Abstract
Background and aims
In the light of growing traditional and novel forms of gambling, the treatment of disordered gambling is gaining increasing importance and practical relevance. Most studies have examined face-to-face treatments. Although trials implementing self-guided treatments have recently been conducted, these options have not yet been systematically examined. The primary objective of this meta-analysis, therefore, was to analyze the efficacy of all types of psychological face-to-face and self-guided treatments.
Methods
A multilevel literature search yielded 27 randomized controlled studies totaling 3,879 participants to provide a comprehensive comparative evaluation of the short- and long-term efficacies of face-to-face and self-guided treatments for disordered gambling.
Results
As expected, the results revealed significantly higher effect sizes for face-to-face treatments (16 studies with Hedges’s g ranging from 0.67 to 1.15) as compared with self-guided treatments (11 studies with Hedges’s g ranging from 0.12 to 0.30) regarding the reduction of problematic gambling behavior. The intensity of treatment moderated the therapy effect, particularly for self-guided treatments.
Discussion and Conclusions
The results of this meta-analysis favor face-to-face treatments over self-guided treatments for the reduction of disordered gambling. Although the findings broaden the scope of knowledge about psychological treatment modalities for disordered gambling, further research is needed to identify the reasons for these differences with the goal to optimize the treatment for this disabling condition.
BACKGROUND AND AIMS
With the publication of the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013), pathological gambling, which is defined as “a maladaptive pattern of wagering that persists despite negative consequences in major areas of functioning such as finances, relationships, and psychological health” (Rash & Petry, 2014, p. 285), was categorized as “gambling disorder” under the new section “Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders.” Consistent with this terminology, we will use the term “gambling disorder” for this condition.
To reduce the high illness burden, various psychological treatment approaches – traditionally delivered through direct face-to-face contacts between patients and therapists – have been investigated over the years. Psychological approaches range from more intensive treatment options that include the most commonly used cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) (for an overview, see Raylu, Loo, & Oei, 2013; Stea & Hodgins, 2011), the 12-step concept of Gamblers Anonymous (GA) groups, based on the principles of Alcoholics Anonymous (e.g., Petry, 2005), couples therapy (e.g., Lee & Awosoga, 2014), integrative interventions including a variety of treatment elements and settings (e.g., Blackman, Simone, & Thoms, 1989; Lesieur & Blume, 1991) to brief motivational strategies (e.g., Petry, Weinstock, Ledgerwood, & Morasco, 2008; Petry, Weinstock, Morasco, & Ledgerwood, 2009).
Because many gamblers do not seek professional help (Slutske, 2006; Suurvali, Hodgins, & Cunningham, 2010), self-guided treatments (SGTs) have been developed to expand accessibility and reduce barriers to treatment, such as stigmatization and cost (Rash & Petry, 2014; Raylu, Oei, & Loo, 2008; Stea & Hodgins, 2011). These treatment options offer patients the opportunity to utilize psychological interventions in an autonomous manner and are commonly delivered through written materials or self-help (SH) workbooks (WBs), by telephone, the Internet, or other media sources (e.g., Raylu et al., 2008). Since the 1990s, a number of trials have been published (for an overview, see Gainsbury & Blaszczynski, 2011; Raylu et al., 2008) using either a single session of motivational interviewing (MI) delivered over the telephone in combination with a mailed CBT-based WB (e.g., Hodgins, Currie, Currie, & Fick, 2009; Hodgins, Currie, & el-Guebaly, 2001), CBT-based WBs or a toolkit only (e.g., Hodgins et al., 2001, 2009; LaBrie et al., 2012), or personalized feedback (PFB) techniques (e.g., Cunningham, Hodgins, Toneatto, & Murphy, 2012; Cunningham, Hodgins, Toneatto, Rai, & Cordingley, 2009) yielding mixed results. Only recently, more intensive and structured Internet-based treatment programs have been producing promising findings. One of these programs (Carlbring & Smit, 2008) examined a successful 8-week Internet-based treatment with MI and CBT elements including homework assignments, regular therapist contacts through e-mail and telephone, and participation in an online discussion group. The impact of the therapist contact on treatment efficacy in SGTs is discussed in the literature with varying results (e.g., Apodaca & Miller, 2003; Barak, Hen, Boniel-Nissim, & Shapira, 2008; Haug, Nordgreen, Öst, & Havik, 2012; Riper et al., 2014; Spek et al., 2007). However, this issue has not yet been examined in the domain of disordered gambling.
Although earlier meta-analyses targeting psychological treatments for disordered gambling (Cowlishaw et al., 2012; Gooding & Tarrier, 2009; Leibetseder, Laireiter, Vierhauser, & Hittenberger, 2011; Pallesen, Mitsem, Kvale, Johnsen, & Molde, 2005) vary in the types of the included psychological interventions, study designs, outcome variables, and moderator variables, they provide support for the efficacy of psychological interventions for gambling when delivered through a face-to-face treatment (FTFT) format. Three of these meta-analyses (Gooding & Tarrier, 2009; Leibetseder et al., 2011; Pallesen et al., 2005) included SGTs, but the number of trials was small, and further research has since been conducted. The latest meta-analysis in gambling disorder (Cowlishaw et al., 2012), however, was exclusively limited to psychological interventions delivered through systematic and face-to-face contact. Therapies provided through telephone or the Internet, SH formats, and minimal FTFTs were not included. Moreover, moderator and sensitivity analyses were not conducted due to the small number of studies. Consequently, a comprehensive investigation of psychological FTFTs is still pending, and SGTs of disordered gambling have not yet been systematically investigated.
The primary objective of this meta-analysis, therefore, was to investigate the efficacy of all types of psychological FTFTs and SGTs based on randomized controlled trials (RCTs) for reducing (a) the global severity of disordered gambling, (b) frequency of gambling, and (c) financial loss from gambling at 0–3 months after treatment (short-term effects) and at the latest follow-up (long-term effects). We expected FTFTs to be more effective than SGTs due to the additional benefits discussed in former treatments, such as therapeutic alliance, empathy, and creating confidence (e.g., Feinstein, Heiman, & Yager, 2015; Wampold, 2015). In addition, our goal was to identify variables moderating the effect sizes of FTFTs and SGTs. We followed the recommendations of the PRISMA Statement (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009).
Methods
Eligibility criteria
Studies were considered for inclusion if they (a) employed any kind of psychological treatment (without restrictions around mode of delivery, setting, or duration of treatment); (b) used a randomized or quasi-randomized controlled study design, such as wait-list (WL) controls, participants not receiving treatment, assessment only, or a placebo intervention (e.g., attention control feedback excluding any kind of intervention-specific ingredients); (c) measured at least one of the three outcome variables; (d) reported sufficient statistical data for effect size calculations; and (e) treated adult participants (18 years or older) with the diagnosis of gambling disorder. Studies were excluded if (a) the study was a case study; (b) the study sample completely overlapped with the sample of another study included in the meta-analysis; and (c) no abstract or full text of the study was available.
Information sources and literature search
We conducted a multilevel literature search using databases, such as PsycINFO (1860), Medline (1902), PubMed (1966), Psyndex (1875), the Cochrane Central Register of Clinical Trials (1988), ProQuest Digital Dissertations (1964), and the web search engine Google Scholar (1969). The search covered all relevant publications from the first available year until March 31, 2017. The search term included the disorder-related key words pathological gambling OR gambl* OR ludomania combined with the intervention-related key words treatment OR intervention OR therapy OR psychotherapy, adding the RCT filter recommended by the Cochrane collaboration (Lefebvre, Manheimer, & Glanville, 2011). Subsequently, we conducted a thorough examination of the reference lists of review articles and original studies retrieved from the databases. In addition, the authors of relevant articles were contacted to ask for unpublished papers suitable for inclusion in the meta-analysis.
Outcome measures
Following the recommendations of the Banff, Alberta Consensus (Walker et al., 2006), we specified three outcome variables to measure the reduction of disordered gambling: (a) the global severity of gambling pathology, quantified by the use of valid and reliable instruments, or the number of DSM criteria; (b) frequency of gambling (e.g., number of days or hours gambled in the last week or month), and (c) financial loss due to gambling (e.g., net expenditure last week or last month), both (b) and (c) quantified using a timeline follow-up interview (Sobell & Sobell, 1992), diary cards, or other self-reporting forms.
Study selection
Study selection was performed by two independent reviewers (the first and the second authors, MG and ML, respectively), and supervised by the last author of this paper (A-RL). Disagreements between the authors were resolved through discussion.
Data collection process and data items
We generated a structured data extraction form that we refined and modified after pilot testing a sample of 10 studies. To calculate controlled effect sizes, data from the treatment group and the respective control group of each study were collected. In addition, we extracted numerical and categorical data from each study to perform moderator analyses. The data extraction was performed by the first author (MG) and validated by the second author (ML). Disagreements were resolved through discussion.
Risk of bias in individual studies
We assessed the internal validity of each study using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies, developed by the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) (Thomas, Ciliska, Dobbins, & Micucci, 2004). This tool has demonstrated content and construct validity (Thomas et al., 2004), and is recommended for use in systematic reviews and meta-analyses (Deeks et al., 2003).
Each study was rated in a standardized manner on six domains: selection bias, study design, identification and control of confounders, blinding, reliability and validity of data collection tools, and reporting and percentage of withdrawals and dropouts. Each domain was evaluated as strong, moderate, or weak. The global rating was composed after the evaluation of the six domains. The first two authors (MG and ML) independently assessed each study and determined the global score of each trial. Interrater reliability was quantified using the kappa statistic. Disagreements between the authors were resolved through discussion.
Moreover, to obtain numerical values for the quality score, which is not directly provided by the EPHPP procedure, we assigned each of the six domains differentiated scores. The domains rated as weak were assigned a value of 0, those rated as moderate were assigned a value of 1, and those rated as strong were assigned a value of 2. Thus, each study could receive a maximum score of 12 points when the values of the six domains were summed. This enabled us to calculate meta-regression analyses based on continuous data, the transformed EPHPP scores.
