Authors:
Aviv Weinstein Department of Behavioral Science, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel

Search for other papers by Aviv Weinstein in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Hodaya Ben Abu Department of Behavioral Science, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel

Search for other papers by Hodaya Ben Abu in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Ayelet Timor Department of Behavioral Science, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel

Search for other papers by Ayelet Timor in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Yaniv Mama Department of Behavioral Science, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel

Search for other papers by Yaniv Mama in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

There is a previous evidence for impulsivity in individuals with Internet and Video Gaming Disorders. The aim of this study was to examine whether Internet and video game addictions are associated with experiential delay discounting, risk-taking, and sensitivity to social rejection using computerized tasks and questionnaires.

Methods

Twenty participants (mean age 24, SD = 1.55) with high score on the Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ) were compared with 20 participants (mean age 24.8, SD = 1.34) with low score on the POGQ. They performed on computerized Balloon Analog Risk Task and Experiential Delay discounting Task (EDT), and filled in the sensitivity to social rejection questionnaire.

Results

Participants with high POGQ scores had lower measures of delay discounting, higher measures of risk-taking, and higher measures of sensitivity to social rejection compared with participants with low POGQ scores.

Discussion

The results of this study support the previous evidence of risk-taking and provide new evidence for difficulties in delay discounting and sensitivity to social rejection among those who score high on Internet and video games.

Conclusions

The results suggest that Internet- and video game-addicted individuals seek immediate gratification and cannot wait for later reward. Furthermore, these individuals spend time in the virtual world, where they feel safe, and avoid social interactions presumably due to fears of social rejection.

Abstract

Background and aims

There is a previous evidence for impulsivity in individuals with Internet and Video Gaming Disorders. The aim of this study was to examine whether Internet and video game addictions are associated with experiential delay discounting, risk-taking, and sensitivity to social rejection using computerized tasks and questionnaires.

Methods

Twenty participants (mean age 24, SD = 1.55) with high score on the Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ) were compared with 20 participants (mean age 24.8, SD = 1.34) with low score on the POGQ. They performed on computerized Balloon Analog Risk Task and Experiential Delay discounting Task (EDT), and filled in the sensitivity to social rejection questionnaire.

Results

Participants with high POGQ scores had lower measures of delay discounting, higher measures of risk-taking, and higher measures of sensitivity to social rejection compared with participants with low POGQ scores.

Discussion

The results of this study support the previous evidence of risk-taking and provide new evidence for difficulties in delay discounting and sensitivity to social rejection among those who score high on Internet and video games.

Conclusions

The results suggest that Internet- and video game-addicted individuals seek immediate gratification and cannot wait for later reward. Furthermore, these individuals spend time in the virtual world, where they feel safe, and avoid social interactions presumably due to fears of social rejection.

Introduction

There is an increasing use of Internet and video games, such as “Grand Theft Auto,” “Mortal Combat,” “Call of duty,” and others, and this resulted in an increasing number of users worldwide and a rapidly growing industry (Király, Nagygyörgy, Griffiths, & Demetrovics, 2014; Kuss, 2013). Playing Internet and video games has become one of the most popular leisure activities regardless of culture, age, and gender (Király et al., 2014; Kuss, 2013). Playing multiplayer games on the Internet involves communication with players around the world, and it is considered social activity. In particular, males who were heavy players scored highly on the “preference to friends” need, but interestingly, they were also likely to see their friends more often outside school, thus providing no support for the theory that computer games are taking the place of normal social interaction (Colwell, Grady, & Rhaiti, 1995).

Other studies have shown the relationships between excessive video games early start and dependence (Fisher, 1994; Griffiths & Hunt, 1998) and that Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) involves excessive or poorly controlled preoccupations, urges, or behaviors regarding computer and video game play that lead to impairment or distress. There are three different models proposed for IGD: an impulse control disorder, an obsessive–compulsive disorder, and a behavioral addiction model (Grant, Potenza, Weinstein, & Gorelick, 2010; Weinstein & Aboujaude, 2015; Weinstein, Feder, Rosenberg, & Dannon, 2014). The behavioral addiction model argues that IGD shows the features of excessive use despite adverse consequences, withdrawal phenomena, and tolerance that characterize substance use disorders. In the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), IGD is identified in Section III as a condition warranting more clinical research and experience before it might be considered for inclusion as a formal disorder (see Weinstein & Aboujaoude, 2015; Weinstein et al., 2014 for review).

It has also been argued that Internet and video game playing should be classified as an impulse control disorder because players often cannot control their urges to play. This model is supported by evidence that individuals who become addicted to computer and video games find it hard to quit or reduce their online gaming despite the efforts they make (Hellman, Schoenmakers, Nordstrom, & van Holst, 2013). IGD is therefore characterized as a compulsive or excessive use of the Internet without control that can interfere with daily activity and can result in harm or distress (Weinstein et al., 2014; Weinstein & Lejoyeux, 2010). It has been argued that Internet and video games are means for satisfaction excitement and reward similar to other behavioral addictions (Griffiths, 2008).

First, consistent with the view that IGD should be considered a behavioral addiction there is the notion that individuals who are addicted to Internet and video games are impulsive and cannot control their urges and prefer immediate reward to long-term gains. Previous studies have shown that impulsivity is enhanced in substance use disorders (Perry & Carrol, 2008; Robbins, Gillan, Smith, de Wit, & Ersche, 2012). There is further evidence that Internet-addicted individuals are characterized by impulsive behavior (Cao, Su, Liu, & Gao, 2007) and that they prefer small immediate rewards to large delayed rewards (Irvine et al., 2013).

There are several paradigms that have been used to investigate risk-taking, such as the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) (Lejuez et al., 2002), Bechara’s gambling task (Iowa Gambling Task, IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 2005), or a task that measures behavioral choice (Mitchell, 1999). In this study, we have used a task that measures risk-taking – the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) (Lejuez et al., 2002). This task has been used extensively in drug, alcohol, nicotine addiction as well as gambling, kleptomania aggression, and unprotected sex in adolescents and adults (Hunt, 2005). The task measures the tendency to pre-empt responses and to respond quickly to external stimuli (balloons).

Second, consistent with the impulsivity hypothesis for IGD, we have investigated the process of delay discounting among individuals with IGD. Studies on drug addiction and behavioral addiction, such as gambling, have shown evidence for delay discounting (Bickel, Odum, & Madden, 1999; Madden, Petry, Badger, & Bickel, 1997; Vuchinich & Simpson, 1998). It is established that there is an association between IGD and delay discounting because Internet use gives immediate reward on the expense of social and professional activities that yield long-term rewards (Irvine et al., 2013; Saville, Gisbert, Kopp, & Telesco, 2011; Young, 2004). It is therefore important to investigate delay discounting among individuals with IGD. Few studies have attempted to compare the performance on the BART, IGT, and Experiential Delay discounting Task (EDT) in IGD (Xu, Korczykowski, Zhu, & Rao, 2013), and the reliability of the trials on these tasks was mixed. Hence, there is a need to compare risk-taking and delay discounting two facets of impulsivity in individuals with IGD.

