Authors:
Dominic Sagoe Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Dominic Sagoe in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Rune Aune Mentzoni Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Rune Aune Mentzoni in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Tony Leino Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Tony Leino in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Helge Molde Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Helge Molde in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Sondre Haga Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Sondre Haga in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Mikjel Fredericson Gjernes Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Mikjel Fredericson Gjernes in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Daniel Hanss Department of Social and Cultural Sciences and Social Work, Darmstadt University of Applied Sciences, Darmstadt, Germany

Search for other papers by Daniel Hanss in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Ståle Pallesen Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway

Search for other papers by Ståle Pallesen in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

Although alcohol intake and gambling often co-occur in related venues, there is conflicting evidence regarding the effects of alcohol expectancy and intake on gambling behavior. We therefore conducted an experimental investigation of the effects of alcohol expectancy and intake on slot machine gambling behavior.

Methods

Participants were 184 (females = 94) individuals [age range: 18–40 (mean = 21.9) years] randomized to four independent conditions differing in information/expectancy about beverage (told they received either alcohol or placebo) and beverage intake [actually ingesting low (target blood alcohol concentration [BAC] < 0.40 mg/L) vs. moderate (target BAC > 0.40 mg/L; ≈0.80 mg/L) amounts of alcohol]. All participants completed self-report questionnaires assessing demographic variables, subjective intoxication, alcohol effects (stimulant and sedative), and gambling factors (behavior and problems, evaluation, and beliefs). Participants also gambled on a simulated slot machine.

Results

A significant main effect of beverage intake on subjective intoxication and alcohol effects was detected as expected. No significant main or interaction effects were detected for number of gambling sessions, bet size and variation, remaining credits at termination, reaction time, and game evaluation.

Conclusion

Alcohol expectancy and intake do not affect gambling persistence, dissipation of funds, reaction time, or gambling enjoyment.

Abstract

Background and aims

Although alcohol intake and gambling often co-occur in related venues, there is conflicting evidence regarding the effects of alcohol expectancy and intake on gambling behavior. We therefore conducted an experimental investigation of the effects of alcohol expectancy and intake on slot machine gambling behavior.

Methods

Participants were 184 (females = 94) individuals [age range: 18–40 (mean = 21.9) years] randomized to four independent conditions differing in information/expectancy about beverage (told they received either alcohol or placebo) and beverage intake [actually ingesting low (target blood alcohol concentration [BAC] < 0.40 mg/L) vs. moderate (target BAC > 0.40 mg/L; ≈0.80 mg/L) amounts of alcohol]. All participants completed self-report questionnaires assessing demographic variables, subjective intoxication, alcohol effects (stimulant and sedative), and gambling factors (behavior and problems, evaluation, and beliefs). Participants also gambled on a simulated slot machine.

Results

A significant main effect of beverage intake on subjective intoxication and alcohol effects was detected as expected. No significant main or interaction effects were detected for number of gambling sessions, bet size and variation, remaining credits at termination, reaction time, and game evaluation.

Conclusion

Alcohol expectancy and intake do not affect gambling persistence, dissipation of funds, reaction time, or gambling enjoyment.

Introduction

Gambling venues, such as casinos, clubs, and pubs, often provide the opportunity for simultaneous gambling and alcohol consumption, or gambling under the influence of alcohol. Ample evidence attests to the relationship between gambling and alcohol consumption problems. Several studies have, for example, shown a positive association between gambling problems and problems related to alcohol consumption (Barnes, Welte, Tidwell, & Hoffman, 2015; Chou & Afifi, 2011; Griffiths, 1994; Martins, Ghandour, Lee, & Storr, 2010) and results from recent US general population studies show that gambling problems and alcohol dependence are significantly related (Chou & Afifi, 2011) and have high co-occurrence or comorbidity (Barnes et al., 2015). Furthermore, the epidemiological literature suggests that about 73% of pathological gamblers report alcohol dependence over their lifetime (Petry, Stinson, & Grant, 2005).

In terms of how alcohol influences gambling behavior, the self-awareness model (Hull, 1981) proposes that alcohol diminishes people’s self-awareness by affecting their processes of encoding. This way, a person’s appreciation or self-evaluation of appropriate behavior decreases (Hull, Levenson, Young, & Sher, 1983). Consistent with this theory, alcohol consumption, even if moderate, has been found to negatively influence attention (Steele & Josephs, 1990; Zoethout, Delgado, Ippel, Dahan, & van Gerven, 2011), response inhibition, and flexibility (Easdon & Vogel-Sprott, 2000; Noël, Tomberg, Verbanck, & Campanella, 2010), as well as planning and organization abilities (Easdon & Vogel-Sprott, 2000; Zoethout et al., 2011). Hence, a potential cognitive distortion following inebriation from alcohol consumption could explain how alcohol affects gambling behavior.

