 7(6), e1000143. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000143

  • Buckley, R. C. (2012). Rush as a key motivation in skilled adventure tourism: Resolving the risk recreation paradox. Tourism Management, 33(4), 961970. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2011.10.002

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buckley, R. C. (2015 a). Adventure thrills are addictive. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1915. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01915

  • Buckley, R. C. (2015 b). Autoethnography helps analyse emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 209. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00209

  • Buckley, R. C. (2016). Qualitative analysis of emotions: Fear and thrill. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1187. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01187

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heirene, R. , Shearer, D. , Roderique-Davies, G. , & Mellalieu, S. (2016). Addiction in extreme sports: An exploration of withdrawal states in rock climbers. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 332341. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.039

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lichtenstein, M. B. , Larsen, K. S. , Christiansen, E. , Støving, R. K. , & Bredahl, T. V. G. (2014). Exercise addiction in team sport and individual sport: Prevalences and validation of the exercise addiction inventory. Addiction Research & Theory, 22(5), 431437. doi:10.3109/16066359.2013.875537

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marks, I. (1990). Behavioural (non-chemical) addictions. British Journal of Addiction, 85(11), 13891394. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01618.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pearson, D. G. , & Craig, T. (2014). The great outdoors? Exploring the mental health benefits of natural environments. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1178. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01178

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pergams, O. R. , & Zaradic, P. A. (2008). Evidence for a fundamental and pervasive shift away from nature-based recreation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(7), 22952300. doi:10.1073/pnas.0709893105

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risky behaviour. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Balmford, A. , Beresford, J. , Green, J. , Naidoo, R. , Walpole, M. , & Manica, A. (2009). A global perspective on trends in nature-based tourism. PLoS Biology, 7(6), e1000144. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000144

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berman, M. G. , Kross, E. , Krpan, K. M. , Askren, M. K. , Burson, A. , Deldin, P. J. , Kaplan, S. , Sherdell, L. , Gotlib, I. H. , & Jonides, J. (2012). Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 140(3), 300305. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.03.012

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bratman, G. N. , Hamilton, J. P. , Hahn, K. S. , Daily, G. C. , & Gross, J. J. (2015). Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual prefrontal cortex activation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(28), 85678572. doi:10.1073/pnas.1510459112

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buckley, R. C. (2009). Parks and tourism. PLoS Biology, 7(6), e1000143. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000143

  • Buckley, R. C. (2012). Rush as a key motivation in skilled adventure tourism: Resolving the risk recreation paradox. Tourism Management, 33(4), 961970. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2011.10.002

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buckley, R. C. (2015 a). Adventure thrills are addictive. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1915. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01915

  • Buckley, R. C. (2015 b). Autoethnography helps analyse emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 209. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00209

  • Buckley, R. C. (2016). Qualitative analysis of emotions: Fear and thrill. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1187. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01187

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heirene, R. , Shearer, D. , Roderique-Davies, G. , & Mellalieu, S. (2016). Addiction in extreme sports: An exploration of withdrawal states in rock climbers. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 332341. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.039

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lichtenstein, M. B. , Larsen, K. S. , Christiansen, E. , Støving, R. K. , & Bredahl, T. V. G. (2014). Exercise addiction in team sport and individual sport: Prevalences and validation of the exercise addiction inventory. Addiction Research & Theory, 22(5), 431437. doi:10.3109/16066359.2013.875537

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marks, I. (1990). Behavioural (non-chemical) addictions. British Journal of Addiction, 85(11), 13891394. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01618.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pearson, D. G. , & Craig, T. (2014). The great outdoors? Exploring the mental health benefits of natural environments. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1178. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01178

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pergams, O. R. , & Zaradic, P. A. (2008). Evidence for a fundamental and pervasive shift away from nature-based recreation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(7), 22952300. doi:10.1073/pnas.0709893105

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risky behaviour. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2024  
Scopus  
CiteScore  
CiteScore rank  
SNIP  
Scimago  
SJR index 2.26
SJR Q rank Q1

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Nov 2024 0 50 23
Dec 2024 0 49 7
Jan 2025 0 71 11
Feb 2025 0 62 9
Mar 2025 0 71 10
Apr 2025 0 28 16
May 2025 0 0 0