Summary measures and synthesis of results
Statistical analysis was performed using the software program Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) version 2.2.064 (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2005). We calculated the effect sizes for the reduction of global severity of gambling, frequency, and financial loss separately for FTFTs and SGTs. The effect size calculations were based on continuous data indicating the difference between the treatment and control groups at posttreatment and the last follow-up assessment. If means and standard deviations (SDs) were not available, the effect sizes were calculated based on equivalent estimation procedures (e.g., t values or exact probability levels). Due to small sample sizes, the effect sizes were corrected for bias using Hedges’s g with the corresponding 95% confidence interval (CI) (Hedges & Olkin, 1984). For studies comparing more than one treatment group with a single control group, separate comparisons were entered into the meta-analysis with the number of participants in the control group equally divided between the comparisons. This procedure ensures that participants in the control group are not counted more than once, and offers the possibility to investigate differences between distinct treatments through moderator analyses (Higgins, Deeks, & Altman, 2011). If an outcome variable was measured by more than one instrument, data for these instruments were entered separately and pooled together for the particular outcome variable (Lipsey & Wilson, 2000). For studies reporting data based on both completers and intention-to-treat (ITT) analyses, the ITT data were considered. The direction of the effect was adjusted according to the “success”: the effect size was positive if the treated group performed superior to the control group. According to Cohen’s recommendations (1977), effect sizes of 0.20–0.30 can be classified as small, those near 0.50 as medium, and those above 0.80 as large.
Assuming heterogeneity among the studies, we decided to use the random effects model for the integration of effect sizes. Heterogeneity of the effect sizes was investigated using the Q statistic with the corresponding p value, and the I2 statistic, indicating to what extent real differences in effect sizes was reflected by the proportion of the variance (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009; Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). I2 values of 25%, 50%, and 75% were classified as low, moderate, and high, respectively (Higgins et al., 2003). Detailed information concerning the calculation and the integration of effect sizes can be found in Borenstein et al. (2005, 2009) and Lipsey and Wilson (2000). The studies most frequently reported untransformed data, which we used for the effect size calculations. We therefore followed the recommendation to avoid the mixture of transformed and untransformed data in a single meta-analysis (Deeks, Higgins, & Altman, 2011).
Risk of bias across studies
To control for publication bias, we conducted a thorough literature search and computed Rosenthal’s fail-safe N (FS N; Rosenthal, 1979). According to Rosenthal (1991), effect sizes are considered robust, if the number of studies required to obtain a non-significant overall effect is greater than 5k + 10, where k represents the number of studies. In addition, we used the trim-and-fill method (Duval & Tweedie, 2000) to estimate missing studies and their impact on the ascertained effect sizes. This method is based on the logic of the funnel plot and assumes a symmetrical distribution of the effect sizes for outcome variables in the absence of publication bias. In the case of asymmetrical distribution, the trim-and-fill method adjusts and corrects the effect sizes (Borenstein et al., 2009); we only applied this method, if 10 studies were available for the analysis (Sterne, Egger, & Moher, 2011). Funnel plot asymmetry was assessed using Begg’s rank correlation test (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994). As singular extreme effect size values produce misleading interpretations of treatment effects (Lipsey & Wilson, 2000), we used the “one-study-removed” method offered by CMA to examine the impact of each study’s effect size on the overall effect (Borenstein et al., 2005). If the recalculated results did not substantially impact the effect size and remained within the 95% CI, the studies were retained in the analyses.
Moderator analyses
To explain the heterogeneity among the effect sizes, we determined the following categorical moderators for both FTFTs and SGTs: gambling type (electronic vs. other types of gambling), data analysis (ITT vs. completers), and type of psychological intervention. The type of psychological intervention was analyzed by dividing psychological strategies into the following subcategories: (a) CBT, covering cognitive and/or behavioral treatments. Studies predominantly applying CBT strategies (i.e., in at least about 70% of the treatment sessions) were assigned to this category. Since a limited number of studies using an equal number of CBT sessions and sessions including other psychological strategies (e.g., MI) were expected to be found in literature search, we decided to subsume this combination under the CBT category; (b) MI, which emphasizes the motivation to change gambling behavior. Studies predominantly implementing MI strategies (e.g., delivery of a single session of MI alone or in combination with a concomitant, self-administered CBT WB) were included in this category, as well as strategies associated with MI (e.g., PFB); and (c) psychological therapies that pertain to other treatment categories [e.g., congruence couple therapy (CCT) and 12-step facilitated (TSF) group therapy]. Considering the relevant publications (e.g., Gooding & Tarrier, 2009; Toneatto & Millar, 2004), we investigated whether CBT and combined CBT yielded higher treatment effects compared with other psychological treatments. With regard to FTFTs, we further investigated the mode of therapy (individual vs. group setting). Regarding SGTs, we considered the moderator variable of intensity of treatment, divided into the following categories: (a) high-intensity treatments (structured therapy program with ≥6 sessions/modules delivered over the Internet and over a period of several weeks, including homework assignments, participation in an online discussion group, or interactive exercises); (b) low-intensity treatments (<6 sessions/modules, e.g., a single session of MI delivered over the telephone); and (c) SH formats (e.g., a mailed CBT-based WB, a toolkit, or PFB). These formats are based on the individual, self-reliant acquisition of knowledge about problematic gambling behavior. Although PFB materials were typically processed within brief, single sessions, these treatment options cannot be classified according to structured modules/sessions. Moreover, the impact of therapist contact was examined by stratifying studies into two groups, such as those with therapist contact during treatment and those without therapist contact. Moderator analyses for categorical variables were conducted using the mixed effects model with pooled estimates of T2 and the Q-test based on the analysis of variance with the corresponding p value for the interpretation of the differences between subgroups (Borenstein et al., 2009).
In case of at least 10 available studies (Deeks et al., 2011), we further conducted meta-regression analyses using the following continuous variables: year of publication, duration of treatment (assessed by the total number of hours spent in FTFTs), and study quality (assessed by the transformed scores of the EPHPP tool). Meta-regression analyses on the mean age and the percentage of male/female participants, however, were not performed, because age and sex across studies differ from those within studies (Thompson & Higgins, 2002). To test the stability of the effect sizes at follow-up, we performed meta-regression analyses using the length of the period between posttreatment and follow-up (measured in weeks) as moderator.
Results
Study selection
A total of 27 studies were identified for inclusion in this meta-analysis. Among these, 16 implemented FTFTs containing 27 comparisons, and 11 studies used SGTs containing 22 comparisons. One publication contained two studies with independent samples (Melville, Davis, Matzenbacher, & Clayborne, 2004). However, only the first of these provided sufficient data for effect size calculation and was included in the analysis. Dowling et al. published three treatment outcome manuscripts based on one eligible sample randomly assigned to treatment and WL control conditions (Dowling, Smith, & Thomas, 2006, 2007, 2009). To avoid overlap, only the trial comparing individual and group CBT (Dowling et al., 2007) was included in our analysis. We further excluded three studies assessed in a previous meta-analysis (Cowlishaw et al., 2012), because participants in the control groups received either a CBT-based SH WB (Diskin & Hodgins, 2009) or participated in GA groups (Grant et al., 2009; Petry et al., 2006), and thus the types of control group did not satisfy the defined selection criteria. One unpublished study was not considered for our analysis, because participants in the control group received advice for dealing with problematic gambling behavior (Abbott et al., 2012). Two further studies cited by a recent review (Marchica & Derevensky, 2016) were excluded, because participants did not receive a formal diagnosis (Celio & Lisman, 2014), or there were insufficient data for effect size calculation (Takushi et al., 2004). The flow diagram of the study selection is presented in Figure 1.

Flow diagram of the study selection process
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034

Flow diagram of the study selection process
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034
Flow diagram of the study selection process
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034
Study characteristics
The present sample of studies varied in type of control condition: The majority implemented WL control groups (k = 18), several studies used only assessment or only attention (k = 8), and one study used no treatment. The results were reported based on completers (k = 14) and ITT analyses (k = 12). One study failed to indicate the type of data analysis (Melville et al., 2004). Five studies provided only follow-up data, collected within a period of 3 months after treatment (Cunningham et al., 2009, 2012; LaBrie et al., 2012; Martens, Arterberry, Takamatsu, Masters, & Dude, 2015; Neighbors et al., 2015). Since mean intervals between pre- and posttreatment of the remaining studies were comparable (k = 19; M = 8.13 weeks, SD = 2.55), these studies were included in the analyses of posttreatment data. Follow-up data for treatment and control groups were provided by 11 studies with periods ranging from 1.5 to 9 months (M = 4.86, SD = 3.03).
Among FTFTs, most studies implemented CBT and combined CBT strategies delivered through individual and group settings in equal shares. The total number of hours spent in FTFTs ranged from 10 min to 24 hr (M = 12.23 hr, SD = 9.01). SGTs typically implemented SH formats (CBT-based WBs and PFB) delivered over the Internet, and without therapist support.
A total of 3,879 participants across all studies were analyzed (n = 912 in FTFTs and n = 2,967 in SGTs). Of those, 2,655 patients were assigned to treatment conditions (n = 587 in FTFTs and n = 2,068 in SGTs), and 1,224 individuals to control groups (n = 325 in FTFTs and n = 899 in SGTs). All participants received a formal diagnosis. Most studies examined electronic gambling as the predominant type of gambling. The total sample was predominantly male (60.87%) with an average age of 39 years. Detailed information regarding the characteristics of studies is presented in Table 1.