Finally, although people often report that the Internet connects them socially and enables communication, it also paradoxically reduces individual involvement in social relations and may lead into isolation and lack of social contact (Davis, Flett, & Besser, 2002). Consequently, those who are addicted to the Internet spend more time by playing multiplayer games rather than using other means for communication, such as email or social networks (Young, 1998). We have decided to investigate one of the consequences of IGD that is social isolation and impairment in interpersonal relationships. This was done using the sensitivity to social rejection or interpersonal sensitivity. These two constructs describe a cognitive–affective worry of rejection that can impair well-being and interpersonal function.

People who have fears and doubts of acceptance by others and who expect rejection act in more hostile, aggressive ways in relationships compared with more secure individuals (e.g., Downey, Feldman, & Ayduk, 2000; Downey, Freitas, Michaelis, & Khouri, 1998; Dutton, Saunders, Staromski, & Bartholomew, 1994; Gaines et al., 1997; Mikulincer, 1998). They experience more troubled and dissatisfying relationships that end sooner (e.g., Downey & Feldman, 1996; Downey et al., 1998; Simpson, Ickes, & Grich, 1999) and are more susceptible to loneliness, social anxiety, and depression following rejection (e.g., Ayduk, Downey, & Kim, 2001; Baldwin, 1994; Cooper, Shaver, & Collins, 1998; Hammen, Burge, Daley, & Davila, 1995; Kobak & Sceery, 1988; Shaver & Hazan, 1987; Simpson, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996).

Rejection sensitivity may also be associated with pathological gambling. Pathological gambling has a close relationship with rejection as both can stem from physiological or emotional pain. Rejection can also cause people to feel socially isolated, and this can be enhanced by further isolation during gambling. The association between pathological gambling and rejection sensitivity needs to be examined further.

It is therefore important to investigate whether IGD is associated with sensitivity to social rejection or interpersonal sensitivity.

It was hypothesized that Internet- and video game-addicted individuals would have higher impulsivity, delay discounting, and higher levels of sensitivity to social rejection compared with non-addicted individuals.

Methods

Procedure

Participants

There were 40 participants and they were split into two groups: 20 participants in the high Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ) score group (19 males and 1 female) and 20 participants in the low POGQ score group (19 males and 1 female) with the age range of 22–28. The high POGQ score group had the mean age of 24 years and 1 month, and in the low POGQ score group, the mean age was 24 years and 10 months. Participants were recruited using a “snowball” method or “a friend brings a friend” method and were not rewarded for participation. They were required to fill in questionnaires on Google forms and to perform on the computerized tasks for 1 h. The experiment was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Ariel University.

Questionnaires

Three questionnaires were administered in this study. First, a demographic questionnaire with personal details, such as age, sex, and years of education. Second, the Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ) – Short Form that assessed online gaming. The questionnaire has 12 items to measure problematic use of computer games (Pápay et al., 2013). The original questionnaire had 18 items (Demetrovics et al., 2012). The questionnaire measures occupation, excessive use, degree of immersion in the virtual reality of the game, social isolation, interpersonal conflicts, and regression. The short form contains 2 items for each of these sub-scales. Ratings are from 1 “never” to 5 “always.” The reliability of the POGQ – Short Form in this study was Cronbach’s α = .90.

Third, the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire (RSQ) was developed by Downey and Feldman (1996). The questionnaire measures personal sensitivity to rejection in social situations. The participants read 18 hypothetical interpersonal descriptions of situations of possible rejection from significant others (e.g., “you ask your partner to move in with you”). They were required to rate from 1 to 6 how worried they are about asking their partner and how high they rate their expectations that the other will respect or accept the request. Score was calculated by multiplying the probability of rejection in each situation with the degree of worry about the request. The questionnaire had a Cronbach’s α of .78 in this study.

Computerized tasks

  1. 1. Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART) [developed by Lejuez et al. (2002)]: The participants sat in a quiet room with neutral background and were required to perform the experiment. They were requested to fill the air in a virtual balloon by pressing the “space” key on the computer board to expand it without exploding the balloon. A certain sum of money was given for each trial. The participant can choose to cash the sum by pressing the “enter” key on the computer board (“pump”). All the money that was acquired is lost if the balloon explodes. There were 30 trials for each participant. The game was finished with a message for each participant with the amount gained on the task.
  2. 2. Experiential Delay discounting Task (EDT) [developed by Rachlin, Raineri, and Cross (1991)]: The participants sat in a quiet room with neutral background and were required to perform the experiment. The participants were presented with two sums of money: a “large” sum of 1.2 that is uncertain and delayed, and a “small” sum that is changeable and immediate but lower than 1.2. If they decide to cash the money, they should press the square in which the money is displayed. There were four rounds with 15 trials in each round with different delay times for each trial (1, 5, 10, and 20 s).

Analysis of the results of the BART

The score is calculated based on three parameters: (a) number of trials without explosion, (b) number of trials with explosion − number of trials without explosion, and (c) the sum of money acquired. This task has a Cronbach’s α reliability of .87 (Ronay & Kim, 2006).

Analysis of the results of the EDT

Delay discounting score was calculated based on counting all choices and times of delay following the method described by Saville et al. (2011) and Reynolds and Schiffbauer (2004). Delay discounting curves are plotted from indifference points. An indifference point was defined as the point during a block of choices when the standard- and adjusting-option amounts were of equal subjective value at a certain delay to receive the standard option. Discounting data are well characterized by the hyperbolic model (Mazur, 1987), notated as follows:
Value=A/(1+kD),
where Value represents the value of the delayed reinforce, and A and D are the amount of reinforcer and length of delay to its delivery, respectively. The term k is a free parameter and indicates the steepness of the discount curve. Higher k-values indicate more rapid discounting that has been defined as more impulsive (e.g., Mazur, 1987; Rachlin, 2000; Richards, Zhang, Mitchell, & de Wit, 1999). A number of studies have shown that patterns of discounting by delay are better characterized (i.e., are fit better) by a hyperbolic function than by an exponential function (e.g., Kirby & Herrnstein, 1995; Myerson & Green, 1995; Rachlin et al., 1991; Richards et al., 1999), which is notated as follows:
Value=AekD,
where again A is the amount of reinforcer, D is the delay to receive the reinforcer, and k is the free parameter indicating steepness of the discount function. The exponential model has historically been the standard of rational choice in the field of economics (e.g., Loewenstein, 1992; Samuelson, 1937). Findings of better fits with a hyperbolic, non-rational model take on importance in the study of impulsive behavior. The quality of the fit index (R2) is determined by a method described by Reynolds and Schiffbauer (2004). A Wilcoxon signed-rank test is used to ensure that the hyperbolic and exponential models did not differ significantly across conditions, thus, justifying the combining of these data. All individual R2 values from Eqs. (1) and (2) were compared using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The Wilcoxon signed-rank test was also used to examine the sleep deprivation effects on the delay discounting task with individual k-values from the model of best fit as the outcome variable (Reynolds & Schiffbauer, 2004).

