Abstract
Background and aims
Gambling disorder (GD) may have its onset in a wide range of ages, from adolescents to old adults. In addition, individuals with GD tend to seek treatment at different moments in their lives. As a result of these characteristics (variable age at onset and variable age at treatment seeking), we find subjects with diverse duration of illness (DOI) in clinical practice. DOI is an important but relatively understudied factor in GD. Our objective was to investigate clinical and neurocognitive characteristics associated with different DOI.
Methods
This study evaluated 448 adults diagnosed with GD. All assessments were completed prior to treatments being commenced.
Results
Our main results were: (a) there is a negative correlation between DOI and lag between first gambling and onset of GD; (b) lifetime history of alcohol use disorder (AUD) is associated with a longer duration of GD; (c) the presence of a first-degree relative with history of AUD is associated with a more extended course of GD; and (d) there is a negative correlation between DOI and quality of life.
Discussion
This study suggests that some important variables are associated with different DOI. Increasing treatment-seeking behavior, providing customized psychological interventions, and effectively managing AUD may decrease the high levels of chronicity in GD. Furthermore, research on GD such as phenomenological studies and clinical trials may consider the duration of GD in their methodology. DOI might be an important variable when analyzing treatment outcome and avoiding confounders.
Introduction
Gambling disorder (GD) affects a significant number of people; for example, up to 3.24 million people have developed GD at some point of their lives in the United States alone (United States Census Bureau, 2016). The disorder is associated with several negative consequences, such as high rates of psychiatric comorbidity and suicidality, frequent legal and occupational problems as well as with lower levels of quality of life (Black, Moyer, & Schlosser, 2003; Grant & Kim, 2005; Petry & Kiluk, 2002; Petry, Stinson, & Grant, 2005). GD poses a considerable economic burden, estimated at $5 billion every year in the United States alone (National Gambling Impact Study Commission, United States of America, 1999). GD is a clinically heterogeneous disorder and different subgroups of affected subjects have been examined (Ledgerwood & Petry, 2006; Ledgerwood, Weinstock, Morasco, & Petry, 2007). A deeper understanding of the subtypes of GD may lead to customized, more effective treatments. This is particularly important given that GD tends to demonstrate high chronicity and relapse rates (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Blanco, Moreyra, Nunes, Saiz-Ruiz, & Ibanez, 2001; Potenza, 2001).
One important but relatively understudied factor in GD is the duration of illness (DOI). For instance, GD may have its onset in a wide range of ages, from adolescents to old adults (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Burge, Pietrzak, Molina, & Petry, 2004; Kessler et al., 2008; Ladd, Molina, Kerins, & Petry, 2003; Lynch, Maciejewski, & Potenza, 2004; Medeiros et al., 2015; Shaffer, Hall, & Vander Bilt, 1999). In addition, individuals with GD tend to seek treatment at different moments in their lives (Petry, 2002). As a result of these characteristics (variable age at onset and variable age at treatment seeking), we find patients with diverse DOI in clinical practice.
Studies with other addictions (substance-associated and other behavioral addictions) have shown the importance of DOI in the clinical presentation of the disorders. In the case of alcohol and other substance use disorders, studies have suggested that longer duration of alcohol use disorder (AUD), methamphetamine use disorder, and heroin use are correlated with more cognitive deficits. This is particularly true for response inhibition, reaction time, and cognitive flexibility (Laloyaux et al., 2012; Martinovic-Mitrovic et al., 2008; Monterosso, Aron, Cordova, Xu, & London, 2005). Moreover, research in AUD suggested that early onset of problems with alcohol is associated with stronger genetic susceptibility and poorer treatment outcome (Babor et al., 1992; Carpenter & Hasin, 2001; Cloninger, 1987; Schuckit et al., 1995). These two elements (genetic vulnerability and worse prognosis) may lead to longer DOI. With regard to behavioral addictions, Mathy and Cooper (2003) found that longer duration of exposure to the Internet was associated with negative outcomes in a non-clinical sample of Internet users. Regarding GD, Tsurumi et al. (2014) found a negative correlation between DOI and insula activation, a neurological area associated with reward anticipation and risk evaluation (Damasio, 1994; Kuhnen & Knutson, 2005; Xue, Lu, Levin, & Bechara, 2010). These findings suggest the need for further clinical and neurocognitive investigations of the influence of DOI in GD. Although DOI seems to be an important variable in understanding GD, no study, to our knowledge, has specifically investigated how this variable might affect the clinical presentation of the disorder.
Our objective was to investigate clinical and neurocognitive characteristics associated with different DOI in a sample of adults with GD. It was anticipated that this study would provide insights regarding how future work might improve treatments for patients. Our main hypotheses were that a longer DOI would be significantly associated with: (a) worse clinical outcomes such as more financial losses, worse severity of GD, and reduced quality of life; (b) a stronger genetic vulnerability clinically evidenced by personal history of AUD and family history of GD and AUD; and (c) worse neurocognitive deficits, especially in response inhibition and cognitive flexibility.