Characteristics of studies
Reference | N a | Treatment (N) | Control (N) | Duration (hours)b | Modality/setting/intensity/therapist contact | FU (months)c | Outcomes (assessment) | Gambling type | ITT/CO | EPHPPd |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Carlbring and Smit (2008) | 66 | CBT + MI (34) | WL (32) | NA | SGT/HI | NA | GS (NODS) | E | ITT | 7 |
4 modules MI + 4 modules CBT delivered over the Internet | Contact | |||||||||
Casey et al. (2017) | 174 | 1. CBT (60) | AOC (55) | 1. 6 | 1. SGT/HI | 12e | GS (G-SAS; SOGS) | E | ITT | 6 |
2. MFS (59) | No contact | FR (gambling activities/week) | ||||||||
Each intervention included 6 modules delivered over the Internet | 2. 1 | 2. SGT/HI | FL (money gambled/week) | |||||||
No contact | ||||||||||
Cunningham et al. (2009) | 49 | MI/PFB (24) | WL (25) | NA | SGT/SH | NA | GS (CPGI) | NA | CO | 8 |
Delivered over the Internet | No contact | FL (money spent past 3 months) | ||||||||
Cunningham et al. (2012) | 209 | 1. MI/PNFB (70) | WL (69) | 1. NA | 1. SGT/SH | 9 | FR (days gambled/month) | NA | ITT | 8 |
2. MI/PFB (70) | No contact | |||||||||
Both interventions delivered over the Internet | 2. NA | 2. SGT/SH | FL (dollars lost/month) | |||||||
No contact | ||||||||||
Doiron and Nicki (2007) | 40 | CBT (20) | WL (20) | NA | FTFT/G | NA | GS (CPGI) | E | CO | 7 |
FR (time spent/month) | ||||||||||
FL (money spent/month) | ||||||||||
Dowling et al. (2007) | 56 | 1. CBT (14) | WL (25) | 1. 18 | 1. FTFT/I | NA | FR (days gambled/week) | E | ITT | 9 |
2. CBT (17) | 2. 24 | 2. FTFT/G | FL (money spent/week) | |||||||
Echeburúa, Báez, and Fernández-Montalvo (1996)f | 51 | 1. CBT (14) | WL (12) | 1. 6.5 | 1. FTFT/I | 6 | FR (gambling activities/week; hours/week) | E | CO | 8 |
2. CBT (13) | 2. 12.5 | 2. FTFT/G | FL (money spent/week) | |||||||
3. CBT (12) | 3. 6 | 3. FTFT/I + G | ||||||||
Hodgins et al. (2001) | 98 | 1. MI (single session) delivered over telephone + CBT WB (31) | WL (34) | 1. NA | 1. SGT/LI | NA | FR (days gambled last 2 months) | E | CO | 8 |
Contact | ||||||||||
2. CBT WB (33) | 2. NA | 2. SGT/SH | FL (money spent last 2 months) | |||||||
No contact | ||||||||||
Hodgins et al. (2009) | 314 | 1. MI (single session) delivered over telephone + CBT WB (83) | WL (65) | 1. NA | 1. SGT/LI | NA | FR (days gambled/month) | E | ITT | 8 |
Contact | ||||||||||
2. MI + 6 booster sessions delivered over telephone + CBT WB (84) | 2. NA | 2. SGT/LI | ||||||||
Contact | FL (dollars lost/month) | |||||||||
3. CBT WB (82) | 3. NA | 3. SGT/SH | ||||||||
No contact | ||||||||||
Hopper (2008) | 60 | MI/PNFB (30) | AOC (30) | NA | SGT/SH | NA | FR (gambling activities past 6 months) | NA | CO | 6 |
Delivered over the Internet | No contact | FL (money spent/month) | ||||||||
LaBrie et al. (2012) | 315 | 1. Toolkit, NV (59) | WL (102) | NA | 1. SGT/SH | 2 | FR (days gambled/month) | NA | ITT | 9 |
2. Toolkit, MA (49) | No contact | |||||||||
3. Guided toolkit (+ minimal therapist contact), NV (55) | 2. SGT/SH | |||||||||
4. Guided toolkit (+ minimal therapist contact), MA (50) | No contact | |||||||||
All interventions delivered over the Internet | 3. SGT/SH | |||||||||
Contact | ||||||||||
4. SGT/SH | ||||||||||
Contact | ||||||||||
Ladouceur et al. (2001) | 64 | CBT (35) | WL (29) | 11 | FTFT/I | NA | GS (DSM-IV) | E | CO | 6 |
FR (gambling sessions; hours gambled/week) | ||||||||||
FL (money spent/week) | ||||||||||
Ladouceur et al. (2003) | 59 | CBT (34) | WL (25) | 20 | FTFT/G | NA | GS (DSM-IV) | NA | CO | 6 |
FR (gambling sessions; hours gambled/week) | ||||||||||
FL (money spent/week) | ||||||||||
Larimer et al. (2012)f | 111 | 1. PFI (40) | AOC (41) | 1. 1.5 | 1. FTFT/I | 6 | GS (DSM-IV criteria) | NA | CO | 8 |
2. CBT (30) | 2. 6 | 2. FTFT/G | FR (gambling activities/month) | |||||||
FL (money spent gambling/month) | ||||||||||
Lee and Awosoga (2014) | 16 | CCT (8) | NT (8) | 12 | FTFT/G | 2 | GS (G-SAS) | L | CO | 9 |
Luquiens and Tanguy (2016) | 1,122 | 1. MI/PNFB (293) | WL (264) | NA | 1. SGT/SH | 1.5 | GS (PGSI)g | IP | CO/ITT | 5 |
2. CBT WB (264) | No contact | |||||||||
3. Guided CBT WB (six weekly modules) (301)h | 2. SGT/SH | FR (gambling sessions/month) | ||||||||
All interventions delivered over the Internet | No contact | |||||||||
3. SGT/SH | FL (total loss/month) | |||||||||
Contact | ||||||||||
Marceaux and Melville (2011) | 33 | 1. CBT (15) | WL (7) | 1. 24 | 1. FTFT/G | NA | GS (DSM-IV) | NA | CO | 10 |
2. TSF (11) | 2. 24 | 2. FTFT/G | FR (gambling episodes)i | |||||||
FL (money spent)i | ||||||||||
Martens et al. (2015) | 333 | 1. MI/PFB (111) | AOC (109) | NA | 1. SGT/SH | NA | GS (CPGI) | NA | ITT | 7 |
2. EDU (113) | No contact | FR (days gambled last 60 days) | ||||||||
Delivered over paper printout | 2. SGT/SH | FL (money spent last 60 days) | ||||||||
No contact | ||||||||||
Melville et al. (2004), study 1j | 13 | 1. CBT mapping (4) | WL (5) | 1. 24 | 1. FTFT/G | NA | GS (DSM-IV) | E | NA | 8 |
2. CBT non-mapping (4) | 2. 24 | 2. FTFT/G | ||||||||
Myrseth, Litlere, Stoylen, and Pallesen (2009) | 14 | CBT + MI elements (7) | WL (7) | 12 | FTFT/G | 3 | GS (DSM-IV) | E | ITT | 8 |
FL (money spent/week) | ||||||||||
Neighbors et al. (2015) | 227 | MI/PNFB (113) | ACO (114) | NA | SGT/SH | 3 | GS (GPI) | C | CO | 7 |
Delivered over computer | No contact | FR (days gambled past year) | ||||||||
FL (money spent/month) | ||||||||||
Oei et al. (2010) | 102 | 1. CBT + MI elements (37) | WL (28) | 1. 12 | 1. FTFT/I | NA | FR (gambling episodes past 2 weeks) | E | ITT | 8 |
2. CBT + MI elements (37) | 2. 15 | 2. FTFT/G | FL (money gambled/day) | |||||||
Petry et al. (2008) | 172 | 1. MI/BA (35) | AOC (47) | 1. 0.2 | 1. FTFT/I | 9 | GS (ASI-G) | SB/L | ITT | 9 |
2. MI/MET (single session) (52) | 2. 1 | 2. FTFT/I | FL (money gambled/week)k | |||||||
3. MI/MET (single session) + CBT (3 sessions) (38) | 3. 4 | 3. FTFT/I | ||||||||
Petry et al. (2009) | 114 | 1. MI/BA (32) | AOC (34) | 1. 0.2 | 1. FTFT/I | 9 | GS (ASI-G) | C/P | ITT | 9 |
2. MI/MET (single session) (30) | 2. 1 | 2. FTFT/I | FR (days gambled/month) | |||||||
3. MI/MET (single session) + CBT (3 sessions) (18) | 3. 4 | 3. FTFT/I | FL (gambling episodes/month)l | |||||||
Sylvain, Ladouceur, and Boisvert (1997) | 29 | CBT (14) | WL (15) | 17 | FTFT/I | NA | GS (DSM-III-R; SOGS) | E | CO | 7 |
FR (gambling sessions; hours spent/week) | ||||||||||
FL (money spent/week) | ||||||||||
Toneatto, Pillai, and Courtice (2014) | 18 | CBT + mindfulness intervention (9) | WL (9) | 7.5 | FTFT/G | NA | GS (DSM-IV) | SB/L | CO | 7 |
Watson (2012) | 20 | MI + PFB (7) | AOC (13) | 2 | FTFT/I | 3 | FR (gambling activities/month) | BG | CO | 6 |
FL (money spent/month) |
Note. ACO: attention control only; AOC: assessment only control; ASI-G: addiction severity index-gambling; BA: brief advice; BG: betting on games; C: cards; CBT: cognitive-behavioral therapy; CCT: congruence couple therapy; CO: completers; CPGI: Canadian Problem Gambling Index; E: electronic gambling; EDU: education program; EPHPP: Effective Public Health Practice Project; FL: financial loss; FR: frequency; FTFT: face-to-face treatment; FU: effect sizes from posttreatment to latest follow-up; G: group setting; GPI: Gambling Problems Index; GS: global severity; G-SAS: Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale; HI: high intensity; I: individual counseling; IP: Internet poker; ITT: intention-to-treat; L: lottery; LI: low intensity; MA: Massachusetts; MET: motivational enhancement therapy; MFS: monitoring, feedback, and support; MI: motivational interviewing; NA: not available; NODS: National Opinion Research Center DSM Screen for Gambling Problems; NT: no treatment; NV: Nevada; PFB: personalized feedback; PFI: personalized feedback intervention; PNFB: personalized normative feedback; PGSI: Problem Gambling Severity Index; P: poker; SB: sports betting; SGT: self-guided treatment; SH: self-help; SOGS: South Oaks Gambling Screen; STAI: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; TSF: 12-step facilitated group therapy; WB: workbook; WL: wait-list.
Number of subjects included in the analysis.
Total number of hours spent in treatment.
Only studies reporting data at follow-up for both treatment and control groups are included.
The transformed EPHPP scores are presented.
Effect sizes at follow-up were not calculated due to insufficient data.
The study reported data for treatment and control groups at follow-up only.
The PGSI scores were based only on a subset of randomized participants. Consequently, the data for this outcome variable were classified as “completers.”
This treatment condition was categorized as pure “SH,” because no information about the intensity of treatment (e.g., homework assignments and communication between client and therapist) was available.
Data for calculating the effect sizes at follow-up for frequency and FL were not available.
This study reported only means for the treatment and control groups at posttreatment, but no SDs. The only available information for effect size calculation was the result from an independent groups t-test for the mapping group versus WL control group regarding the reduction of global severity. This statistic was used to calculate the SD for the combined CBT treatment groups versus WL control group (see also Cowlishaw et al., 2012). The formulae for the combination of subgroups and the calculation of SDs from t values are available in Higgins and Deeks (2011).
Effect sizes for FL were not included, because data were log transformed.