Ethics

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB, Helsinki Committee) of the Ariel University. All participants signed an informed consent form.

Results

Table 1 shows means on the POGQ, BART, delay discounting task, and RSQs in the high POGQ and low POGQ groups (SD in brackets).

Table 1.

Means on the POGQ, BART, delay discounting task, and Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaires in the high and low POGQ groups (SD in brackets)

Questionnaires and tasks, mean (SD) High POGQ (n = 20) Low POGQ (n = 20) Cohen’s d Between group comparison
POGQ 42 (5) 13 (2.92) 7.08 t(1, 19) = 4.54, p < .001
BART
 Number of trials with explosion 633 (182) 398 (168) 1.34 t(1, 19) = 4.34, p < .001
 Number of trials without explosion 464 (87) 344 (143) 1.01 t(1, 19) = 3.62, p < .01
 Number of key pressing (pumps) that results in balloon explosion − number of key pressing (pumps) that does not result in balloon explosion 169 (136) 54 (43) 1.14 t(1, 19) = 3.36, p < .01
 Mean number of key pressing 31.61 (2.03) 19.89 (8.37) 1.92 t(1, 19) = 4.34, p < .001
 Mean number of explosions 6 (2.714) 3.3 (1.26) 1.28 t(1, 19) = 4.24, p < .001
Delay discounting 0.038 (0.03) 0.019 (0.016) 0.68 t(2, 16) = 2.16, p < .05
Rejection sensitivity 9.29 (1.64) 5.26 (2.06) 2.16 t(1, 19) = 6.57, p < .001

Note. POGQ = Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire; BART = Balloon Analog Risk Task.

Between group comparison of all measures

The BART

First, a comparison of scores on the BART between the high and low POGQ control groups showed that the high POGQ group scored higher on risk-taking measure on the task (number of key pressing that results in balloon explosion − number of key pressing that does not result in balloon explosion) than the low POGQ group [t(1, 19) = 3.36, p < .01]. Cohen’s d = 0.68. Cohen’s d is calculated as
kgroup1kgroup2/SD(group1+group2).

Second, a comparison of the average number of key pressing in the high and low POGQ groups showed that the high POGQ group had average number of more key pressings than the low POGQ group [t(1, 19) = 4.34, p < .001]. The mean number of inflations of the high and low POGQ groups was 31.61 (SD = 9.09) and 19.89 (SD = 8.4), respectively.

Third, a comparison of the mean number of explosions between the groups showed that the high POGQ group made more explosions than the low POGQ group [t(1, 19) = 4.24, p < .001]. Figure 1 shows differences in performance on the BART between the high and low POGQ groups. It indicates higher number of overall key pressing and risky key pressing in the high POGQ group compared with the low POGQ group.

Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Differences in performance on the BART between the high and low POGQ groups

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 5, 4; 10.1556/2006.5.2016.081

Delay discounting

There was a significant difference between the high and low POGQ groups in the k-value (slope of the delay discounting curve) [t(2, 16) = 2.16, p < .05].

Total mean k score of the high and low POGQ groups was 0.038 (SD = 0.033) and 0.019 (SD = 0.016), respectively. There was also a significant difference between the high and low POGQ groups in R2 [t(1, 16) = 2.26, p < .05]. Total mean R2 score of the high and low POGQ groups was 0.81 (SD = 0.16) and 0.65 (SD = 0.22), respectively. Cohen’s d for R2 was 0.81.

Figure 2 shows differences in average group performance on the delay discounting task between the high and low POGQ groups. It indicates a much steeper slope of delay discounting in the high POGQ group compared with the low POGQ group.

Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Differences in average group performance on the delay discounting task between the high and low POGQ groups

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 5, 4; 10.1556/2006.5.2016.081

Rejection sensitivity

A comparison between the high and low POGQ groups on scores of the RSQ showed that the high POGQ group scored higher on the questionnaire than the low POGQ group [t(1, 19) = 6.8, p < .001]. The total mean R2 score of the high and low POGQ groups was 9.29 (SD = 1.64) and 5.26 (SD = 2.06), respectively.

Discussion

The results of the study demonstrated that participants with high problematic Internet and video game use compared with low problematic Internet and video game use had higher scores on all counts of risk-taking that were tested on the BART, namely higher number of total key pressing (“pumps”), higher score of the difference between explosion and non-explosion pressing, and total gains on the task. These results support the early findings of error processing and response inhibition in excessive computer game players by Littel et al. (2012). It is possible that IGD results in risk-taking behavior that indicates impulse control problems (Baumeister, Vohs, & Tice, 2007). Risk-taking behavior has also been strongly associated with sensation seeking and impulsivity (Lejuez et al., 2002, 2003).

Impulsivity is a multidimensional construct that can characterize the inclination of a person to succumb to urges or inability to plan or consider the possible options before making a decision (Kirby & Finch, 2010). Impulsivity has also been described as an inclination to look for excitement or adventure, impatience, and inability to evaluate the consequences of one’s actions and to behave inappropriately without inhibition (Reynolds, Ortengren, Richards, & de Wit, 2006; Reynolds, Penfold, & Patak, 2008). The results of this study indicate that individuals with Internet and Video Gaming Disorder have risk-taking behavior that is an indication for vulnerability to mental disorders, a finding that has major clinical implications.

The previous studies have shown that impulsivity is enhanced in various psychiatric disorders including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, substance dependence, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and eating disorders (Perry & Carrol, 2008; Robbins et al., 2012). Research using personality measures of impulsivity (a trait related to disinhibition, approach motivation, novelty seeking, and sensation seeking) points to the importance of impulsivity as a temperamental vulnerability factor for substance use (Acton, 2003). It was proposed that common genetic liability to behavioral disinhibition is expressed in part through brain mechanisms related to cognitive control, impulsivity, and sensitivity to reward, all of which are maturing during adolescence and during this period, problem behaviors emerge, including the initiation of substance use (Iacono, Malone, & McGue, 2008).