Experimental studies have provided some further insights to the link between alcohol consumption and gambling behavior. A recent study on rat gambling suggests that frequent alcohol exposure has debilitating effects on decision-making during gambling task acquisition, and is linked to diminished ability to alter behavior due to feedback (Spoelder et al., 2015). In a recent comparison of pathological gamblers and a control group, evidence is provided that consumption of a moderately intoxicating dose of alcohol increases the rate of double-up betting (Ellery & Stewart, 2014). In another recent experimental study using video-lottery terminals, alcohol consumption strongly increased the propensity to gamble (Barrett, Collins, & Stewart, 2015). There is also evidence from a cross-sectional survey that males who drink alcohol when gambling gamble with increased bet size, seek extra money when at a casino, and more frequently incur higher losses than they can pay for (Giacopassi, Stitt, & Vandiver, 1998). Recent evidence from behavioral tracking data analysis also suggests that within-session gambling behavior is more variable in alcohol-serving venues than non-alcohol-serving venues (Leino et al., 2017).

However, some studies have produced contradictory results. Although alcohol consumption is associated with higher bets and faster loss of funds during slot machine gambling, neither main nor interaction effects were found for persistence (Cronce & Corbin, 2010). In another experimental study, no significant effect of alcohol consumption on gambling behavior was found in a comparison of a moderate dose of an alcohol group, a placebo group, and a no-alcohol control group (Breslin, Sobell, Cappell, Vakili, & Poulos, 1999). While the dissimilarities in findings may be attributable to differences in alcohol dosages administered in these studies, evidence regarding the effects of alcohol on gambling behavior is mixed and further studies are required to elucidate how alcohol affects gambling behavior (Ellery & Stewart, 2014).

The preponderance of previous studies compares alcohol and placebo groups on gambling-related variables. This design allows for examination of the effect of beverage (alcohol or placebo) intake on gambling behavior and related variables. However, most previous studies (Barrett et al., 2015; Breslin et al., 1999; Ellery, Stewart, & Loba, 2005; Giacopassi et al., 1998) with one exception (Ellery & Stewart, 2014) have not controlled for alcohol expectancy effects or the combined effects of alcohol intake and expectancy effects on gambling behavior. The absence or poor control of expectancy is a limitation with evidence that alcohol expectancy can alter perception (Bègue, Bushman, Zerhouni, Subra, & Ourabah, 2013). Relatedly, functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence indicates that alcohol expectancy and intoxication produce opposite results on the activation of neurons in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal areas (Gundersen, Specht, Grüner, Ersland, & Hugdahl, 2008). Moreover, to our knowledge, there is a dearth of knowledge on the combined effect of beverage intake (alcohol or placebo) and information (provided prior to beverage intake: alcohol or placebo) on gambling behavior.

Against this backdrop, the purpose of this experimental study was to simultaneously examine the effect of beverage intake [actually ingesting low (target blood alcohol concentration [BAC] < 0.40 mg/L) vs. moderate (target BAC > 0.40 mg/L; ≈0.80 mg/L) amounts of alcohol] and information/expectancy (alcohol or placebo) on gambling behavior. Specifically, we examined: (a) the main effect of information/expectancy (alcohol vs. placebo); (b) the main effect of beverage [low (target BAC < 0.40 mg/L) vs. moderate (target BAC > 0.40 mg/L; ≈0.80 mg/L) amounts of alcohol] intake; and (c) the interaction effect of information/expectancy and beverage intake on gambling behavior and gambling evaluation.

We hypothesized that compared with the low alcohol-ingesting groups, the moderate alcohol-ingesting groups will have higher perceived intoxication and slower reaction time. In addition, we predicted that in comparison with the low alcohol-ingesting groups, the moderate alcohol-ingesting groups will place more and larger bets, vary their bet size more, and have lower funds at termination. Furthermore, we expected the moderate alcohol-ingesting groups to indicate a more favorable gambling experience compared with the low alcohol-ingesting groups. Due to the dearth of a strong empirical foundation, we did not make predictions regarding the influence of alcohol expectancy on gambling behavior.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were 184 (females = 94) volunteers. Their ages ranged from 18 to 40 years. Other characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study sample by information and beverage condition