Effect sizes for FL were not included, because data were reported as medians and interquartile ranges.
Risk of bias within studies
The transformed global EPHPP scores ranged from 5 to 10 points [maximum score: 12 points; M = 7.58, SD = 1.25 (M = 7.81, SD = 1.22 for FTFTs; M = 7.18, SD = 1.17 for SGTs)]. Validity assessment yielded an interrater reliability of κ = .75. The transformed EPHPP scores for the global ratings are shown in Table 1.
Synthesis of results and risk of bias across studies
The pooled effect sizes for both types of treatment on all outcomes at 0–3 months posttreatment and follow-up, the 95% CI, and the significance tests are outlined in Table 2. Further results concerning the effect sizes for FTFTs and SGTs and their corresponding forest plots are presented in Figure 2.
Effect sizes for both modalities of treatment and outcomes during posttreatment and follow-up
Outcome | Effect | FTFTs | SGTs | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
k | g | 95% CI | z | p | I 2 | FS N | k | g | 95% CI | z | p | I 2 | FS N | ||
Global severity | Post | 16 | 1.15 | [0.63, 1.67] | 4.35 | <.001 | 85.97 | 418 | 10 | 0.30 | [−0.02, 0.63] | 1.83 | .068 | 82.94 | –a |
FU | 10 | 0.42 | [0.21, 0.63] | 3.90 | <.001 | 0.00 | 32 | 4 | 0.15 | [−0.06, 0.36] | 1.38 | .168 | 0.00 | –a | |
Frequency | Post | 14 | 0.74 | [0.48, 0.99] | 5.63 | <.001 | 44.37 | 207 | 20 | 0.12 | [0.02, 0.22] | 2.24 | <.05 | 28.95 | 23 |
FU | 9 | 0.49 | [0.25, 0.73] | 3.98 | <.001 | 0.00 | 34 | 9 | 0.08 | [−0.04, 0.20] | 1.37 | .171 | 13.81 | –a | |
FL | Post | 12 | 0.67 | [0.47, 0.87] | 6.49 | <.001 | 0.00 | 122 | 17 | 0.13 | [0.05, 0.22] | 3.01 | <.01 | 0.00 | 36 |
FU | 7 | 0.25 | [−0.03, 0.54] | 1.75 | .081 | 0.00 | –a | 5 | 0.07 | [−0.05, 0.18] | 1.09 | .275 | 0.00 | –a |
Note. CI: confidence interval; FS N: fail-safe N (number of studies required to obtain a non-significant treatment effect); FU: effect sizes from posttreatment to latest follow-up; g: Hedges’s g; I2: percentage of total variation across studies; k: number of treatment conditions.
FS N was not calculated, because p was not significant.

Overall effect sizes for each treatment modality and outcome at posttreatment. BA: brief advice; BS: booster sessions; CBT: cognitive-behavioral therapy; CCT: congruence couple therapy; CI: confidence interval; EDU: education program; G: group setting; I: individual counseling; map.: mapping group; Mass.: Massachusetts site; MFS: motivation, feedback, support; MI: motivational interviewing; Nev.: Nevada site; non-map.: non-mapping group; PFB: personalized feedback; PNFB: personalized normative feedback; RW: relative weight; TSF: 12-step facilitated therapy; WB: workbook
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034

Overall effect sizes for each treatment modality and outcome at posttreatment. BA: brief advice; BS: booster sessions; CBT: cognitive-behavioral therapy; CCT: congruence couple therapy; CI: confidence interval; EDU: education program; G: group setting; I: individual counseling; map.: mapping group; Mass.: Massachusetts site; MFS: motivation, feedback, support; MI: motivational interviewing; Nev.: Nevada site; non-map.: non-mapping group; PFB: personalized feedback; PNFB: personalized normative feedback; RW: relative weight; TSF: 12-step facilitated therapy; WB: workbook
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034
Overall effect sizes for each treatment modality and outcome at posttreatment. BA: brief advice; BS: booster sessions; CBT: cognitive-behavioral therapy; CCT: congruence couple therapy; CI: confidence interval; EDU: education program; G: group setting; I: individual counseling; map.: mapping group; Mass.: Massachusetts site; MFS: motivation, feedback, support; MI: motivational interviewing; Nev.: Nevada site; non-map.: non-mapping group; PFB: personalized feedback; PNFB: personalized normative feedback; RW: relative weight; TSF: 12-step facilitated therapy; WB: workbook
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.034


Effect sizes of FTFTs at posttreatment and follow-up
At posttreatment, all results were significant with a large effect size for the reduction of global severity and medium effect sizes for the remaining outcomes. At follow-up, the analyses revealed significant results with a small-to-medium effect size for the reduction of global severity and a medium effect size for the reduction of frequency. A small and non-significant effect size was registered for the reduction of financial loss. As depicted in Table 2, high and moderate heterogeneity across the studies was observed regarding the reduction of global severity and frequency. The effect size distribution for the reduction of financial loss was homogeneous.
The trim-and-fill method identified five studies causing funnel plot asymmetry for the reduction of global severity (Begg’s test, p = .005) and two studies for the reduction of frequency (Begg’s test, p = .035). If the asymmetry was due to publication bias, our analyses with these filled studies suggested a markedly reduced effect size for the reduction of global severity (g = 0.55, 95% CI [−0.03, 1.13]) indicating substantial impact of publication bias, and a slightly reduced effect size for the reduction of frequency (g = 0.66, 95% CI [0.39, 0.93]) indicating trivial impact of publication bias. No indication for publication bias was found for the reduction of financial loss (Begg’s test, p = .065). According to the FS Ns, the effect sizes were considered robust for all outcome variables at posttreatment. However, the effect sizes at follow-up were not robust.
Effect sizes of SGTs at posttreatment and follow-up
At posttreatment, the analyses revealed a non-significant and small effect size for the reduction of global severity, and significant and small effect sizes for the reduction of frequency and financial loss. At follow-up, all the results were non-significant with a small effect size for the reduction of global severity, and effect sizes approaching zero for the reduction of frequency and financial loss. As shown in Table 2, considerable heterogeneity across studies was observed for the reduction of global severity. For the remaining outcomes, the effect size distributions showed low heterogeneity or homogeneity.
The trim-and-fill method identified five studies causing funnel plot asymmetry regarding the reduction of frequency (Begg’s test, p = .043), and four studies regarding the reduction of financial loss (Begg’s test, p = .016). When considering these studies, the effect sizes changed only slightly for the reduction of frequency (g = 0.04, 95% CI [−0.08, 0.15]) and financial loss (g = 0.10, 95% CI [0.001, 0.19]). Furthermore, no indication for publication bias was found for the reduction of global severity (Begg’s test, p = .464). The FS Ns suggested that none of the effect sizes for the outcome variables were robust. Outlier identification through the one-study-removed procedure showed no impact of any single study on the overall effects.
Moderator analyses
The results of the moderator analyses for categorical and continuous variables at 0–3 months posttreatment are presented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively.
Moderator analyses for categorical variables
Variable | k | g | SE | 95% CI | z | p | Q between | p (Q) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Face-to-face treatments | ||||||||
Type of psychological intervention | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
CBT | 10 | 1.46 | 0.31 | [0.86, 2.06] | 4.78 | <.001 | 2.98 | .084 |
Other | 6 | 0.61 | 0.39 | [−0.16, 1.33] | 1.55 | .120 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
CBT | 10 | 0.78 | 0.15 | [0.48, 1.08] | 5.08 | <.001 | 0.28 | .598 |
Other | 4 | 0.61 | 0.27 | [−0.08, 1.15] | 2.24 | <.05 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
CBT | 10 | 0.65 | 0.11 | [0.44, 0.86] | 6.05 | <.001 | 0.54 | .464 |
Other | 2 | 0.94 | 0.38 | [0.19, 1.69] | 2.45 | <.05 | ||
Mode of therapy | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
Individual | 8 | 0.73 | 0.34 | [0.07, 1.39] | 2.17 | <.05 | 3.23 | .072 |
Group | 8 | 1.63 | 0.37 | [0.91, 2.35] | 4.43 | <.001 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
Individual | 8 | 0.68 | 0.18 | [0.33, 1.02] | 3.82 | <.001 | 0.36 | .549 |
Group | 6 | 0.84 | 0.22 | [0.42, 1.27] | 3.90 | <.001 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
Individual | 5 | 0.70 | 0.15 | [0.40, 1.00] | 4.61 | <.001 | 0.08 | .784 |
Group | 7 | 0.64 | 0.14 | [0.37, 0.92] | 4.57 | <.001 | ||
Type of gambling a | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
Electronic | 5 | 1.76 | 0.34 | [1.09, 2.42] | 5.20 | <.001 | 10.31 | <.01 |
Other | 8 | 0.41 | 0.25 | [−0.08, 0.90] | 1.63 | .104 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
Electronic | 7 | 0.88 | 0.13 | [0.63, 1.13] | 7.00 | <.001 | 6.48 | <.05 |