Among the behavioral addictions, Internet-addicted individuals are more impulsive than non-addicted individuals (Cao et al., 2007). A study using the IGT found that Internet-addicted individuals have deficits in decision-making function, chiefly a strategy learning lag rather than an inability to learn from task contingencies. They showed better performance on a Go/No-Go task, suggesting some dissociation between mechanisms of decision making and those of prepotent response inhibition in IGD (Sun et al., 2009). Contrary results were shown in another study that has investigated decision-making (IGT), potential to take risks (BART), and personality of college students with Internet addiction (Ko et al., 2010). Performance on the gambling task indicated better decision-making, and performance on the BART indicated less risk-taking behavior in Internet-addicted individuals. Their scores on Cloninger’s (1994) Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire showed lower reward dependence and higher novelty seeking. These findings of higher performance on the IGT differentiate this Internet addiction group from substance use, pathological gamblers, and Internet-addicted individuals who have been shown to be deficient in decision-making on the Iowa test. There is further evidence for the association between excessive video game playing and impulsivity showing that those who spend more time playing video games subsequently have more attention problems (Gentile, Swing, Lim, & Khoo, 2012). IGD has also been associated with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in school children (Weinstein, Yaacov, et al., 2015). These findings imply that IGD can result in faulty impulsive decisions with negative consequences for task completions on an information sampling and a delay discounting task (Irvine et al., 2013).

Consistent with the previous evidence (Irvine et al., 2013), participants of the high POGQ group also had impaired delay discounting, which is often described as the reduction in value of reward as a function of an increase in the delay time before getting it (Madden, Francisco, Brewer, & Stein, 2011). Extreme delay discounting exemplifies an impulsive choice that prefers small but immediate reward to a large delayed reward (Madden et al., 2011). On the other hand, self-control happens when an organism prefers the delayed but larger reward (Ainslie, 1975; Logue, 1988; Mischel & Ebbeson, 1970; Rachlin, 2000; Skinner, 1953). The inability to delay discounting or alternatively the subjective reduction in evaluation of reward when it is delayed is a mechanism that stands as the basis of impulsive behavior (Bickel & Marsch, 2001; Critchfield & Kollins, 2001; Green & Myerson, 2004; Logue, 1988). Our results are in conformity with the previous evidence for delay discounting in opiate-dependent patients (Madden et al., 1997), social and problem alcohol drinkers (Vuchinich & Simpson, 1998), nicotine smokers (Bickel et al., 1999), and pathological gamblers (Madden et al., 2011). It is also established that there is an association between IGD and delay discounting because Internet use gives immediate reward on the expense of social and professional activities that yield long-term rewards (Saville et al., 2011; Young, 2004) and that they prefer small immediate rewards to large delayed rewards (Irvine et al., 2013). Although participants with IGD preferred short-term gains to long-term higher rewards in this study, no causality can be inferred from the results. Nevertheless, we interpret the results as indicating that spending long periods of time in highly immediately rewarding games may affect the player inasmuch that they would prefer playing games that give immediate rewards to participate in activities that may only pay off in the long run, such as social and personal activities or relationships.

Although participants with high POGQ scores had high rates of risk-taking compared with those with low POGQ scores, they scored higher on the task and it seems that their risk-taking behavior paid off. On the other hand, lower delay discounting meant lower gains on the task. Xu et al. (2013) suggested that risk-related cognitive processing mediating the BART behavior may be different than the delay discounting. Delay discounting refers to the degree to which the subjective value of a commodity decreases as a function of a delay in its delivery. However, the BART refers to risk-taking on a continuum in which risk is problematic only after a certain point, and that point varies across different trials. In other words, delay discounting depends on time delay, whereas risk-taking depends on a certain threshold to be crossed. It is difficult to extrapolate from these tasks to real life, but it is plausible that taking risks until a certain point may be advantageous for some situations, but the inability to delay gratification over a period of time may not.

Finally, the sensitivity to social rejection or interpersonal sensitivity in the high POGQ group indicated one of the consequences of IGD that is social isolation and impairment in interpersonal relationships. These two constructs describe a cognitive–affective worry of rejection, which can impair well-being and interpersonal function. They can result in avoidant personality disorder, social anxiety disorder, or borderline personality disorder (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Although people often report that the Internet connects them socially and enables communication, it also paradoxically reduces individual involvement in social relations and may lead into isolation and lack of social contact (Davis et al., 2002; Weinstein, Dorani, et al., 2015). Individuals lacking social skills who are being exposed to the Internet develop IGD that results in lack of face-to-face interactions (Caplan, 2002, 2003; Davis, 2001; Davis et al., 2002). Caplan (2003) has found that some people find online interaction to be safer, more efficient, and more convenient than real-life interaction, and this can be liberated to individuals who are socially inhibited. That in turn makes it harder for them to develop relationships outside the computer environment (Davis et al, 2002). Others argued that Internet and video games are acceptable means for social activity (Brus, 2013).

Studies showed that those who are addicted to the Internet spend more time playing multiplayer games rather than using other means for communication, such as email or social networks (Young, 1998). IGD results in behavioral and social problems as well as problems in academic achievements (Morahan-Martin & Schumacher, 2000; Scherer, 1997; Young, 1998). The higher scores of rejection sensitivity among the IGD group support the evidence so far the participants with IGD find Internet and video games as an escape from daily life into a virtual reality in which they control their social interactions that might lead to social rejection they fear most. Responses to social rejection include hostility, depression, emotional regression, and jealousy and on the hand over politeness. These responses are also typical of various psychiatric conditions, such as avoidant personality disorder, social anxiety, and borderline personality disorder (Downey & Feldman, 1996). Rejection sensitivity has also been shown to predict the course and outcome of depression. Greater interpersonal sensitivity is associated with more depressive symptoms (Ayduk et al., 2001; Downey & Feldman, 1996), greater severity and duration of current major depressive episodes (Posternak & Zimmerman, 2001), increased propensity toward depression over time (Boyce, Parker, Barnett, Cooney, & Smith, 1991), and among people with clinical depression, decreased likelihood of being clinically remitted at 1-year follow-up (Boyce et al, 1992). Recent studies have found that increased rejection sensitivity during depressive episodes was linked to increased physical pain in both unipolar and bipolar depressions (Ehnvall et al., 2011; Ehnvall, Mitchell, Hadzi-Pavlovic, Malhi, & Parker, 2009). Finally, people with bipolar I disorder reported higher rejection sensitivity scores than the control participants (Ng & Johnson, 2013). Within the bipolar sample, rejection sensitivity at baseline predicted increased depression but not mania, over the following 6 months; heightened rejection sensitivity was also correlated with poorer quality of life, social support, and psychological well-being.

Limitations

This study had several limitations including a relatively small sample of participants. Furthermore, participants were divided into IGD and non-IGD groups based on questionnaire ratings rather than placing the participants on a spectrum of Internet and video game addictions which may include those who are at risk for developing IGD. Finally, the participants knew that the tasks are only games and did not get monetary reward that would influence their decisions.