Methods
Participants
This study evaluated 448 adults diagnosed with GD. In all, 197 (44.0%) individuals were males and 251 individuals (56.0%) were females. The mean and median age of our sample were 47.6 (±11.3) and 49.0 years, respectively. The studied sample consisted of individuals who participated in clinical trials on GD (pharmacotherapy or cognitive behavioral therapy) at the University of Minnesota and at the University of Chicago. The subjects were recruited by media advertisements (public places, Internet, and newspapers). All assessments were completed prior to treatments being commenced.
The inclusion criteria were: (a) age ≥18 years, (b) current GD diagnosis, and (c) able to attend the clinical center for assessment. This research excluded subjects who: (a) needed emergency care or presented unstable medical illness; (b) showed clinically significant abnormalities on physical examination; (c) were unable to complete the study procedures; (d) demonstrated psychotic symptoms; and (e) did not provide written consent to participate in the study.
All studied individuals underwent a semi-structured clinical interview conducted by a board-certified and research-trained psychiatrist. Individuals were financially compensated (gift cards to local stores) for their participation in this study.
Measures
GD diagnosis
The diagnosis of GD was performed using the Structured Clinical Interview for Pathological Gambling. This diagnostic instrument was originally developed and validated using the criteria of the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) (Grant, Steinberg, Kim, Rounsaville, & Potenza, 2004). As the data collected before the release of DSM-5 were electronically saved, the authors retrospectively processed them for a proper adaptation to DSM-5 GD criteria. This approach consisted of removing the criterion “committed illegal acts such as forgery, fraud, theft, or embezzlement to finance gambling regarding illegal acts,” which was present in previous manual, DSM-IV. Moreover, we reduced the diagnostic threshold from five to four criteria, consistent with DSM-5. The remaining criteria were unchanged.
During the diagnostic interview, we also evaluated [age at first gambling] and [age at onset of GD]. The trained board-certified psychiatrists conducted all diagnostic interviews.
DOI
DOI was defined as the difference between age at intake and age when full criteria for GD met.
Demographics
Age at intake, gender, marital status, educational status, and ethnicity were obtained from the participants.
Gambling behavior
-
– Age at first gambling and progression from recreational gambling to GD (lag between first gambling and onset of GD): this variable was calculated as the difference between [age at onset of GD] and [age at first gambling] (collected in the diagnostic interview – see GD Diagnosis subsection).
-
– Gambling frequency: it is assessed by how many days per week the subject gambled.
-
– Primary form of gambling: the participants were asked to specify the main form of gambling based on money spent, frequency, and negative consequences. We grouped the form of betting in strategic (sports betting, cards, dice, stock markets, etc.) and non-strategic methods (slot machines, pull tabs, lottery, video poker, etc.) (Grant, Odlaug, Chamberlain, et al., 2012).
-
– Monetary losses: we assessed money lost due to gambling in the last year (in American dollars) and income in the last year (in American dollars). The coefficient of money lost/income provided the percentage of income lost in gambling (last year).
-
– Severity of GD: we evaluated GD severity using the Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale (G-SAS) and the Yale–Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale modified for Pathological Gambling (PG-YBOCS). The G-SAS is a valid and reliable 12-item scale that investigates gambling symptoms in the past week (Kim, Grant, Potenza, Blanco, & Hollander, 2009). The PG-YBOCS is a 10-item questionnaire that has demonstrated high validity (r = .895) and reliability (Cronbach’s α = .970) (Pallanti, DeCaria, Grant, Urpe, & Hollander, 2005). The PG-YBOCS provides an overall score as well as scores in two subscales: (a) urge subscale and (b) behavior subscale.
Psychiatric symptoms/antecedents and quality of life
-
– Lifetime prevalence of co-occurring psychiatric disorders: the subjects were assessed for co-occurring psychiatric disorders by two validated semi-structured interviews: the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) and the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview (M.I.D.I.). The M.I.N.I. evaluates the lifetime existence of main co-occurring psychiatric disorder comorbidities (Sheehan et al., 1998), whereas the M.I.D.I. assesses impulsive/compulsive behaviors (Odlaug & Grant, 2010).
-
– Current depressive and anxiety symptoms: the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAM-A) is a valid and reliable 14-item questionnaire that evaluates the severity of anxiety symptoms in the past week (Beck & Steer, 1991; Hamilton, 1969; Maier, Buller, Philipp, & Heuser, 1988; Snaith, Baugh, Clayden, Husain, & Sipple, 1982).
-
– Previous treatment for GD: the subjects were asked (self-report measure) if they had sought previous treatment specifically for GD. Individual outpatient treatment, group outpatient treatment, and inpatient treatment were considered formalized treatments.
-
– Family history of GD and AUD: the family history of AUD was evaluated as a self-report measure from the subject. They were asked if they had a first-degree relative with GD or AUD.
-
– Quality of life: it is investigated by the Quality of Life Inventory (QOLI), which is a 17-item questionnaire that assesses the subjects’ overall quality of life (Frisch et al., 1992).
Neurocognitive measures
the participants undertook two selected tests from the computerized Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTABeclipse, version 3, Cambridge Cognition Ltd., Cambridge, UK; Cambridge Cognition, 2015).