IA-GMA (n = 37) IA-GLA (n = 51) IP-GMA (n = 49) IP-GLA (n = 47) Overall (N = 184) Group comparison
Characteristics n n n n n χ2
Sex (female) 17 25 28 24 94 χ2(3) = 1.20, p = .754, Cramer’s V = 0.081
Educational level χ2(12) = 15.15, p = .233, Cramer’s V = 0.233
 Doctoral 0 0 2 0 2
 Master 10 9 14 15 48
 Bachelor 14 15 8 9 46
 Bachelor’s foundation 9 14 14 13 50
 Other 4 13 11 10 38
Marital status χ2(9) = 11.15, p = .266, Cramer’s V = 0.143
 Divorced 1 0 0 0 1
 Married 0 0 0 1 1
 Cohabitation 8 8 14 6 36
 Single 28 42 35 40 145
Living situation χ2(12) = 11.74, p = .467, Cramer’s V = 0.146
 Partner 7 7 9 6 29
 Parents 2 3 5 2 12
 Shared flat 21 30 25 33 109
 Alone 5 11 9 4 29
 Other 2 0 1 1 4
Gambling problems (PGSI) χ2(9) = 9.03, p = .435, Cramer’s V = 0.128
 Non-problem 23 34 30 31 118
 Low risk 8 14 15 15 52
 Moderate risk 5 3 4 1 13
 Problem 1 0 0 0 1
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) ANOVA
Age (years) 21.84 (3.69) 21.82 (2.70) 22.12 (2.91) 22.21 (3.64) 22.01 (3.20) Range: 18–40; F(3, 180) = 0.17, p = .914
Working hours (weekly) 5.05 (9.05) 4.71 (7.00) 6.06 (8.65) 5.36 (6.52) 5.30 (7.75) Range: 0–38; F(3, 180) = 0.27, p = .849
Gambling loss (past month; NOK) 36.49 (97.64) 44.61 (153.73) 40.51 (167.94) 28.72 (72.79) 37.83 (130.76) Range: 0–1,100; F(3, 180) = 0.13, p = .943
Days gambled (past month) 1.62 (5.47) 0.57 (1.19) 0.45 (1.19) 0.51 (1.56) 0.73 (2.73) Range: 0–30; F(3, 180) = 1.67, p = .176
Gambling beliefs (GBQ)
 Luck/perseverance 74.46 (12.63) 75.04 (11.53) 76.08 (13.63) 77.20 (11.89) 75.77 (12.38) Range: 19–91; F(3, 174) = 0.40, p = .756
 Illusion of control 40.41 (10.97) 41.25 (8.59) 41.06 (11.71) 40.36 (10.09) 40.80 (10.27) Range: 10–56; F(3, 180) = 0.09, p = .966
Blood alcohol content (mg/L) 0.76abd (0.19) 0.06abc (0.09) 0.71bcd (0.16) 0.07acd (0.09) 0.38 (0.35) Range: 0–1.15; F(3, 180) = 362.35, p = .000

Note. IA-GMA: Informed Alcohol-Given Moderate Alcohol; IA-GLA: Informed Alcohol-Given Low Alcohol; IP-GMA: Informed Placebo-Given Moderate Alcohol; IP-GLA: Informed Placebo-Given Low Alcohol; PGSI: Problem Gambling Severity Index; GBQ: Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire; NOK: Norwegian Kroner (NOK 1 ≈ US$ 0.2).

Values sharing a superscript on the same row are significantly different (p = .000).

Instruments/measures

A self-report questionnaire containing the following measures was used in the survey.

Demographics

Demographic information assessed in the survey questionnaire included age, gender, and relationship status.

Gambling problems

A subscale of the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (Ferris & Wynne, 2001), the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI), was used for assessment of problem gambling. The PGSI has a total of nine items regarding gambling problems and the negative effects of gambling. An example item is “Has your gambling caused any financial problems for you or your household?” All the nine items are answered on a 4-point rating scale ranging from “never” (0) to “almost always” (3). Based on the composite score, participants are assigned to one of the four categories: non-problem gambler (a composite score of 0), low-risk gambler (a composite score of 1 or 2), moderate-risk gambler (a composite score ranging between 3 and 7), and problem gambler (a composite score of 8 or higher). In this study, the PGSI yielded a Cronbach’s α of .73.

Gambling evaluation

The eight-item Bergen Evaluation of Games Scale (BEGS; Mentzoni, Laberg, Brunborg, Molde, & Pallesen, 2014) was administered to assess the degree to which participants find a game they had played enjoyable. An example item is “All in all, I enjoyed playing the game.” Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “completely disagree” (1) to “completely agree” (7). Two items are reverse-scored. We computed a total score by adding the scores on all individual items. In this study, the BEGS yielded a Cronbach’s α of .91.