Other | 4 | 0.31 | 0.18 | [−0.05, 0.67] | 1.70 | .090 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
Electronic | 8 | 0.68 | 0.12 | [0.44, 0.91] | 5.64 | <.001 | –b | –b |
Other | 1 | 0.53 | 0.46 | [−0.73, 1.43] | 1.16 | .246 | ||
ITT versus completer analyses | ||||||||
Global severityc | ||||||||
ITT | 7 | 0.17 | 0.21 | [−0.23, 0.58] | 0.84 | .399 | 34.41 | <.001 |
Completers | 8 | 1.97 | 0.23 | [1.53, 2.41] | 8.72 | <.001 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
ITT | 7 | 0.68 | 0.19 | [0.31, 1.05] | 3.60 | <.001 | 0.26 | .609 |
Completers | 7 | 0.82 | 0.20 | [0.43, 1.21] | 4.09 | <.001 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
ITT | 5 | 0.57 | 0.16 | [0.25, 0.89] | 3.52 | <.001 | 0.63 | .428 |
Completers | 7 | 0.74 | 0.13 | [0.48, 1.00] | 5.51 | <.001 | ||
Self-guided treatments | ||||||||
Type of psychological intervention | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
CBT | 4 | 0.47 | 0.27 | [−0.07, 1.00] | 1.72 | .086 | 0.57 | .450 |
Other | 6 | 0.21 | 0.21 | [−0.20, 0.62] | 1.00 | .317 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
CBT | 5 | 0.24 | 0.09 | [0.06, 0.42] | 2.56 | <.05 | 2.50 | .114 |
Other | 15 | 0.06 | 0.06 | [−0.05, 0.18] | 1.08 | .279 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
CBT | 5 | 0.12 | 0.08 | [−0.03, 0.26] | 1.53 | .126 | 0.10 | .752 |
Other | 12 | 0.14 | 0.06 | [0.04, 0.25] | 2.61 | <.01 | ||
Treatment intensity | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
High intensity | 3 | 1.10 | 0.17 | [0.77, 1.43] | 6.54 | <.001 | 40.27 | <.001 |
Low intensity | NA | |||||||
SH | 7 | 0.03 | 0.10 | [−0.16, 0.22] | 0.27 | .784 | ||
EDU | 1 | 0.01 | 0.18 | [−0.34, 0.37] | 0.08 | .939 | ||
PFB | 4 | 0.14 | 0.10 | [−0.06, 0.34] | 1.41 | .160 | ||
WB | 2 | −0.35 | 0.22 | [−0.77, 0.07] | −1.65 | .100 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
High intensity | 2 | 0.71 | 0.17 | [0.39, 1.04] | 4.28 | <.001 | 21.19 | <.001 |
Low intensity | 3 | 0.19 | 0.15 | [−0.10, 0.48] | 1.28 | .201 | ||
SH | 15 | 0.05 | 0.05 | [−0.04, 0.14] | 1.14 | .253 | ||
EDU | 1 | −0.07 | 0.17 | [−0.39, 0.26] | −0.40 | .691 | ||
PFB | 6 | −0.05 | 0.07 | [−0.18, 0.08] | −0.78 | .435 | ||
WB | 8 | 0.17 | 0.07 | [0.04, 0.30] | 2.59 | <.05 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
High intensity | 2 | 0.39 | 0.16 | [0.07, 0.71] | 2.38 | <.05 | 6.90 | .141 |
Low intensity | 3 | 0.43 | 0.17 | [0.11, 0.76] | 2.62 | <.01 | ||
SH | 12 | 0.09 | 0.05 | [−0.008, 0.18] | 1.80 | .073 | ||
EDU | 1 | 0.15 | 0.17 | [−0.17, 0.48] | 0.93 | .354 | ||
PFB | 7 | 0.09 | 0.07 | [−0.04, 0.22] | 1.36 | .173 | ||
WB | 4 | 0.07 | 0.08 | [−0.09, 0.22] | 0.85 | .393 | ||
Therapist contact | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
Contact | 2 | 0.40 | 0.40 | [−0.39, 1.19] | 0.98 | .325 | 0.07 | .799 |
No contact | 8 | 0.28 | 0.19 | [−0.08, 0.65] | 1.52 | .128 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
Contact | 6 | 0.19 | 0.10 | [−0.02, 0.39] | 1.82 | .069 | 0.62 | .433 |
No contact | 14 | 0.09 | 0.06 | [−0.03, 0.21] | 1.50 | .135 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
Contact | 4 | 0.22 | 0.10 | [0.03, 0.41] | 2.28 | .<.05 | 1.05 | .305 |
No contact | 13 | 0.11 | 0.05 | [0.01, 0.21] | 2.22 | .<.05 | ||
Type of gambling | ||||||||
Global severityd | ||||||||
Electronic | 3 | 1.10 | 0.18 | [0.75, 1.44] | 6.24 | <.001 | 28.91 | <.001 |
Other | 4 | −0.12 | 0.14 | [−0.40, 0.16] | −0.84 | .402 | ||
Frequencye | ||||||||
Electronic | 7 | 0.35 | 0.10 | [0.15, 0.55] | 4.34 | <.01 | 5.04 | <.05 |
Other | 4 | 0.06 | 0.08 | [−0.09, 0.22] | 0.77 | .443 | ||
Financial lossf | ||||||||
Electronic | 7 | 0.34 | 0.10 | [0.13, 0.53] | 3.27 | <.01 | 4.52 | <.05 |
Other | 4 | 0.08 | 0.06 | [−0.04, 0.20] | 1.25 | .213 | ||
ITT versus completer analyses | ||||||||
Global severity | ||||||||
ITT | 5 | 0.69 | 0.20 | [0.29, 1.09] | 3.40 | <.01 | 7.55 | <.01 |
Completers | 5 | −0.12 | 0.21 | [−0.53, 0.30] | −0.54 | .586 | ||
Frequency | ||||||||
ITT | 16 | 0.14 | 0.06 | [0.03, 0.25] | 2.55 | <.05 | 1.39 | .238 |
Completers | 4 | −0.02 | 0.12 | [−0.26, 0.22] | −0.15 | .885 | ||
Financial loss | ||||||||
ITT | 12 | 0.11 | 0.05 | [−0.01, 0.21] | 2.18 | <.05 | 1.16 | .283 |
Completers | 5 | 0.23 | 0.10 | [0.04, 0.42] | 2.34 | <.05 |
Note. Due to the small number of studies, moderator analyses were not conducted on the reduction of depression and anxiety. k: number of treatment conditions; CBT: cognitive-behavioral therapy; CI: confidence interval; EDU: education program; g: Hedges’s g; ITT: intention-to-treat; NA: not available; PFB: personalized feedback; Qbetween: homogeneity statistic for differences between subgroups; SE: standard error; SH: self-help; WB: workbook.
Studies excluded: Ladouceur et al. (2003) and Marceaux and Melville (2011), because no information regarding the type of gambling was available.
Moderator analyses were not performed, because only a single study remained in one subgroup.
Studies excluded: Melville et al. (2004), because no information regarding the type of data analysis was available.
Studies excluded: Cunningham et al. (2009) and Martens et al. (2015), because no information regarding the type of gambling was available.
Moderator analyses for continuous variables
Variable | B value | SE | 95% CI | z value | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Face-to-face treatments | |||||
Publication year | |||||
Global severity | −0.09 | 0.06 | [−0.19, −0.02] | −1.55 | .122 |
Frequency | 0.01 | 0.03 | [−0.06, 0.08] | 0.31 | .757 |
Financial loss | −0.01 | 0.02 | [−0.06, 0.03] | −0.60 | .546 |
Treatment duration a | |||||
Global severity | 0.01 | 0.02 | [0.06, 0.15] | 4.71 | <.001 |
Frequency | 0.03 | 0.02 | [0.00, 0.06] | 1.96 | .050 |
Financial loss | 0.08 | 0.02 | [−0.03, 0.05] | 0.43 | .670 |
Quality | |||||
Global severity | −0.33 | 0.15 | [−0.62, −0.04] | −2.20 | <.05 |
Frequency | −0.005 | 0.09 | [−0.17, 0.18] | 0.06 | .951 |
Financial loss | 0.03 | 0.08 | [−0.13, 0.19] | 0.33 | .746 |
Stability | |||||
Global severity | −0.01 | 0.01 | [−0.04, 0.01] | −0.93 | .352 |
Frequency | 0.01 | 0.02 | [−0.02, 0.04] | 0.81 | .420 |
Financial loss | 0.004 | 0.02 | [−0.04, 0.05] | 0.16 | .869 |
Self-guided treatments | |||||
Publication year | |||||
Global severity | −0.04 | 0.60 | [−0.16, 0.07] | −0.72 | .473 |
Frequency | 0.005 | 0.01 | [−0.02, 0.03] | 0.33 | .741 |
Financial loss | −0.008 | 0.01 | [−0.03, 0.02] | −0.65 | .519 |
Quality | |||||
Global severity | 0.20 | 0.18 | [−0.14, 0.55] | 1.16 | .246 |
Frequency | −0.01 | 0.04 | [−0.09, 0.06] | −0.35 | .724 |
Financial loss | 0.02 | 0.04 | [−0.05, 0.10] | 0.66 | .509 |
Stability | |||||
Global severity | −0.05 | 0.04 | [−0.12, 0.03] | −1.19 | .233 |
Frequency | −0.009 | 0.007 | [−0.02, 0.006] | −1.21 | .227 |
Financial loss | −0.002 | 0.008 | [−0.01, 0.02] | 0.22 | .828 |
Note. CI: confidence interval; SE: standard error.
Study excluded: Doiron and Nicki (2007), because no information regarding the hours spent in treatment was available.
The effect sizes were not moderated by the types of psychological interventions and the year of publication in both FTFTs and SGTs. Significantly larger effect sizes, however, were found for individuals preferring electronic gambling compared with those reporting other gambling activities with regard to the reduction of global severity and frequency in FTFTs, as well as all outcome variables in SGTs. In addition, the type of data analysis moderated the effect size for the reduction of global severity in both FTFTs and SGTs. In FTFTs, studies reporting data based on completers produced larger effect sizes compared with those using ITT analyses. In SGTs, larger effect sizes in studies using ITT analyses compared with those reporting data based on completers were found.
When examining FTFTs, study quality and treatment duration moderated the effect size for the reduction of global severity evidencing larger effect sizes in studies with lower validity scores, and applying a higher number of hours spent in treatment. The mode of therapy, however, had no impact on the effect sizes. In SGTs, high-intensity treatments (≥6 sessions/modules) produced significantly larger effect sizes compared with the other categories regarding the reduction of global severity and frequency. Moderator analyses of therapist contact revealed no significant results.
Due to the high variability of follow-up periods, we conducted meta-regression analyses despite the small number of available studies. The results showed that effect sizes at follow-up in both FTFTs and SGTs were not moderated by the length of the periods between posttreatment and follow-up.
FTFTs versus SGTs
The comparisons between the two treatment modalities at posttreatment showed significantly larger effect sizes for FTFTs regarding the reduction of global severity (Qbetween = 6.92, df = 1, p < .01), frequency (Qbetween = 23.38, df = 1, p < .001), and financial loss (Qbetween = 22.74, df = 1, p < .001).