-
– Response inhibition: it is evaluated by the stop-signal task. This assessment measures the participant’s ability to suppress/inhibit motor responses. The subjects react to an arrow stimulus, by pressing either a left or right key depending on the position of the arrow. When an auditory stimulus occurs, the individual attempts to inhibit their motor response for the particular trial (Morein-Zamir & Sahakian, 2010). The measurement of the response inhibition is the stop-signal reaction time, a score that evaluates the time taken for the participant’s brain to suppress a response that would normally be performed.
-
– Cognitive flexibility: it is evaluated by the intra-/extra-dimensional set shifting test. This task assessed rule learning, reversal, and changing of attentional focus. The trial provides visual stimuli (white lines and colorful shapes) and feedback to the subjects in a way that they can learn an underlying “rule” regarding which stimulus is correct, based on trial and error. The underlining rule that controls what is “correct” and “incorrect” shifts several times and evaluates the individual’s capacity to respond with cognitive flexibility (Cambridge Cognition, 2015). The overall performance is assessed by adjusted total number of errors.
Statistical analysis
First, we conducted a one-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test to evaluate the distribution of the continuous variables. As all continuous variables showed a non-parametric dispersal, we evaluated the association between DOI and the clinical/neurocognitive variables using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient and Mann–Whitney U test, respectively, for continuous and categorical variables. To investigate which variables ultimately affected DOI, we performed multiple linear regressions. In this procedure, we introduced the clinical variables with level of significance (sig.) <.01. Additional controlled analyses assessing DOI and continuous variables were conducted using partial correlation coefficients.
Ethics
This research was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Chicago and University of Minnesota. The research procedures were described to the participants prior to providing consent. The investigators provided time for the subjects to ask questions. All individuals provided written informed consent for participation. The study procedures followed the rules of the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki, which describe ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects (World Medical Association, 2002).
Results
The mean and median DOI in our sample were 10.2 (±7.7) and 8.0 years, respectively. This variable showed a right-skewed distribution (see Figure 1).

Distribution of duration of illnessa in treatment-seeking subjects with gambling disorder (n = 448). aDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [current age] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.029

Distribution of duration of illnessa in treatment-seeking subjects with gambling disorder (n = 448). aDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [current age] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.029
Distribution of duration of illnessa in treatment-seeking subjects with gambling disorder (n = 448). aDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [current age] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]
Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.029
In terms of severity, the participants demonstrated a mean G-SAS total score of 34.6 (±12.5), therefore in the severe range (from 31 to 40 points) (Kim et al., 2009). Table 1 describes the main demographic and clinical variables in our sample.
Description of demographics and main clinical variables in treatment-seeking adults with gambling disorder (n = 448)
Variables | Mean (SDa)/median or % (n) |
---|---|
Demographics | |
Age | 47.6 (±11.3)/49.0 |
Gender | |
Male | 44.0 (197) |
Female | 56.0 (251) |
Marital status | |
With partner | 36.7 (173) |
Without partner | 63.3 (275) |
Educational level | |
Less than college | 36.7 (164) |
College or more | 63.3 (283) |
Ethnicity | |
Caucasian | 89.2 (397) |
Non-Caucasian | 10.8 (48) |
Clinical variables | |
Duration of illnessb | 10.2 (±7.7)/8.0 |
Age at first gambling | 28.1 (±13.2)/25.0 |
Age at onset of gambling disorder | 37.4 (±12.2)/37.0 |
Frequency of gambling (times a week) [Nc = 365] | 6.6 (±9.4)/2.5 |
G-SASd total score [N = 382] | 34.6 (±12.5)/33.0 |
PG-YBOCSe total score [N = 290] | 20.8 (±5.4)/20.0 |
Previous formalized treatmentf for gambling disorder [N = 365] | 18.6 (68) |
Previous gamblers anonymous treatment [N = 365] | 39.7 (145) |
Lifetime prevalence of any affective disorder | 27.2 (122) |
Lifetime prevalence of any anxiety disorder | 12.3 (55) |
Lifetime prevalence of alcohol use disorder | 23.2 (104) |
Lifetime prevalence of substance use disorder | 11.7 (52) |
Note. %: relative values; n: absolute values.
aStandard deviation. bDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [age at intake] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]. cNumber of valid subjects for the variable. If N is not displayed, the total sample (n = 448) was evaluated for the variable. dThe Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale (Kim et al., 2009). eYale–Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale modified for Pathological Gambling (Pallanti et al., 2005). fFormalized treatment: individual outpatient treatment and/or group outpatient treatment and/or inpatient treatment.
Regarding gambling behavior, we found a statistically significant negative correlation between DOI and lag between first gambling and onset of GD. A longer DOI was not associated with higher severity scores in G-SAS or PG-YBOCS. Table 2 displays the association between DOI and gambling behavior variables.