Conclusions

The results of this study demonstrated that individuals with a high score of measure of IGD display risk-taking, delay discounting and high measures of sensitivity to social rejection. These findings support the argument that IGD results in faulty decision-making and preference for short-term gains at the cost of long-term gains resulting from academic and social activities. The novelty of the study is using two different paradigms that assess two components of impulsivity – risk-taking and delay discounting, in individuals with high and low IGD together with a measure of rejection sensitivity. The clinical implication of the study is that IGD results in dysfunctional decision-making process and social–emotional interaction indicated by high rejection sensitivity. Favoring risk-taking and short-term gains together with rejection sensitivity is detrimental to long-term academic, work, and social life achievements and may result in propensity to have conduct disorders, drug and alcohol addiction, and mood disorders. It is difficult to determine whether individuals who are drawn into IGD have higher traits of risk-taking, delay discounting, and rejection sensitivity or that excessive playing of these games causes or enhances such traits. Perhaps a longitudinal study could answer these issues of causality.

Authors’ contribution

All individuals included as the authors of the paper have substantially contributed to the scientific process leading up to the writing of the paper. The authors have also contributed to the conception and design of the project, performance of the experiments, analysis and interpretation of the results, and preparing the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no interests or activities that might be seen as influencing the research (e.g., financial interests in a test or procedure and funding by pharmaceutical companies for research).

Acknowledgements

This study was presented in the 3rd ICBA meeting in Geneva, Switzerland in March 2016.

References

  • Acton, G. S. (2003). Measurement of impulsivity in a hierarchical model of personality traits: Implications for substance use. Substance Use and Misuse, 38(1), 6783. doi:10.1081/JA-120016566

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82(4), 463496. doi:10.1037/h0076860

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ayduk, O. , Downey, G. , & Kim, M. (2001). Rejection sensitivity and depression in women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(7), 868877. doi:10.1177/0146167201277009

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baldwin, M. W. (1994). Primed relational schemas as a source of self-evaluative reactions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13(4), 380403. doi:10.1521/jscp.1994.13.4.380

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baumeister, R. F. , Vohs, K. D. , & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bechara, A. , Damasio, H. , Tranel, D. , & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: Some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 159162. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.002

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bickel, W. K. , & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96(1), 7386. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bickel, W. K. , Odum, A. L. , & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 447454. doi:10.1007/PL00005490

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boyce, P. , Hickie, I. , Parker, G. , Mitchell, P. , Wilhelm, K. , & Brodaty, H. (1992). Interpersonal sensitivity and the one-year outcome of a depressive episode. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 26(2), 156161. doi:10.3109/00048679209072022

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boyce, P. , Parker, G. , Barnett, B. , Cooney, M. , & Smith, F. (1991). Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159(1), 106114. doi:10.1192/bjp.159.1.106

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brus, A. (2013). A young people’s perspective on computer game addiction. Addiction Research & Theory, 21(5), 365375. doi:10.3109/16066359.2012.733466

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cao, F. , Su, L. , Liu, T. , & Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. European Psychiatry, 22(7), 466471. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Caplan, S. E. (2002). Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: Development of a theory based cognitive-behavioral measurement instrument. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(5), 553575. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00004-3

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Caplan, S. E. (2003). Preference for online social interaction: A theory of problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being. Communication Research, 30(6), 625648. doi:10.1177/0093650203257842

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cloninger, C. R. (1994). The temperament and character inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Colwell, J. , Grady, C. , & Rhaiti, S. (1995). Computer games, self-esteem and gratification of needs in adolescents. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 5(3), 195206. doi:10.1002/casp.2450050308

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cooper, M. L. , Shaver, P. R. , & Collins, N. L. (1998). Attachment styles, emotion regulation, and adjustment in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 13801397. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1380

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Critchfield, T. S. , & Kollins, S. H. (2001). Temporal discounting: Basic research and the analysis of socially important behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 101122. doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-101

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 17(2), 187195. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, R. A. , Flett, G. L. , & Besser, A. (2002). Validation of a new scale for measuring problematic Internet use: Implications for pre-employment screening. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 5(4), 331345. doi:10.1089/109493102760275581

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demetrovics, Z. , Urbán, R. , Nagygyörgy, K ., Farkas, J., Griffiths, M. D., Pápay, O., Kökönyei, G., Felvinczi, K., & Oláh, A. (2012). Development of the problematic online gaming questionnaire (POGQ). PLoS ONE, 7(5), e36417. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036417

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , & Feldman, S. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 13271343. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1327

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , Feldman, S. , & Ayduk, O. (2000). Rejection sensitivity and male violence in romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 7(1), 4561. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00003.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , Freitas, A. L. , Michaelis, B. , & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 545560. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.545

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dutton, D. G. , Saunders, K. , Staromski, A. , & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Intimacy anger and insecure attachment as precursors of abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(15), 13671386. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01554.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ehnvall, A. , Mitchell, P. B. , Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. , Loo, C. , Breakspear, M. , Wright, A. , Roberts, G. , Frankland, A. , & Corry, J. (2011). Pain and rejection sensitivity in bipolar depression. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 5966. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00892.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ehnvall, A. , Mitchell, P. B. , Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. , Malhi, G. S. , & Parker, G. (2009). Pain during depression and relationship to rejection sensitivity. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 119(5), 375382. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01316.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fisher, S. (1994). Identifying video game addiction in children and adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 19(5), 545553. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)90010-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaines, S. O. Jr. , Reis, H. T. , Summers, S. , Rusbult, C. E. , Cox, C. L. , Wexler, M. O. , Marelich, W. D. , & Kudand, G. J. (1997). Impact of attachment style on reactions to accommodative dilemmas in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 4(2), 93113. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1997.tb00133.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. , Swing, E. , Lim, C. , & Khoo, A. (2012). Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: Evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(1), 6270. doi:10.1037/a0026969

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grant, J. E. , Potenza, M. N. , Weinstein, A. , & Gorelick, D. A. (2010). Introduction to behavioral addictions. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 233241. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491884

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Green, L. , & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769792. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2008). Videogame addiction: Further thoughts and observations. International Journal of Mental Health Addiction, 6(2), 182185. doi:10.1007/s11469-007-9128-y

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. , & Hunt, N. (1998). Dependence on computer games by adolescents. Psychological Reports, 82(2), 475480. doi:10.2466/pr0.1998.82.2.475

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hammen, C. L. , Burge, D. , Daley, S. E. , & Davila, J. (1995). Interpersonal attachment cognitions and prediction of symptomatic responses to interpersonal stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(3), 436443. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.104.3.436

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hellman, M. , Schoenmakers, T. M. , Nordstrom, B. R. , & van Holst, R. J. (2013). Is there such a thing as online video game addiction? A cross-disciplinary review. Addiction Research and Theory, 21(2), 102112. doi:10.3109/16066359.2012.693222

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hunt, M. (2005). Construct validity of the balloon analog risk task (BART): Associations with psychopathy and impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416428. doi:10.1177/1073191105278740

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Iacono, W. G. , Malone, S. M. , & McGue, M. (2008). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 325348. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Irvine, M. A. , Worbe, Y. , Bolton, S. , Harrison, N. A. , Bullmore, E. T. , & Voon, V. (2013). Impaired decisional impulsivity in pathological videogamers. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e75914. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075914