Discussion
The objective of this paper was to investigate the efficacy of FTFTs and SGTs for disordered gambling and to identify possible predictors of treatment outcome. We found that FTFTs effectively reduced frequency and financial loss from gambling at 0–3 months after treatment. We also ascertained strong effect sizes for the reduction of global severity; however, the results from the trim-and-fill analysis indicated asymmetry in the underlying study sample. Although this could be caused by publication bias, it is more reasonable to assume that the between-study heterogeneity led to the funnel asymmetry (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997; Sterne, Egger, & Smith, 2001). Furthermore, the robustness of the training effect of FTFTs to reduce global gambling severity is supported by the FS N analyses. No firm conclusions can be drawn with regard to the treatment success over a longer period of time, since effect sizes at follow-up were not robust. Previous meta-analyses in this area reported comparable short-term effects (Gooding & Tarrier, 2009; Leibetseder et al., 2011; Pallesen et al., 2005), but better long-term effects. The reasons for this may be that effect sizes were pooled across different study designs and across scales with different contents (e.g., abstinence rates and cognitive distortions) (Leibetseder et al., 2011; Pallesen et al., 2005), or that they were limited to CBT (Gooding & Tarrier, 2009). Thus, our results regarding the durability of treatment effects correspond to those reported in the latest meta-analysis based on RCTs (Cowlishaw et al., 2012). Moreover, we extended these results by including minimal FTFTs, SGTs, examining publication bias, and performing moderator analyses.
In accordance with relevant publications emphasizing CBT as the most supported treatment (Rash & Petry, 2014; Toneatto & Millar, 2004), we found an advantage of CBT over other therapies for the reduction of global severity, which, however, did not reach the statistical significance. This reflects the favorable results of the TSF group therapy (Marceaux & Melville, 2011) and CCT (Lee & Awosoga, 2014) yielding effect sizes comparable with those of CBT. Although these therapies proved to be successful in non-controlled trials (Lee & Rovers, 2008; Toneatto & Dragonetti, 2008) as well, more research is required to substantiate their efficacy relative to controls. Treatment success was not affected by the method of delivery in individual or group format, but increased with the number of hours spent in therapy, supporting the results of earlier research in this area (Dowling et al., 2007; Gooding & Tarrier, 2009; Leibetseder et al., 2011; Oei, Raylu, & Casey, 2010; Pallesen et al., 2005). The latter finding suggests that the chronic manifestation of disordered gambling requires more intensive treatment to achieve meaningful improvement. Of note, significant results were found only for the reduction of global severity. Since this effect size was also moderated by the quality of studies and the type of data analysis, the treatment effect for the reduction of global severity may be overestimated, and thus the association between treatment duration and treatment success should be interpreted with caution. Together with the similarly positive relation between treatment benefit and treatment duration for the reduction of frequency, our findings correspond to those reported in meta-analyses investigating other mental health problems (Glombiewski et al., 2010; Khoury et al., 2013; Kleinstäuber, Witthöft, & Hiller, 2011).
With regard to the type of gambling, individuals who preferred electronic gambling seemed to benefit more from treatment than other types of gamblers. The significantly lower effect size of the category subsuming gamblers favoring other gambling activities in FTFTs may be explained by the results of two studies (Petry et al., 2008, 2009), which differed from the remaining studies in applying brief interventions and yielding small effect sizes. Similarly, the significantly larger effect sizes of the category including electronic gamblers in SGTs may be explained by the results of two studies (Carlbring & Smit, 2008; Casey et al., 2017), which differed from the others by using high-intensity treatments and yielding large effect sizes. Therefore, the duration and the intensity of interventions may account for the between-group differences observed. Consequently, our results correspond to those reported in an earlier meta-analysis suggesting that all gamblers may share common mechanisms of addiction, which were effectively reduced during treatment (Gooding & Tarrier, 2009).
Comparable with the moderator analyses on the type of gambling, the significant differences between the effect sizes based on completers and ITT analyses for the reduction of global severity in both FTFTs and SGTs may be explained by the duration and intensity of interventions rather than the type of data analysis. In FTFTs, the relatively small effect size for studies applying ITT analyses may be caused by the small effect sizes of the two studies applying brief interventions (Petry et al., 2008, 2009). In SGTs, the small effect size for studies reporting data based on completers may be explained by the poor results of a trial using pure SH materials (Luquiens & Tanguy, 2016).
According to our hypothesis, SGTs yielded significantly inferior results compared with FTFTs. A number of reasons might account for these findings. First, the majority of SGTs included pure SH resources (e.g., CBT-based WBs, and PFB materials typically processed within a brief, single session) indicating limited treatment benefit relative to controls. Within the alcohol addiction area, significant but comparably small effect sizes were found for the use of bibliotherapy materials (Apodaca & Miller, 2003) and for brief Internet-delivered interventions (including studies using PFB) to reduce alcohol consumption for college drinkers (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Elliott, Garey, & Carey, 2012), adult problem drinking (Riper et al., 2009, 2011, 2014), and alcohol and tobacco use (Rooke, Thorsteinsson, Karpin, Copeland, & Allsop, 2010). Alternatively, the poor results of one trial (Luquiens & Tanguy, 2016) may have influenced the treatment effects, which may especially be true for the reduction of global severity. Non-treatment-seeking online gamblers, particularly those assigned to the e-mail-guided CBT program, failed to show treatment success compared with controls. According to the study authors, the disapproving attitude of non-treatment-seeking gamblers toward therapeutic guidance, the therapist contact solely occurring over the Internet, and the lack of intrinsic motivation to change gambling behavior may have contributed to high attrition rates and poor treatment success. More general aspects, such as baseline assessments raising the awareness of problematic behavior, regression to the mean (Swan & Hodgins, 2015), or the natural recovery from gambling (e.g., Slutske, 2006) may also account for small between-group differences, particularly affecting brief interventions, which show limited treatment benefit (see also Hansen et al., 2012; Kypri, 2007).
Although a closer examination of our data showed that pure SH materials may not be helpful for all gamblers and for all behavioral aspects of gambling, some affected individuals may benefit from these treatment formats. Particularly notable is the use of WBs, which proved to reduce gambling frequency to a small, however, significant extent. Similarly, PFB materials showed a trend to ameliorate global symptom severity highlighting the clinical relevance of these SH formats, particularly for at-risk and problem gamblers (e.g., Marchica & Derevensky, 2016). Despite the small effect sizes, brief SH materials – especially when delivered over the Internet – offer key advantages compared with traditional, extensive FTFTs as they reach a large number of individuals, ensure cost-effectiveness, and provide treatment flexibility and anonymity (Gainsbury & Blaszczynski, 2011). Moreover, considering variations in motivational levels and symptom severity of gamblers, these treatment options align with the notion of the different stages of behavior change (Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992) and the conceptualization of the stepped-care approach recommended for affected individuals (Gainsbury & Blaszczynski, 2011; Swan & Hodgins, 2015; Weinstock & Rash, 2014).
Nonetheless, the moderator analyses revealed more effective SH options providing support for high-intensity, structured Internet-based programs with MI and CBT elements (Carlbring & Smit, 2008; Casey et al., 2017). The effect size is equivalent to that obtained from FTFTs. Since only two such trials were available, the results should be interpreted with caution, and require further research. This preliminary finding, however, corresponds to those of more intense, Internet-delivered treatments for the reduction of mood and anxiety disorders (e.g., Andersson & Cuijpers, 2009; Arnberg, Linton, Hultcrantz, Heintz, & Jonsson, 2014; Haug et al., 2012; Richards & Richardson, 2012) suggesting that structured self-guided Internet-based interventions may lead to treatment benefits, possibly comparable with those found in FTFTs, and superior to bibliotherapy materials (Haug et al., 2012). Moreover, considering the reduction of financial loss, we found a significant and medium effect size for low-intensity treatments (i.e., a single session of MI delivered over the telephone alone or in combination with a CBT SH WB), which is comparable with that of high-intensity treatments underscoring the efficacy of MI for disordered gambling that represents a promising option for concerned individuals to change problematic behavior (Yakovenko, Quigley, Hemmelgarn, Hodgins, & Ronksley, 2015). It should be noted that MI was delivered through personal communication with therapists. Similar to the research focusing on CBT SH for the treatment of depression and anxiety (e.g., Andersson & Cuijpers, 2009; Spek et al., 2007), SGTs for disordered gambling with therapist contact showed an advantage over those without therapist support, although between-group differences were not significant. This finding may be due to the small number of studies including therapist contact, or mirrors the lack of acceptance of therapist guidance by concerned gamblers (Apodaca & Miller, 2003; Luquiens & Tanguy, 2016). Given the preliminary nature of these results, further studies are required to systematically investigate the influence of therapist support in SGTs on treatment outcome including the amount and the type of therapist support (Haug et al., 2012).
The limitations of this meta-analytic review include the following: First and foremost, our meta-analysis covered a relatively small number of studies. However, the short-term effect sizes were robust. Second, as is true for most meta-analytic reviews, the included studies differed in their methodological quality. However, we did not observe a systematic bias in the effect sizes due to differences in the qualities of the studies. Third, study participants varied in terms of gambling severity (pathological, moderate, or problem/at-risk gamblers). This variable was not considered in this meta-analysis, because some studies examining SGTs obtained symptoms of gambling pathology over the Internet. Despite the benefits of online tests for gamblers (for a review, see Wood & Griffiths, 2007), the validity of online diagnoses seems to be uncertain, because systematic investigations evidencing the psychometrical properties of traditional offline instruments used on the Internet have not been carried out (Andersson & Titov, 2014; Buchanan, 2003). Furthermore, most studies examining SGTs included gamblers with varying degrees of symptom severity without reporting separate data for the different subgroups of gamblers. Because these studies could not be considered for inclusion in subgroup analyses, meaningful results around this moderator were precluded due to the lack of data. Therefore, future studies are encouraged to report separate data for “problem/at-risk” and “pathological” gamblers to examine the impact of the different degrees of symptom severity on treatment outcomes. Similarly, the influential aspect of comorbidity on treatment outcomes was not addressed in this meta-analysis, because only a limited number of studies indicated the types and rates of comorbid disorders. Therefore, it remains an important issue for further investigation (e.g., Hodgins & el-Guebaly, 2010; Hodgins, Peden, & Cassidy, 2005). In addition, some participants were recruited from the general population through advertisements or from universities. Consequently, the results cannot be generalized to all patients (see also Cowlishaw et al., 2012). Again, this is a rather typical study limitation.
Despite these limitations, this study favors FTFTs over SGTs for the reduction of problematic gambling behavior, especially, when conducted over an extended period of time. Given the rapid growth of gambling problems, future research requires to further improve the treatments for this disabling condition.
Authors’ contribution
MG conducted the literature search, extracted the data, and performed the analyses. Papers for inclusion in the meta-analysis were screened by MG and ML, who also validated the data extraction. A-RL supervised these processes. MG and ML rated the validity of the studies. HMM supported the organization of the data and gave statistical advice. The manuscript was written by MG with comments provided by HMM, A-RL, and SGH. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of interest
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr. Amandine Luquiens for providing additional data, and also thank Anita Todd, Leigh Andrews, and Kristina Conroy, who assisted in the proofreading of the manuscript.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.