Association between gambling behavior and duration of illnessa (DOI) in treatment-seeking adults with gambling disorder (n = 448)
Gambling behavior variables | Correlation coefficientb or mean DOI (SDc) | p value |
---|---|---|
Lag between first gambling and onset of gambling disorder | −0.094 | .049 |
Gambling frequency (times a week) [Nd = 365] | 0.030 | .563 |
Primary form of gambling (strategic/non-strategic) | 10.8 (±8.1)/9.9 (±7.0) | .524 |
Percentage of income lost in gambling (last year) | −0.096 | .088 |
G-SASe total score [N = 382] | −0.082 | .108 |
PG-YBOCSf total score [N = 290] | 0.057 | .332 |
PG-YBOCS urge subscale [N = 290] | −0.019 | .745 |
PG-YBOCS gambling behavior subscale [N = 290] | 0.112 | .056 |
Note. p values with statistically significant differences (p < .05) are highlighted in bold.
aDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [age at intake] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]. bSpearman’s rank correlation coefficient. cStandard deviation. dNumber of valid subjects for the variable. If N is not displayed, the total sample (n = 448) was evaluated for the variable. eThe Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale (Kim et al., 2009). fYale–Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale modified for Pathological Gambling (Pallanti et al., 2005).
With respect to psychiatric antecedents and quality of life, a longer DOI was significantly associated with personal history of AUD and the presence of a first-degree relative with history of AUD. In addition, DOI presented a negative correlation with quality of life. These results are displayed in Table 3.
Association between duration of illnessa (DOI), psychiatric antecedents, quality of life, and neurocognitive variables in treatment-seeking subjects with gambling disorder (n = 448)
Variables | Mean DOI (SDb) or correlation coefficientc | p value |
---|---|---|
Psychiatric antecedents and quality of life | ||
Lifetime prevalence of any affective disorder (yes/no) | 10.2 (±7.5)/10.3 (±7.8) | .820d |
Depressive symptoms (Hamilton Depression Rating Scale) | −0.063 | .182 |
Lifetime prevalence of any anxiety disorder (yes/no) | 8.0 (±5.7)/10.5 (±7.9) | .059d |
Anxiety symptoms (Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale) | −0.050 | .327 |
Lifetime prevalence of alcohol use disorder (yes/no) | 11.9 (±8.4)/9.7 (±7.4) | .037 d |
Lifetime prevalence of substance use disorder (yes/no) | 10.5 (±7.6)/10.2 (±7.7) | .497d |
Lifetime prevalence of any impulse control disorder (yes/no) | 10.4 (±7.9)/10.2 (±7.7) | .942d |
Current smoking (yes/no) | 10.9 (±8.5)/9.5 (±6.7) | .161 |
Previous formalized treatmente for GD (yes/no) [Nf = 365] | 9.6 (±8.1)/10.1 (±7.9) | .665 |
Previous gamblers anonymous treatment [N = 365] | 10.3 (±8.6)/9.8 (±7.5) | .657 |
First-degree relative with history of GD (yes/no) | 10.9 (±7.9)/9.8 (±7.6) | .057 |
First-degree relative with history of AUD (yes/no) | 11.2 (±8.0)/8.9 (±7.1) | .001 |
Quality of life (QOLIg) [N = 134] | −0.170 | .049 |
Neurocognitive variables h | ||
Response inhibition (delay at the stop-signal task) [N = 77] | 0.100 | .388 |
Cognitive flexibility (intra-/extra-dimensional set shifting test) [N = 77] | −0.019 | .873 |
Note. p values with statistically significant differences (p < .05) are highlighted in bold.
aDuration of illness was defined as the difference between [age at intake] and [age at onset of gambling disorder]. bStandard deviation. cSpearman’s rank correlation coefficient. dp values adjusted for age. eFormalized treatment: individual outpatient treatment and/or group outpatient treatment and/or inpatient treatment. fNumber of valid subjects for the variable. If N is not displayed, the total sample (n = 448) was evaluated for the variable. gQuality of Life Inventory (Frisch et al., 1992). hThe following measures were used: [delay at the stop-signal task] = stop-signal reaction time (ms); [intra-/extra-dimensional set shifting test] = total errors (adjusted).
Finally, we conducted multiple linear regressions where we introduced the clinical variables with level of sig. <.05 in the univariate analysis. The variables inserted in this model were: [lag between first gambling and onset of GD], [lifetime prevalence of AUD], [first-degree relative with history of AUD], and [quality of life (QOLI)]. The model did not reach statistical significance. Final model = [QOLI] (B = −.077; 95% confidence interval for B = lower limit: −.165, upper limit: .010; constant: B = 13.069; sig. < .001); model summary: R = .154; R2 = .024; degrees of freedom = 4; sig. = .084. Strategy used = backward. Therefore, the multivariate approach was not more informative than the conclusions withdrawn from the univariate analysis.
Discussion
This research evaluated the association between DOI, clinical, and neurocognitive variables in a sample of 448 adult subjects with GD. This is, to our knowledge, the first study to investigate specifically DOI in GD. We assessed gambling behavior, psychiatric antecedents, quality of life, and neurocognitive variables. Our main results were: (a) there is a negative correlation between DOI and lag between first gambling and onset of GD; (b) lifetime history of AUD is associated with a longer duration of GD; (c) the presence of a first-degree relative with history of AUD is associated with a more extended course of GD; and (d) there is a negative correlation between DOI and quality of life.
DOI, GD severity, and financial losses
Contrary to our initial hypothesis, no associations were seen between DOI and GD severity in this study. Neither we were able to find significant correlations between DOI and financial losses (evaluated by percentage of income lost last year). It is important to notice that, although a longer DOI was not significantly associated with greater financial losses in the past year, more extended DOI tends to be correlated with higher lifetime losses. This is a result of recurrent losses and expenses in bets throughout the years. The money spent in gambling could be invested in healthier, self-fulfilling, and productive ways. Chronic monetary losses may also explain in part why subjects with long-duration GD report a lower quality of life (see more in DOI and Quality of Life subsection).