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Király, O. , Nagygyörgy, K. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). Problematic online gaming. In K. P. Rosenberg & L. C. Feder (Eds.), Behavioral addictions: Criteria, evidence and treatment (pp. 6197). Burlington, USA: Elsevier Science.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kirby, K. N. , & Finch, J. C. (2010). The hierarchical structure of self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(6), 704713. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.019

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kirby, K. N. , & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychological Science, 6, 8389. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00311.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Hsiao, S. , Liu, G. C. , Yen, J. U. , Yang, M. J. , & Yen, C. F. (2010). The characteristics of decision making, potential to take risks, and personality of college students with Internet addiction. Psychiatry Research, 175(1–2), 121125. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.004

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kobak, R. R. , & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59(1), 135146. doi:10.2307/1130395

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. (2013). Internet gaming addiction: Current perspectives. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 6, 125137. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S39476

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lejuez, C. W. , Aklin, W. M. , Jones, H. A. , Richards, J. R. , Strong, D. R. , Kahler, C. W. , & Read, J. P. (2003). The Balloon Analogue Risk Task differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 2633. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.11.1.26

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lejuez, C. W. , Read, J. P. , Kahler, C. W. , Richards, J. B. , Ramsey, S. E. , Stuart, G. L. , Strong, D. R. , & Brown, R. A. , (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 7584. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Littel, M. , Berg, I. , Luijten, M. , Rooij, A. J. , Keemink, L. , & Franken, I. H. (2012). Error processing and response inhibition in excessive computer game players: An event-related potential study. Addiction Biology, 17(5), 934947. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00467.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loewenstein, G. (1992). The fall and rise of psychological explanations in the economics of intertemporal choice. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 334). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Logue, A. W. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(4), 665679. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00053978

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Madden, G. J. , Francisco, M. T. , Brewer, A. T. , & Stein, J. S. (2011). Delay discounting and gambling. Behavioural Processes, 87(1), 4349. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2011.01.012

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Madden, G. J. , Petry, N. M. , Badger, G. J. , & Bickel, W. K. (1997). Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: Drug and monetary rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(3), 256262. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.256

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: The effects of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (Vol. 5, pp. 5573). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and individual differences in functional versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 513524. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.513

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mischel, W. , & Ebbeson, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329337. doi:10.1037/h0029815

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 455464. doi:10.1007/PL00005491

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morahan-Martin, J. , & Schumacher, P. (2000). Incidence and correlates of pathological Internet use among college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 16(1), 1329. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(99)00049-7

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Myerson, J. , & Green, L. (1995). Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 263276. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.64-263

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ng, T. H. , & Johnson, S. L. (2013). Rejection sensitivity is associated with quality of life, psychosocial outcome, and the course of depression in euthymic patients with bipolar I disorder. Cognitive Therapy Research, 37(6), 11691178. doi:10.1007/s10608-013-9552-1

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pápay, O. , Urbán, R. , Griffiths, M. , Nagygyörgy, K. , Farkas, J. , Kökönyei, G. , Felvinczi, K. , Oláh, A. , Elekes, Z. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2013). Psychometric properties of the problematic online gaming questionnaire short-form and prevalence of problematic online gaming in a national sample of adolescents. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(5), 340348. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0484

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Perry, J. L. , & Carrol, M. E. (2008) The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology, 200, 126. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Posternak, M. A. , & Zimmerman, M. (2001). Symptoms of atypical depression. Psychiatry Research, 104(2), 175181. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00301-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Rachlin, H. , Raineri, A. , & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233244. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , Ortengren, A. , Richards, J. B. , & de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(2), 305315. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.024

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , Penfold, R. B. , & Patak, M. (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: Laboratory behavioral assessments. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(2), 124131. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.16.2.124

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , & Schiffbauer, R. (2004). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: An experiential discounting task. Behavioural Processes, 67(3), 343356. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(04)00140-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Richards, J. B. , Zhang, L. , Mitchell, S. , & de Wit, H. (1999). Delay and probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71(2), 121143. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W. , Gillan, C. M. , Smith, D. G. , de Wit, S. , & Ersche, K. D. (2012) Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: Towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(1), 8191. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ronay, R. , & Kim, D. (2006). Gender differences in explicit and implicit risk attitudes: A socially facilitated phenomenon. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(2), 397419. doi:10.1348/014466605X66420

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155161. doi:10.2307/2967612

  • Saville, B. K. , Gisbert, A. , Kopp, J. , & Telesco, C. (2011). Internet addiction and delay discounting in college students. The Psychological Record, 60(2), 5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Scherer, K. (1997). College life on-line: Healthy and unhealthy Internet use. Journal of College Student Development, 38, 655665.

  • Shaver, P. , & Hazan, C. (1987). Being lonely, falling in love: Perspectives from attachment theory. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 105124.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simpson, J. A. , Ickes, W. , & Grich, J. (1999). When accuracy hurts: Reactions of anxious-ambivalent dating partners to a relationship threatening situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 754769. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.754

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simpson, J. A. , Rholes, W. S. , & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(5), 899914. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.5.899

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: The Free Press.

  • Sun, D. L. , Chen, Z. J. , Ma, N. , Zhang, X. C. , Fu, X. M. , & Zhang, D. R. (2009). Decision-making and prepotent response inhibition functions in excessive Internet users. CNS Spectrum, 14(2), 7581. doi:10.1017/S1092852900000225

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vuchinich, R. E. , & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 292305. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.6.3.292

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Aboujaoude, E. (2015). Problematic Internet use: An overview. In E. Aboujaoude & V. Starcevic (Eds.), Mental health in the digital age: Grave dangers, great promise (pp. 326). Oxford, USA: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Dorani, D. , Elhadif, R. , Bukovza, Y. , Yarmulnik, A. , & Dannon, P. (2015). Internet addiction is associated with social anxiety in young adults. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 27(1), 49.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Feder, K , Rosenberg, K. , & Dannon, P. (2014). Internet addiction disorder: Overview and controversies. In K. P. Rosenberg & L. C. Feder (Eds.), Behavioral addictions: Criteria, evidence and treatment (pp. 99117). Burlington, USA: Elsevier Science.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Lejoyeux, M. (2010). Internet addiction or excessive Internet use. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 277283. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491880

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Yaacov, Y. , Manning, M. , Danon, P. , & Weizman, A. (2015). Internet addiction and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) among school children. Israeli Medical Association Journal, 17(12), 731734.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, S. , Korczykowski, M. , Zhu, S. , & Rao, H. (2013). Assessment of risk-taking and impulsive behaviors: A comparison between three tasks. Social Behavior and Personality, 41(3), 477486. doi:10.2224/sbp.2013.41.3.477

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 1(3), 237244. doi:10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (2004). Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(4), 402415. doi:10.1177/0002764204270278

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Acton, G. S. (2003). Measurement of impulsivity in a hierarchical model of personality traits: Implications for substance use. Substance Use and Misuse, 38(1), 6783. doi:10.1081/JA-120016566