Abbott, M. , Bellringer, M. , Hodgins, D. , Palmer Du Preez, K. , Landon, J. , Sullivan, S., & Feigin, V. (2012). Effectiveness of problem gambling brief telephone interventions: A randomised controlled trial. Final report prepared for the Ministry of Health, Wellington, New Zealand. Retrieved from http://www.health.govt.nz/system/files/documents/pages/effectiveness-problem-gambling-brief-telephone-interventions.pdf
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Andersson, G., & Cuijpers, P. (2009). Internet-based and other computerized psychological treatments for adult depression: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38, 196–205. doi:10.1080/16506070903318960
Andersson, G., & Titov, N. (2014). Advantages and limitations of Internet‐based interventions for common mental disorders. World Psychiatry, 13, 4–11. doi:10.1002/wps.20083
Apodaca, T. R., & Miller, W. R. (2003). A meta‐analysis of the effectiveness of bibliotherapy for alcohol problems. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 59, 289–304. doi:10.1002/jclp.10130
Arnberg, F. K. , Linton, S. J. , Hultcrantz, M. , Heintz, E., & Jonsson, U. (2014). Internet-delivered psychological treatments for mood and anxiety disorders: A systematic review of their efficacy, safety, and cost-effectiveness. PLoS One, 9(5), e98118. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098118
Barak, A. , Hen, L. , Boniel-Nissim, M., & Shapira, N. (2008). A comprehensive review and a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of Internet-based psychotherapeutic interventions. Journal of Technology in Human Services, 26, 109–160. doi:10.1080/15228830802094429
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics, 50, 1088–1101. doi:10.2307/2533446
Blackman, S. , Simone, R. V., & Thoms, D. R. (1989). The gamblers treatment clinic of St. Vincent’s North Richmond Community Mental Health Center: Characteristics of the clients and outcome of treatment. International Journal of the Addictions, 24, 29–37. doi:10.3109/10826088909047273
Borenstein, M. , Hedges, L. V. , Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2005). Comprehensive meta-analysis, version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat Inc.
Borenstein, M. , Hedges, L. V. , Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Buchanan, T. (2003). Internet-Based Questionnaire assessment: Appropriate use in clinical contexts. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 32, 100–109. doi:10.1080/16506070310000957
Carey, K. B. , Scott-Sheldon, L. A. , Elliott, J. C. , Garey, L., & Carey, M. P. (2012). Face-to-face versus computer-delivered alcohol interventions for college drinkers: A meta-analytic review, 1998 to 2010. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 690–703. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.08.001
*Carlbring, P., & Smit, F. (2008). Randomized trial of Internet-delivered self-help with telephone support for pathological gamblers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 1090–1094. doi:10.1037/a0013603
*Casey, L. M. , Oei, T. P. , Raylu, N. , Horrigan, K. , Day, J. , Ireland, M., & Clough, B. A. (2017). Internet-based delivery of cognitive behaviour therapy compared to monitoring, feedback and support for problem gambling: A randomised controlled trial. Journal of Gambling Studies. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10899-016-9666-y
Celio, M. A., & Lisman, S. A. (2014). Examining the efficacy of a personalized normative feedback intervention to reduce college student gambling. Journal of American College Health, 62, 154–164. doi:10.1080/07448481.2013.865626
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (Rev. ed .). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cowlishaw, S. , Merkouris, S. , Dowling, N. , Anderson, C. , Jackson, A., & Thomas, S. (2012). Psychological therapies for pathological and problem gambling. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 11(11), CD008937. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD008937.pub2
*Cunningham, J. A. , Hodgins, D. C. , Toneatto, T., & Murphy, M. (2012). A randomized controlled trial of a personalized feedback intervention for problem gamblers. PLoS One, 7(2), e31586. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031586
*Cunningham, J. A. , Hodgins, D. C. , Toneatto, T. , Rai, A., & Cordingley, J. (2009). Pilot study of a personalized feedback intervention for problem gamblers. Behavior Therapy, 40, 219–224. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2008.06.005
Deeks, J. J. , Dinnes, J. , D’Amico, R. , Sowden, A. , Sakarovitch, C. , Song, F. , Petticrew, M., & Altman, D. (2003). Evaluating non-randomised intervention studies. Health Technology Assessment, 7, 1–173. doi:10.3310/hta7270
Deeks, J. J. , Higgins, J. P. T., & Altman, D. G. (2011). Analysing data and undertaking meta-analysis. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook of systematic reviews and interventions (Chapter 9). Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). Retrieved from www.cochrane-handbook.org
Diskin, K. M., & Hodgins, D. C. (2009). A randomized controlled trial of a single session motivational intervention for concerned gamblers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 382–388. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.018
*Doiron, J. P., & Nicki, R. M. (2007). Prevention of pathological gambling: A randomized controlled trial. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 36, 74–84. doi:10.1080/16506070601092966
Dowling, N. , Smith, D., & Thomas, T. (2006). Treatment of female pathological gambling: The efficacy of a cognitive-behavioural approach. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22, 355–372. doi:10.1007/s10899-006-9027-3
*Dowling, N. , Smith, D., & Thomas, T. (2007). A comparison of individual and group cognitive-behavioural treatment for female pathological gambling. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2192–2202. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.003
Dowling, N. , Smith, D., & Thomas, T. (2009). A preliminary investigation of abstinence and controlled gambling as self-selected goals of treatment for female pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 25, 201–214. doi:10.1007/s10899-009-9116-1
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel‐plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta‐analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463. doi:10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x
*Echeburúa, E. , Báez, C., & Fernández-Montalvo, J. (1996). Comparative effectiveness of three therapeutic modalities in the psychological treatment of pathological gambling: Long-term outcome. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 24, 51–72. doi:10.1017/S1352465800016830
Egger, M. , Smith, G. D. , Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ, 315, 629–634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Feinstein, R. , Heiman, N., & Yager, J. (2015). Common factors affecting psychotherapy outcomes: Some implications for teaching psychotherapy. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 21, 180–189. doi:10.1097/PRA.0000000000000064
Gainsbury, S., & Blaszczynski, A. (2011). Online self-guided interventions for the treatment of problem gambling. International Gambling Studies, 11, 289–308. doi:10.1080/14459795.2011.617764
Glombiewski, J. A. , Sawyer, A. T. , Gutermann, J. , Koenig, K. , Rief, W., & Hofmann, S. G. (2010). Psychological treatments for fibromyalgia: A meta-analysis. Pain, 151, 280–295. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.06.011
Gooding, P., & Tarrier, N. (2009). A systematic review and meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioural interventions to reduce problem gambling: Hedging our bets? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 592–607. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.04.002
Grant, J. E. , Donahue, C. B. , Odlaug, B. L. , Kim, S. W. , Miller, M. J., & Petry, N. M. (2009). Imaginal desensitisation plus motivational interviewing for pathological gambling: Randomised controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 266–267. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.108.062414
Hansen, A. B. G. , Becker, U. , Nielsen, A. S. , Grønbæk, M. , Tolstrup, J. S., & Thygesen, L. C. (2012). Internet-based brief personalized feedback intervention in a non-treatment-seeking population of adult heavy drinkers: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 14(4), e98. doi:10.2196/jmir.1883
Haug, T. , Nordgreen, T. , Öst, L. G., & Havik, O. E. (2012). Self-help treatment of anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis and meta-regression of effects and potential moderators. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 425–445. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.04.002
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1984). Nonparametric estimators of effect size in meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 573–580. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.96.3.573
Higgins, J. P. T., & Deeks, J. J. (2011). Selecting studies and collecting data. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Chapter 7). Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). Retrieved from www.cochrane-handbook.org
Higgins, J. P. T. , Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2011). Special topics in statistics. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Chapter 16). Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). Retrieved from www.cochrane-handbook.org
Higgins, J. P. T. , Thompson, S. G. , Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ, 327, 557–560. doi:10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
*Hodgins, D. C. , Currie, S. R. , Currie, G., & Fick, G. H. (2009). Randomized trial of brief motivational treatments for pathological gamblers: More is not necessarily better. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 950–960. doi:10.1037/a0016318
*Hodgins, D. C. , Currie, S. R., & el-Guebaly, N. (2001). Motivational enhancement and self-help treatments for problem gambling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 50–57. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.69.1.50
Hodgins, D. C., & el-Guebaly, N. (2010). The influence of substance dependence and mood disorders on outcome from pathological gambling: Five-year follow-up. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26, 117–127. doi:10.1007/s10899-009-9137-9
Hodgins, D. C. , Peden, N., & Cassidy, E. (2005). The association between comorbidity and outcome in pathological gambling: A prospective follow-up of recent quitters. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21, 255–271. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-3099-3
*Hopper, R. A. H. (2008). A brief electronic personalized normative feedback intervention for the prevention of problematic gambling among college students (Doctoral dissertation). Oklahoma State University. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/304382980
Khoury, B. , Lecomte, T. , Fortin, G. , Masse, M. , Therien, P. , Bouchard, V. , Chapleau, M.-A. , Paquin, K., & Hofmann, S. G. (2013). Mindfulness-based therapy: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 763–771. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.05.005
Kleinstäuber, M. , Witthöft, M., & Hiller, W. (2011). Efficacy of short-term psychotherapy for multiple medically unexplained physical symptoms: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 146–160. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.09.001
Kypri, K. (2007). Methodological issues in alcohol screening and brief intervention research. Substance Abuse, 28, 31–42. doi:10.1300/J465v28n03_04
*LaBrie, R. A. , Peller, A. J. , Laplante, D. A. , Bernhard, B. , Harper, A. , Schrier, T., & Shaffer, H. J. (2012). A brief self-help toolkit intervention for gambling problems: A randomized multisite trial. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 82, 278–289. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2012.01157.x
*Ladouceur, R. , Sylvain, C. , Boutin, C. , Lachance, S. , Doucet, C., & Leblond, J. (2003). Group therapy for pathological gamblers: A cognitive approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 587–596. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00036-0