The absence of a cross-sectional correlation between DOI and GD severity in treatment-seeking/clinical trial samples may be due to a possible selection bias (i.e., individuals with chronic GD may be less prone to seek treatment or participate in clinical trials). Future studies assessing the association between duration of GD and severity of symptoms in non-clinical samples are needed.
DOI and lag between first gambling and GD
This study found a negative correlation between DOI and lag between first gambling and onset of GD. Previous studies have suggested that women tend to have a faster progression from recreational gambling to GD when compared with men (Grant, Odlaug, & Mooney, 2012; Ibáñez, Blanco, Moreryra, & Sáiz-Ruiz, 2003; Tavares et al., 2003). Therefore, gender differences in DOI may cause this discrepancy. However, this study did not find statistically significant differences between men and women in terms of DOI nor lag between first gambling and GD [mean DOI: men = 11.1 (±8.8) years, women = 9.6 (±6.7) years, sig. = .250; mean lag between first gambling and GD: men = 10.1 (±10.6) years, women = 8.8 (±9.7) years, sig. = .309). In this context, it is unlikely that the differences observed in the progression to GD in the current research is a consequence of gender-associated issues.
A faster progression from recreational gambling to GD appears to be associated with higher severity. For example, we found a negative correlation between lag from first gambling to GD and severity of gambling behavior in accordance with PG-YBOCS (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient = −0.155; sig. = .009) and G-SAS (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient = −0.245; sig. < .001). Furthermore, lag from first gambling to GD was positively correlated with QOLI (Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient = 0.301; sig. = .001).
These findings are consistent with what Blaszczynski and Nower (2002) describe as the “impulsive gambler.” Impulsive gamblers show more severe gambling levels, more impact in non-gambling-related areas, earlier onset, and unresponsiveness to treatment. Therefore, they are less likely to recover and more prone to present longer DOI. In the clinical setting, patients with longer DOI and faster progression from recreational gambling to GD may benefit from long-term customized psychotherapeutic interventions focused on impulse control (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2002).
Personal history of AUD and family history of AUD and GD
The subjects with lifetime AUD showed a significantly longer DOI when compared with those without the history of AUD. The family history of AUD was also correlated with a more extended DOI. In addition, the participants with family history of GD showed a trend to present longer DOI.
The association between longer DOI and AUD may possibly represent two underlying processes. The first element appears to be a common genetic vulnerability between GD and AUD. Shaffer et al. (1999) suggested that AUD was associated with an increased risk of developing GD. Accordingly, Slutske et al. (2000) investigated twins and found that there is a common genetic susceptibility for GD and AUD. Recent research has shown that GD and AUD addictions share significant similarities, such as continuous behavior despite relevant negative consequences, development of tolerance/withdrawal symptoms, presence of urges/craving, neurocognitive findings, and a chronic course (Bechara, 2001; Blanco et al., 2001; Potenza, 2001, 2008).
The second possible factor is a behavioral interaction between AUD and gambling. Alcohol use is associated with greater impulsivity (Dougherty, Marsh-Richard, Hatzis, Nouvion, & Mathias, 2008) and risky behaviors, such as risky sex, irresponsible driving, and gambling (Cooper, 2002; McCarthy, Niculete, Treloar, Morris, & Bartholow, 2012). Therefore, subjects who use alcohol may also be prone to gamble or bet more due to behavioral disinhibition when intoxicated. As a result, co-occurring AUD may facilitate relapse and lead to a more chronic course. In this context, assessing and managing AUD is an important step in the treatment of GD.
Proper treatment of AUD may reduce the DOI and avoid further negative consequences.
DOI and quality of life
As expected, this study observed a negative correlation between DOI and quality of life. GD is correlated with a wide range of negative consequences, such as legal, relational, financial, and occupational problems (Grant & Kim, 2005; Petry & Kiluk, 2002; Petry et al., 2005). As a result, individuals with chronic GD tend to suffer adverse impacts for a longer period and this in turn may ultimately affect the quality of life.
This is worrisome since disordered gamblers overall (regardless of DOI) tend to have a lower quality of life when compared with the general population (Black et al., 2003). In our sample, the mean [33.2 (±13.8)] and median (34.0) QOLI were in the very low range (QOLI scores of 37 or less) (Frisch, 2009). In this context, the chronic disordered gamblers tend to present an even lower quality of life.
Regarding treatment-seeking behavior, only 18.6% of the subjects in this study had sought formalized treatment specifically for GD. This percentage is consistent with the previous literature which suggests that the vast majority of individuals with gambling problems do not seek treatment (Slutske, 2006). Embarrassment and negation of the problem seem to be important explanatory factors (Suurvali, Cordingley, Hodgins, & Cunningham, 2009). Therefore, appropriate training of health professionals regarding GD, motivational interviewing, and stigma may increase the treatment-seeking behavior. As psychological and pharmacological interventions may provide significant benefits (Dell’Osso, Allen, & Hollander, 2005; Pallesen, Mitsem, Kvale, Johnsen, & Molde, 2005), strategies to increase adequate care of disordered gamblers may improve their quality of life.