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82(4), 463496. doi:10.1037/h0076860

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ayduk, O. , Downey, G. , & Kim, M. (2001). Rejection sensitivity and depression in women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(7), 868877. doi:10.1177/0146167201277009

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baldwin, M. W. (1994). Primed relational schemas as a source of self-evaluative reactions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 13(4), 380403. doi:10.1521/jscp.1994.13.4.380

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baumeister, R. F. , Vohs, K. D. , & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bechara, A. , Damasio, H. , Tranel, D. , & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: Some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 159162. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.002

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bickel, W. K. , & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96(1), 7386. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bickel, W. K. , Odum, A. L. , & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 447454. doi:10.1007/PL00005490

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boyce, P. , Hickie, I. , Parker, G. , Mitchell, P. , Wilhelm, K. , & Brodaty, H. (1992). Interpersonal sensitivity and the one-year outcome of a depressive episode. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 26(2), 156161. doi:10.3109/00048679209072022

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Boyce, P. , Parker, G. , Barnett, B. , Cooney, M. , & Smith, F. (1991). Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159(1), 106114. doi:10.1192/bjp.159.1.106

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brus, A. (2013). A young people’s perspective on computer game addiction. Addiction Research & Theory, 21(5), 365375. doi:10.3109/16066359.2012.733466

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cao, F. , Su, L. , Liu, T. , & Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. European Psychiatry, 22(7), 466471. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Caplan, S. E. (2002). Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: Development of a theory based cognitive-behavioral measurement instrument. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(5), 553575. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(02)00004-3

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Caplan, S. E. (2003). Preference for online social interaction: A theory of problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being. Communication Research, 30(6), 625648. doi:10.1177/0093650203257842

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cloninger, C. R. (1994). The temperament and character inventory (TCI): A guide to its development and use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Colwell, J. , Grady, C. , & Rhaiti, S. (1995). Computer games, self-esteem and gratification of needs in adolescents. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 5(3), 195206. doi:10.1002/casp.2450050308

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cooper, M. L. , Shaver, P. R. , & Collins, N. L. (1998). Attachment styles, emotion regulation, and adjustment in adolescence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 13801397. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1380

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Critchfield, T. S. , & Kollins, S. H. (2001). Temporal discounting: Basic research and the analysis of socially important behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34(1), 101122. doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-101

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 17(2), 187195. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, R. A. , Flett, G. L. , & Besser, A. (2002). Validation of a new scale for measuring problematic Internet use: Implications for pre-employment screening. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 5(4), 331345. doi:10.1089/109493102760275581

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Demetrovics, Z. , Urbán, R. , Nagygyörgy, K ., Farkas, J., Griffiths, M. D., Pápay, O., Kökönyei, G., Felvinczi, K., & Oláh, A. (2012). Development of the problematic online gaming questionnaire (POGQ). PLoS ONE, 7(5), e36417. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036417

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , & Feldman, S. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 13271343. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1327

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , Feldman, S. , & Ayduk, O. (2000). Rejection sensitivity and male violence in romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 7(1), 4561. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00003.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Downey, G. , Freitas, A. L. , Michaelis, B. , & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(2), 545560. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.545

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dutton, D. G. , Saunders, K. , Staromski, A. , & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Intimacy anger and insecure attachment as precursors of abuse in intimate relationships. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24(15), 13671386. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1994.tb01554.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ehnvall, A. , Mitchell, P. B. , Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. , Loo, C. , Breakspear, M. , Wright, A. , Roberts, G. , Frankland, A. , & Corry, J. (2011). Pain and rejection sensitivity in bipolar depression. Bipolar Disorders, 13(1), 5966. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00892.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ehnvall, A. , Mitchell, P. B. , Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. , Malhi, G. S. , & Parker, G. (2009). Pain during depression and relationship to rejection sensitivity. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 119(5), 375382. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01316.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fisher, S. (1994). Identifying video game addiction in children and adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 19(5), 545553. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(94)90010-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaines, S. O. Jr. , Reis, H. T. , Summers, S. , Rusbult, C. E. , Cox, C. L. , Wexler, M. O. , Marelich, W. D. , & Kudand, G. J. (1997). Impact of attachment style on reactions to accommodative dilemmas in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 4(2), 93113. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1997.tb00133.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. , Swing, E. , Lim, C. , & Khoo, A. (2012). Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: Evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(1), 6270. doi:10.1037/a0026969

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grant, J. E. , Potenza, M. N. , Weinstein, A. , & Gorelick, D. A. (2010). Introduction to behavioral addictions. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 233241. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491884

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Green, L. , & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5), 769792. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2008). Videogame addiction: Further thoughts and observations. International Journal of Mental Health Addiction, 6(2), 182185. doi:10.1007/s11469-007-9128-y

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. , & Hunt, N. (1998). Dependence on computer games by adolescents. Psychological Reports, 82(2), 475480. doi:10.2466/pr0.1998.82.2.475

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hammen, C. L. , Burge, D. , Daley, S. E. , & Davila, J. (1995). Interpersonal attachment cognitions and prediction of symptomatic responses to interpersonal stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(3), 436443. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.104.3.436

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hellman, M. , Schoenmakers, T. M. , Nordstrom, B. R. , & van Holst, R. J. (2013). Is there such a thing as online video game addiction? A cross-disciplinary review. Addiction Research and Theory, 21(2), 102112. doi:10.3109/16066359.2012.693222

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hunt, M. (2005). Construct validity of the balloon analog risk task (BART): Associations with psychopathy and impulsivity. Assessment, 12(4), 416428. doi:10.1177/1073191105278740

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Iacono, W. G. , Malone, S. M. , & McGue, M. (2008). Behavioral disinhibition and the development of early-onset addiction: Common and specific influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 325348. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Irvine, M. A. , Worbe, Y. , Bolton, S. , Harrison, N. A. , Bullmore, E. T. , & Voon, V. (2013). Impaired decisional impulsivity in pathological videogamers. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e75914. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075914

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Király, O. , Nagygyörgy, K. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2014). Problematic online gaming. In K. P. Rosenberg & L. C. Feder (Eds.), Behavioral addictions: Criteria, evidence and treatment (pp. 6197). Burlington, USA: Elsevier Science.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kirby, K. N. , & Finch, J. C. (2010). The hierarchical structure of self-reported impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(6), 704713. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.019

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kirby, K. N. , & Herrnstein, R. J. (1995). Preference reversals due to myopic discounting of delayed reward. Psychological Science, 6, 8389. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00311.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Hsiao, S. , Liu, G. C. , Yen, J. U. , Yang, M. J. , & Yen, C. F. (2010). The characteristics of decision making, potential to take risks, and personality of college students with Internet addiction. Psychiatry Research, 175(1–2), 121125. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.004