*Ladouceur, R. , Sylvain, C. , Boutin, C. , Lachance, S. , Doucet, C. , Leblond, J., & Jacques, C. (2001). Cognitive treatment of pathological gambling. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189, 774–780. doi:10.1097/00005053-200111000-00007
*Larimer, M. E. , Neighbors, C. , Lostutter, T. W. , Whiteside, U. , Cronce, J. M. , Kaysen, D., & Walker, D. D. (2012). Brief motivational feedback and cognitive behavioral interventions for prevention of disordered gambling: A randomized clinical trial. Addiction, 107, 1148–1158. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03776.x
*Lee, B. K., & Awosoga, O. (2014). Congruence couple therapy for pathological gambling: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31, 1047–1068. doi:10.1007/s10899-014-9464-3
Lee, B. K., & Rovers, M. (2008). ‘Bringing torn lives together again’: Effects of the first congruence couple therapy training application to clients in pathological gambling. International Gambling Studies, 8, 113–129. doi:10.1080/14459790701878137
Lefebvre, C. , Manheimer, E., & Glanville, J. (2011). Searching for studies. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Chapter 6). Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). Retrieved from www.cochrane-handbook.org
Leibetseder, M. , Laireiter, A.-R. , Vierhauser, M., & Hittenberger, B. (2011). Efficacy and effectiveness of psychological and psycho-pharmacological treatments in pathological gambling – A meta-analysis. Sucht, 57, 275–285. doi:10.1024/0939-5911.a000120
Lesieur, H. R., & Blume, S. B. (1991). Evaluation of patients treated for pathological gambling in a combined alcohol, substance abuse and pathological gambling treatment unit using the Addiction Severity Index. British Journal of Addiction, 86, 1017–1028. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01863.x
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. (2000). Practical meta-analysis (applied social research methods). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
*Luquiens, A., & Tanguy, M. L. (2016). The efficacy of three modalities of Internet-based psychotherapy for non-treatment-seeking online problem gamblers: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 18(2), e36. doi:10.2196/jmir.4752
*Marceaux, J. C., & Melville, C. L. (2011). Twelve-step facilitated versus mapping-enhanced cognitive-behavioral therapy for pathological gambling: A controlled study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27, 171–190. doi:10.1007/s10899-010-9196-y
Marchica, L., & Derevensky, J. L. (2016). Examining personalized feedback interventions for gambling disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5, 1–10. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.006
*Martens, M. P. , Arterberry, B. J. , Takamatsu, S. K. , Masters, J., & Dude, K. (2015). The efficacy of a personalized feedback-only intervention for at-risk college gamblers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 494–499. doi:10.1037/a0038843
*Melville, C. L. , Davis, C. S. , Matzenbacher, D. L., & Clayborne, J. (2004). Node-link-mapping-enhanced group treatment for pathological gambling. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 73–87. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(03)00091-1
Moher, D. , Liberati, A. , Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151, 264–269. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135
*Myrseth, H. , Litlere, I. , Stoylen, I. J., & Pallesen, S. (2009). A controlled study of the effect of cognitive-behavioural group therapy for pathological gamblers. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 63, 22–31. doi:10.1080/08039480802055139
*Neighbors, C. , Rodriguez, L. M. , Rinker, D. V. , Gonzales, R. G. , Agana, M. , Tackett, J. L., & Foster, D. W. (2015). Efficacy of personalized normative feedback as a brief intervention for college student gambling: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 500–511. doi:10.1037/a0039125
*Oei, T. P. , Raylu, N., & Casey, L. M. (2010). Effectiveness of group and individual formats of a combined motivational interviewing and cognitive behavioral treatment program for problem gambling: A randomized controlled trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 38, 233–238. doi:10.1017/S1352465809990701
Pallesen, S. , Mitsem, M. , Kvale, G. , Johnsen, B. H., & Molde, H. (2005). Outcome of psychological treatments of pathological gambling: A review and meta-analysis. Addiction, 100, 1412–1422. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01204.x
Petry, N. M. (2005). Pathological gambling: Etiology, comorbidity, and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Petry, N. M. , Ammerman, Y. , Bohl, J. , Doersch, A. , Gay, H. , Kadden, R. , Molina, C., & Steinberg, K. (2006). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for pathological gamblers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 555–567. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.555
*Petry, N. M. , Weinstock, J. , Ledgerwood, D. M., & Morasco, B. (2008). A randomized trial of brief interventions for problem and pathological gamblers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 318–328. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.318
*Petry, N. M. , Weinstock, J. , Morasco, B. J., & Ledgerwood, D. M. (2009). Brief motivational interventions for college student problem gamblers. Addiction, 104, 1569–1578. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02652.x
Prochaska, J. O. , DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C. (1992). In search of how people change: Applications to addictive behaviors. American Psychologist, 47, 1102–1114. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.47.9.1102
Rash, C. J., & Petry, N. M. (2014). Psychological treatments for gambling disorder. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 7, 285–295. doi:10.2147/prbm.s40883
Raylu, N. , Loo, J., & Oei, T. P. (2013). Treatment of gambling problems in Asia: Comprehensive review and implications for Asian problem gamblers. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 27, 297–322. doi:10.1891/0889-8391.27.3.297
Raylu, N. , Oei, T. P., & Loo, J. (2008). The current status and future direction of self-help treatments for problem gamblers. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1372–1385. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.07.005
Richards, D., & Richardson, T. (2012). Computer-based psychological treatments for depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 329–342. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.004
Riper, H. , Blankers, M. , Hadiwijaya, H. , Cunningham, J. , Clarke, S. , Wiers, R. , Ebert, D., & Cuijpers, P. (2014). Effectiveness of guided and unguided low-intensity Internet interventions for adult alcohol misuse: A meta-analysis. PLoS One, 9(6), e99912. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099912
Riper, H. , Spek, V. , Boon, B. , Conijn, B. , Kramer, J. , Martin-Abello, K., & Smit, F. (2011). Effectiveness of E-self-help interventions for curbing adult problem drinking: A meta-analysis. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 13(2), e42. doi:10.2196/jmir.1691
Riper, H. , van Straten, A. , Keuken, M. , Smit, F. , Schippers, G., & Cuijpers, P. (2009). Curbing problem drinking with personalized-feedback interventions: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 36, 247–255. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2008.10.016
Rooke, S. , Thorsteinsson, E. , Karpin, A. , Copeland, J., & Allsop, D. (2010). Computer‐delivered interventions for alcohol and tobacco use: A meta‐analysis. Addiction, 105, 1381–1390. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.02975.x
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research (Vol. 6). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Slutske, W. S. (2006). Natural recovery and treatment-seeking in pathological gambling: Results of two U.S. national surveys. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 297–302. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.297
Sobell, L., & Sobell, M. (1992). Timeline follow-back. In R. Litten & J. Allen (Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption (pp. 41–72). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press.
Spek, V. , Cuijpers, P. , Nyklícek, I. , Riper, H. , Keyzer, J., & Pop, V. (2007). Internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy for symptoms of depression and anxiety: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 37, 319–328. doi:10.1017/S0033291706008944
Stea, J. N., & Hodgins, D. C. (2011). A critical review of treatment approaches for gambling disorders. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 4, 67–80. doi:10.2174/1874473711104020067
Sterne, J. A. C. , Egger, M., & Moher, D. (2011). Addressing reporting biases. In J. P. T. Higgins & S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions (Chapter 10). Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). Retrieved from www.cochrane-handbook.org
Sterne, J. A. C. , Egger, M., & Smith, G. D. (2001). Investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. British Medical Journal, 323, 101–105. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7304.101
Suurvali, H. , Hodgins, D. C., & Cunningham, J. A. (2010). Motivators for resolving or seeking help for gambling problems: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26, 1–33. doi:10.1007/s10899-009-9151-y
Swan, J. L., & Hodgins, D. C. (2015). Brief interventions for disordered gambling. Canadian Journal of Addiction, 6, 29–36.
*Sylvain, C. , Ladouceur, R., & Boisvert, J.-M. (1997). Cognitive and behavioral treatment of pathological gambling: A controlled study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 727–732. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.65.5.727
Takushi, R. Y. , Neighbors, C. , Larimer, M. E. , Lostutter, T. W. , Cronce, J. M., & Marlatt, G. A. (2004). Indicated prevention of problem gambling among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20, 83–93. doi:10.1023/B:JOGS.0000016705.58903.8f
Thomas, B. H. , Ciliska, D. , Dobbins, M., & Micucci, S. (2004). A process for systematically reviewing the literature: Providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews on Evidence‐Based Nursing, 1, 176–184. doi:10.1111/j.1524-475X.2004.04006.x
Thompson, S. G., & Higgins, J. P. T. (2002). How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1559–1573. doi:10.1002/sim.1187
Toneatto, T., & Dragonetti, R. (2008). Effectiveness of community-based treatment for problem gambling: A quasi-experimental evaluation of cognitive-behavioral vs. twelve-step therapy. American Journal on Addictions, 17, 298–303. doi:10.1080/10550490802138830
Toneatto, T., & Millar, G. (2004). Assessing and treating problem gambling: Empirical status and promising trends. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 517–525. doi:10.1177/070674370404900803
*Toneatto, T. , Pillai, S., & Courtice, E. L. (2014). Mindfulness-enhanced cognitive behavior therapy for problem gambling: A controlled pilot study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12, 197–205. doi:10.1007/s11469-014-9481-6
Walker, M. , Toneatto, T. , Potenza, M. N. , Petry, N. M. , Ladouceur, R. , Hodgins, D. C. , el-Guebaly, N. , Echeburua, E., & Blaszczynski, A. (2006). A framework for reporting outcomes in problem gambling treatment research: The Banff, Alberta Consensus. Addiction, 101, 504–511. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01341.x
Wampold, B. E. (2015). How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update. World Psychiatry, 14, 270–277. doi:10.1002/wps.20238
*Watson, A. N. (2012). Exploring a treatment for problem gambling by at-risk adolescents: A randomized trial of a brief intervention (Doctoral dissertation). The University of Memphis. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1080814179.
Weinstock, J., & Rash, C. J. (2014). Clinical and research implications of gambling disorder in DSM-5. Current Addiction Reports, 1, 159–165. doi:10.1007/s40429-014-0026-7
Wood, R. T., & Griffiths, M. D. (2007). Online data collection from gamblers: Methodological issues. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 5, 151–163. doi:10.1007/s11469-007-9055-y
Yakovenko, I. , Quigley, L. , Hemmelgarn, B. R. , Hodgins, D. C., & Ronksley, P. (2015). The efficacy of motivational interviewing for disordered gambling: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 43, 72–82. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.12.011