Limitations and future studies
This study has significant strengths such as: (a) the investigation of an important and understudied clinical variable in the treatment of disordered gambling (DOI); (b) the use of a large sample (n = 483); and (c) a comprehensive evaluation including gold-standard measures on psychiatric symptoms, co-occurring disorders, overall functioning, and neurocognitive variables. However, this study presents some limitations. First, this is a cross-sectional research, and although it provides measures of association, causal conclusions cannot be inferred. It is important to replicate and further understand our findings in other study designs such as longitudinal studies. Second, this study used subjects who participated in clinical trials. Therefore, caution is needed when generalizing our findings to other patient populations. Third, some of the data in this research (such as [age of first gambling], [age at onset of GD], and [money lost last year]) were retrospectively evaluated. Consequently, it is possible that these assessments had some degree of recall bias. Despite the limitations, this study presents an innovative approach to GD and provides an important insight on a clinically relevant and understudied topic. Longitudinal research approaching clearer causal associations and the biological changes associated with different DOI is needed. It will be particularly interesting to investigate the possible biological changes associated with different DOI. It is important not only to further study treatment-seeking samples but also to expand research on DOI to non-treatment-seeking samples.
Conclusions
This study suggests that some important variables are associated with different DOI. DOI was negatively correlated with a lag from recreational gambling to GD (in years). In addition, the subjects with lifetime history of AUD and family history of AUD demonstrated a longer DOI. Finally, there was a negative correlation between DOI and quality of life. Increasing treatment-seeking behavior, providing customized psychological interventions, and effectively managing AUD may decrease the high levels of chronicity in GD. Furthermore, research on GD such as phenomenological studies and clinical trials may consider the duration of GD in their methodology. DOI might be an important variable when analyzing treatment outcome and avoiding confounders.
Authors’ contribution
We confirm that all persons designated as authors were qualified for authorship. Each author participated sufficiently in the work to take public responsibility for the content. The corresponding author affirms that he had access to all data from the study, both what is reported and what is unreported, and also that he had complete freedom to direct its analysis and its reporting, without influence from the sponsors. The corresponding author also affirms that there was no editorial direction or censorship from the sponsors.
GCM conducted the literature searches, the statistical analysis, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SAR and SRC made edits and amends to the first draft of the manuscript. JEG designed the study, wrote the protocol, supervised the literature searches and statistical analysis, and reviewed the final version of the paper. All authors contributed to and have approved the final version of the manuscript.
Conflict of interest
GCM and SAR have no conflict of interest to report. SRC consults for Cambridge Cognition. This research was supported by a Clinical Fellowship grant from the Welcome Trust to SRC (110049/Z/15/Z).
JEG has received research grants from American Foundation for Suicide Prevention, The TLC Foundation for Body Focused Repetitive Behaviors, Brainsway, and Takeda. He received yearly compensation from Springer Publishing for acting as Editor-in-Chief of the Journal of Gambling Studies and has received royalties from Oxford University Press, American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc., Norton Press, McGraw-Hill, and Johns Hopkins University Press.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Babor, T. F. , Dolinsky, Z. S. , Meyer, R. E. , Hesselbrock, M. , Hofmann, M. , & Tennen, H. (1992). Types of alcoholics: Concurrent and predictive validity of some common classification schemes. British Journal of Addiction, 87(10), 1415–1431. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1992.tb01921.x
Bechara, A. (2001). Neurobiology of decision-making: Risk and reward. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 6(3), 205–216. doi:10.1053/scnp.2001.22927
Beck, A. T. , & Steer, R. A. (1991). Relationship between the Beck Anxiety Inventory and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale with anxious outpatients. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 5(3), 213–223. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(91)90002-B
Black, D. W. , Moyer, T. , & Schlosser, S. (2003). Quality of life and family history in pathological gambling. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191(2), 124–126. doi:10.1097/01.NMD.0000050942.86352.47
Blanco, C. , Moreyra, P. , Nunes, E. V. , Saiz-Ruiz, J. , & Ibanez, A. (2001). Pathological gambling: Addiction or compulsion? Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 6(3), 167–176. doi:10.1053/scnp.2001.22921
Blaszczynski, A. , & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00015.x
Burge, A. N. , Pietrzak, R. H. , Molina, C. A. , & Petry, N. M. (2004). Age of gambling initiation and severity of gambling and health problems among older adult problem gamblers. Psychiatric Services, 55(12), 1437–1439. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.55.12.1437
Cambridge Cognition. (2015). Executive function test (CANTAB batteries). Retrieved from http://www.cambridgecognition.com/academic/cantabsuite/executive-function-tests
Carpenter, K. M. , & Hasin, D. S. (2001). Reliability and discriminant validity of the Type I/II and Type A/B alcoholic subtype classifications in untreated problem drinkers: A test of the Apollonian–Dionysian hypothesis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 63(1), 51–67. doi:10.1016/S0376-8716(00)00190-3
Cloninger, C. (1987). Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Science, 236(4800), 410–416. doi:10.1126/science.2882604
Cooper, M. L. (2002). Alcohol use and risky sexual behavior among college students and youth: Evaluating the evidence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Supplement (s14), 101–117. doi:10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.101
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain (pp. 195–201). New York, NY: Putnam Publishing.