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kobak, R. R. , & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59(1), 135146. doi:10.2307/1130395

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. (2013). Internet gaming addiction: Current perspectives. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 6, 125137. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S39476

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lejuez, C. W. , Aklin, W. M. , Jones, H. A. , Richards, J. R. , Strong, D. R. , Kahler, C. W. , & Read, J. P. (2003). The Balloon Analogue Risk Task differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11(1), 2633. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.11.1.26

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lejuez, C. W. , Read, J. P. , Kahler, C. W. , Richards, J. B. , Ramsey, S. E. , Stuart, G. L. , Strong, D. R. , & Brown, R. A. , (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 7584. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Littel, M. , Berg, I. , Luijten, M. , Rooij, A. J. , Keemink, L. , & Franken, I. H. (2012). Error processing and response inhibition in excessive computer game players: An event-related potential study. Addiction Biology, 17(5), 934947. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00467.x

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loewenstein, G. (1992). The fall and rise of psychological explanations in the economics of intertemporal choice. In G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time (pp. 334). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Logue, A. W. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(4), 665679. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00053978

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Madden, G. J. , Francisco, M. T. , Brewer, A. T. , & Stein, J. S. (2011). Delay discounting and gambling. Behavioural Processes, 87(1), 4349. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2011.01.012

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Madden, G. J. , Petry, N. M. , Badger, G. J. , & Bickel, W. K. (1997). Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: Drug and monetary rewards. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(3), 256262. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.256

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: The effects of delay and intervening events on reinforcement value (Vol. 5, pp. 5573). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and individual differences in functional versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 513524. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.513

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mischel, W. , & Ebbeson, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329337. doi:10.1037/h0029815

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mitchell, S. H. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 455464. doi:10.1007/PL00005491

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morahan-Martin, J. , & Schumacher, P. (2000). Incidence and correlates of pathological Internet use among college students. Computers in Human Behavior, 16(1), 1329. doi:10.1016/S0747-5632(99)00049-7

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Myerson, J. , & Green, L. (1995). Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64(3), 263276. doi:10.1901/jeab.1995.64-263

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ng, T. H. , & Johnson, S. L. (2013). Rejection sensitivity is associated with quality of life, psychosocial outcome, and the course of depression in euthymic patients with bipolar I disorder. Cognitive Therapy Research, 37(6), 11691178. doi:10.1007/s10608-013-9552-1

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pápay, O. , Urbán, R. , Griffiths, M. , Nagygyörgy, K. , Farkas, J. , Kökönyei, G. , Felvinczi, K. , Oláh, A. , Elekes, Z. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2013). Psychometric properties of the problematic online gaming questionnaire short-form and prevalence of problematic online gaming in a national sample of adolescents. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(5), 340348. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0484

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Perry, J. L. , & Carrol, M. E. (2008) The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology, 200, 126. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Posternak, M. A. , & Zimmerman, M. (2001). Symptoms of atypical depression. Psychiatry Research, 104(2), 175181. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00301-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rachlin, H. (2000). The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Rachlin, H. , Raineri, A. , & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233244. doi:10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , Ortengren, A. , Richards, J. B. , & de Wit, H. (2006). Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(2), 305315. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.03.024

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , Penfold, R. B. , & Patak, M. (2008). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in adolescents: Laboratory behavioral assessments. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(2), 124131. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.16.2.124

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reynolds, B. , & Schiffbauer, R. (2004). Measuring state changes in human delay discounting: An experiential discounting task. Behavioural Processes, 67(3), 343356. doi:10.1016/S0376-6357(04)00140-8

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Richards, J. B. , Zhang, L. , Mitchell, S. , & de Wit, H. (1999). Delay and probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71(2), 121143. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Robbins, T. W. , Gillan, C. M. , Smith, D. G. , de Wit, S. , & Ersche, K. D. (2012) Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: Towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16(1), 8191. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ronay, R. , & Kim, D. (2006). Gender differences in explicit and implicit risk attitudes: A socially facilitated phenomenon. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(2), 397419. doi:10.1348/014466605X66420

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Samuelson, P. A. (1937). A note on measurement of utility. The Review of Economic Studies, 4(2), 155161. doi:10.2307/2967612

  • Saville, B. K. , Gisbert, A. , Kopp, J. , & Telesco, C. (2011). Internet addiction and delay discounting in college students. The Psychological Record, 60(2), 5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Scherer, K. (1997). College life on-line: Healthy and unhealthy Internet use. Journal of College Student Development, 38, 655665.

  • Shaver, P. , & Hazan, C. (1987). Being lonely, falling in love: Perspectives from attachment theory. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 105124.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simpson, J. A. , Ickes, W. , & Grich, J. (1999). When accuracy hurts: Reactions of anxious-ambivalent dating partners to a relationship threatening situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 754769. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.754

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simpson, J. A. , Rholes, W. S. , & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An attachment perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(5), 899914. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.5.899

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: The Free Press.

  • Sun, D. L. , Chen, Z. J. , Ma, N. , Zhang, X. C. , Fu, X. M. , & Zhang, D. R. (2009). Decision-making and prepotent response inhibition functions in excessive Internet users. CNS Spectrum, 14(2), 7581. doi:10.1017/S1092852900000225

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vuchinich, R. E. , & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 292305. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.6.3.292

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Aboujaoude, E. (2015). Problematic Internet use: An overview. In E. Aboujaoude & V. Starcevic (Eds.), Mental health in the digital age: Grave dangers, great promise (pp. 326). Oxford, USA: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Dorani, D. , Elhadif, R. , Bukovza, Y. , Yarmulnik, A. , & Dannon, P. (2015). Internet addiction is associated with social anxiety in young adults. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 27(1), 49.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Feder, K , Rosenberg, K. , & Dannon, P. (2014). Internet addiction disorder: Overview and controversies. In K. P. Rosenberg & L. C. Feder (Eds.), Behavioral addictions: Criteria, evidence and treatment (pp. 99117). Burlington, USA: Elsevier Science.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Lejoyeux, M. (2010). Internet addiction or excessive Internet use. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 277283. doi:10.3109/00952990.2010.491880

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , Yaacov, Y. , Manning, M. , Danon, P. , & Weizman, A. (2015). Internet addiction and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) among school children. Israeli Medical Association Journal, 17(12), 731734.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Xu, S. , Korczykowski, M. , Zhu, S. , & Rao, H. (2013). Assessment of risk-taking and impulsive behaviors: A comparison between three tasks. Social Behavior and Personality, 41(3), 477486. doi:10.2224/sbp.2013.41.3.477

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 1(3), 237244. doi:10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (2004). Internet addiction: A new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(4), 402415. doi:10.1177/0002764204270278

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2024 0 259 50
Nov 2024 0 139 42
Dec 2024 0 149 34
Jan 2025 0 165 29
Feb 2025 0 211 45
Mar 2025 0 117 42
Apr 2025 0 0 0