Dell’Osso, B. , Allen, A. , & Hollander, E. (2005). Comorbidity issues in the pharmacological treatment of pathological gambling: A critical review. Clinical Practice and Epidemiology in Mental Health, 1(1), 21. doi:10.1186/1745-0179-1-21
Dougherty, D. M. , Marsh-Richard, D. M. , Hatzis, E. S. , Nouvion, S. O. , & Mathias, C. W. (2008). A test of alcohol dose effects on multiple behavioral measures of impulsivity. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 96(1 –2), 111–120. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.02.002
Frisch, M. B. (2009). Quality of Life Inventory handbook: A guide for laypersons, clients, and coaches. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson and Pearson Assessments.
Frisch, M. B. , Cornell, J. , Villanueva, M. , & Retzlaff, P. J. (1992). Clinical validation of the Quality of Life Inventory. A measure of life satisfaction for use in treatment planning and outcome assessment. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 92. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.92
Gerstein, D. , Hoffmann, J. P. , Larison, C. , Engelman, L. , Murphy, S. , Palmer, A. , & Hill, M. A. (1999). Gambling impact and behavior study: Report to the National Gambling Impact Study Commission. New York, NY: Christiansen/Cummings Associates. Retrieved from http://www.norc.org/pdfs/publications/gibsfinalreportapril1999.pdf
Grant, J. E. , & Kim, S. W. (2005). Quality of life in kleptomania and pathological gambling. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 46(1), 34–37. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.07.022
Grant, J. E. , Odlaug, B. L. , Chamberlain, S. R. , & Schreiber, L. R. (2012). Neurocognitive dysfunction in strategic and non-strategic gamblers. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 38(2), 336–340. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.006
Grant, J. E. , Odlaug, B. L. , & Mooney, M. E. (2012). Telescoping phenomenon in pathological gambling: Association with gender and comorbidities. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 200(11), 996–998. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182718a4d
Grant, J. E. , Steinberg, M. A. , Kim, S. W. , Rounsaville, B. J. , & Potenza, M. N. (2004). Preliminary validity and reliability testing of a structured clinical interview for pathological gambling. Psychiatry Research, 128(1), 79–88. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.006
Hamilton, M. (1969). Diagnosis and rating of anxiety. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 3(special issue), 76–79.
Ibáñez, A. , Blanco, C. , Moreryra, P. , & Sáiz-Ruiz, J. (2003). Gender differences in pathological gambling. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(3), 295–301. doi:10.4088/JCP.v64n0311
Kessler, R. C. , Hwang, I. , LaBrie, R. , Petukhova, M. , Sampson, N. A. , Winters, K. C. , & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine, 38(9), 1351–1360. doi:10.1017/S0033291708002900
Kim, S. W. , Grant, J. E. , Potenza, M. N. , Blanco, C. , & Hollander, E. (2009). The Gambling Symptom Assessment Scale (G-SAS): A reliability and validity study. Psychiatry Research, 166(1), 76–84. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.008
Kuhnen, C. M. , & Knutson, B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron, 47(5), 763–770. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.008
Ladd, G. T. , Molina, C. A. , Kerins, G. J. , & Petry, N. M. (2003). Gambling participation and problems among older adults. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 16(3), 172–177. doi:10.1177/0891988703255692
Laloyaux, J. , Michel, C. , Mourad, H. , Bertrand, H. , Domken, M. A. , Van der Linden, M. , & Larøi, F. (2012). Performance on an everyday life activity in persons diagnosed with alcohol dependency compared to healthy controls: Relations between a computerized shopping task and cognitive and clinical variables. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 47(3), 240–247. doi:10.1093/alcalc/ags014
Ledgerwood, D. M. , & Petry, N. M. (2006). Psychological experience of gambling and subtypes of pathological gamblers. Psychiatry Research, 144(1), 17–27. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.08.017
Ledgerwood, D. M. , Weinstock, J. , Morasco, B. J. , & Petry, N. M. (2007). Clinical features and treatment prognosis of pathological gamblers with and without recent gambling-related illegal behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 35(3), 294–301.
Lynch, W. J. , Maciejewski, P. K. , & Potenza, M. N. (2004). Psychiatric correlates of gambling in adolescents and young adults grouped by age at gambling onset. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(11), 1116–1122. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1116
Maier, W. , Buller, R. , Philipp, M. , & Heuser, I. (1988). The Hamilton Anxiety Scale: Reliability, validity and sensitivity to change in anxiety and depressive disorders. Journal of Affective Disorders, 14(1), 61–68. doi:10.1016/0165-0327(88)90072-9
Martinovic-Mitrovic, S. , Dickov, A. , Vuckovic, N. , Mitrovic, D. , Budisa, D. , & Vasic, D. (2008). Reaction time in relation to the duration of heroin abuse. European Psychiatry, 23, S314–S315. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.01.1082
Mathy, R. M. , & Cooper, A. (2003). The duration and frequency of Internet use in a nonclinical sample: Suicidality, behavioral problems, and treatment histories. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 40(1 – 2), 125–135. doi:10.1037/0033-3204.40.1-2.125
McCarthy, D. M. , Niculete, M. E. , Treloar, H. R. , Morris, D. H. , & Bartholow, B. D. (2012). Acute alcohol effects on impulsivity: Associations with drinking and driving behavior. Addiction, 107(12), 2109–2114. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03974.x
Medeiros, G. C. , Leppink, E. , Yaemi, A. , Mariani, M. , Tavares, H. , & Grant, J. (2015). Gambling disorder in older adults: A cross-cultural perspective. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 58, 116–121. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.12.021
Monterosso, J. R. , Aron, A. R. , Cordova, X. , Xu, J. , & London, E. D. (2005). Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 79(2), 273–277. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.02.002
Morein-Zamir, S. , & Sahakian, B. J. (2010). Stop-signal task. In I. P. Stolerman (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychopharmacology (p. 1285). Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Odlaug, B. L. , & Grant, J. E. (2010). Impulse-control disorders in a college sample: Results from the self-administered Minnesota Impulse Disorders Interview (MIDI). Primary Care Companion to the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 12(2), e1–e5. doi:10.4088/PCC.09m00842whi
Pallanti, S. , DeCaria, C. M. , Grant, J. E. , Urpe, M. , & Hollander, E. (2005). Reliability and validity of the pathological gambling adaptation of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (PG-YBOCS). Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(4), 431–443. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-5557-3
Pallesen, S. , Mitsem, M. , Kvale, G. , Johnsen, B. H. , & Molde, H. (2005). Outcome of psychological treatments of pathological gambling: A review and meta-analysis. Addiction, 100(10), 1412–1422. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01204.x
Petry, N. M. (2002). A comparison of young, middle-aged, and older adult treatment-seeking pathological gamblers. The Gerontologist, 42(1), 92–99. doi:10.1093/geront/42.1.92
Petry, N. M. , & Kiluk, B. D. (2002). Suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in treatment-seeking pathological gamblers. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(7), 462–469. doi:10.1097/00005053-200207000-00007
Petry, N. M. , Stinson, F. S. , & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(5), 564–574. doi:10.4088/JCP.v66n0504
Potenza, M. N. (2001). The neurobiology of pathological gambling. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 6(3), 217–226. doi:10.1053/scnp.2001.22929
Potenza, M. N. (2008). The neurobiology of pathological gambling and drug addiction: An overview and new findings. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 3181–3189. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0100
Schuckit, M. A. , Tipp, J. E. , Smith, T. L. , Shapiro, E. , Hesselbrock, V. M. , Bucholz, K. K. , Reich, T. , & Nurnberger, J. I. (1995). An evaluation of type A and B alcoholics. Addiction, 90(9), 1189–1203. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1995.tb01089.x
Shaffer, H. J. , Hall, M. N. , & Vander Bilt, J. (1999). Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: A research synthesis. American Journal of Public Health, 89(9), 1369–1376. doi:10.2105/AJPH.89.9.1369
Sheehan, D. V. , Lecrubier, Y. , Sheehan, K. H. , Amorim, P. , Janavs, J. , Weiller, E. , Hergueta, T. , Baker, R. , & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD 10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl . 20), 22–33; quiz 34–57.
Slutske, W. S. (2006). Natural recovery and treatment-seeking in pathological gambling: Results of two US national surveys. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(2), 297–302. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.2.297
Slutske, W. S. , Eisen, S. , True, W. R. , Lyons, M. J. , Goldberg, J. , & Tsuang, M. (2000). Common genetic vulnerability for pathological gambling and alcohol dependence in men. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(7), 666–673. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.7.666
Snaith, R. P. , Baugh, S. J. , Clayden, A. D. , Husain, A. , & Sipple, M. A. (1982). The Clinical Anxiety Scale: An instrument derived from the Hamilton Anxiety Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 141(5), 518–523. doi:10.1192/bjp.141.5.518
Suurvali, H. , Cordingley, J. , Hodgins, D. C. , & Cunningham, J. (2009). Barriers to seeking help for gambling problems: A review of the empirical literature. Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(3), 407–424. doi:10.1007/s10899-009-9129-9
Tavares, H. , Martins, S. S. , Lobo, D. S. , Silveira, C. M. , Gentil, V. , & Hodgins, D. C. (2003). Factors at play in faster progression for female pathological gamblers: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 64(4), 433–438. doi:10.4088/JCP.v64n0413
Tsurumi, K. , Kawada, R. , Yokoyama, N. , Sugihara, G. , Sawamoto, N. , Aso, T. , Fukuyama, H. , Murai, T. , & Takahashi, H. (2014). Insular activation during reward anticipation reflects duration of illness in abstinent pathological gamblers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1013. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01013
United States Census Bureau. (2016). U.S. and world population clock. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/popclock/ (accessed on August 27, 2016).
World Medical Association. (2002). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Journal of Postgraduate Medicine, 48(3), 206.
Xue, G. , Lu, Z. , Levin, I. P. , & Bechara, A. (2010). The impact of prior risk experiences on subsequent risky decision-making: The role of the insula. NeuroImage, 50(2), 709–716. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.097