Gambling beliefs

The Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire (GBQ; Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan, 2002) was used in the evaluation of the degree to which participants have cognitive distortions about gambling. This 21-item scale, comprising two subscales [luck/perseverance (13 items, e.g., “If I lose money gambling, I should try to win it back”) and illusion of control (eight items, e.g., “I have a ‘lucky’ technique that I use when I gamble”)], is answered on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly agree” (1) to “strongly disagree” (7). A composite score was computed for each subscale by adding the scores across corresponding items. In this study, Cronbach’s αs were .87 and .86 for luck/perseverance and illusion of control subscales, respectively.

Subjective intoxication

A Visual Analog Scale (VAS; Kushner et al., 1996) was used to assess participants’ degree of intoxication following drink (alcohol or placebo) consumption. Participants indicated their degree of intoxication by placing a mark on a 10-cm line [range: 0 (not at all) to 10 cm (extremely so)]. The distance from 0 mm to a participant’s mark represents his/her VAS score.

Alcohol effects

The Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale (BAES; Martin, Earleywine, Musty, Perrine, & Swift, 1993) was administered to assess participants’ experiences of the effects of alcohol intake. After beverage consumption, participants indicated the degree to which they feel a set of adjectives applied to them (e.g., elated and, down) on an 11-point scale ranging from “not at all” (0) to “extremely” (10). An index score was computed by adding participants’ scores on all adjectives. In this study, the BAES yielded a Cronbach’s α of .87.

BAC

Following the beverage intake, participants’ BAC was tested using a portable breath alcohol testing equipment: Alcotest 5510 (Dräger, Lübeck, Germany).

Slot simulator

We used the Fat Cat simulation software (Rain Games AS) developed for the University of Bergen’s gambling research purposes. The simulator consists of a video game displayed on a computer screen. It represents a simple slot machine with buttons that can be clicked to place bets, spin the wheel, and terminate a game. A spin lasts 2.8 s. Bets could be varied from 1 to 10 credits and could be reduced or increased by 1 credit increments by pressing a button. It has three parallel reels with a central horizontal payline. Above the reels, a win table is provided, displaying the respective wins for different combinations of matching symbols. Located on both sides of the reels are lights that illuminate as the reels are spinning and when a win is obtained. It also has background noise mimicking a gaming club or casino’s ambiance. The simulator illuminates and plays a lively melody with a win. These effects are absent with a loss. Figure 1 presents a screenshot of the simulator.


              Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Screen display for the Fat Cat slot simulator

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.031

Procedure and design

Participants were volunteers recruited primarily during academic lectures at the University of Bergen. The experiment was conducted in a laboratory located at the Faculty of Psychology. When prospective participants arrived in the laboratory, they first completed an informed consent form. Oral confirmation of fasting for about 2 hr prior to arrival in the laboratory was obtained. Females were offered a pregnancy test kit with private rooms for self-testing. Oral confirmation or evidence of a negative pregnancy test on the kit was a prerequisite for further participation in the study.

The amount of alcohol for each participant was calculated using the BAC calculator (Virginia Tech., VA, USA). We used a target BAC of 0.08 g% as the level of intoxication for the moderate alcohol-ingesting conditions as it reliably differentiates between alcohol and placebo effects (Corbin & Cronce, 2007; Fromme, Katz, & D’Amico, 1997). Participants were assigned unique serial numbers and randomized (using www.randomizer.org) to one of the four conditions.

The first condition comprises those informed that they were ingesting alcohol and were given a moderate amount of alcohol (Informed Alcohol-Given Moderate Alcohol, IA-GMA). Those informed that they were ingesting alcohol but were given a low amount of alcohol (Informed Alcohol-Given Low Alcohol, IA-GLA) comprised the second condition. Third was those informed that they were ingesting a placebo but were given a moderate amount of alcohol (Informed Placebo-Given Moderate Alcohol, IP-GMA). The final condition comprised those informed that they were ingesting a placebo but were given a low amount of alcohol (Informed Placebo-Given Low Alcohol, IP-GLA). They were then allocated laboratory cubicles and completed the first part of the self-report questionnaire that included demographic items, the GBQ, and the PGSI.

While participants completed the baseline questionnaires, drinks were mixed in a room outside the view of both participants and experimenters to blind the participants to the condition (Orne, 1962) and to control for experimenter bias (Rosenthal, 1967). The moderate alcohol beverage contained 45 ml of vodka and 135 ml of tonic water. The low alcohol beverage consisted of 20 ml of gin essence and 150 ml of tonic water. Accordingly, the recipe for the moderate alcohol beverage yielded a drink consisting of approximately 15 ml of alcohol, whereas the low alcohol beverage contained approximately 5 ml of alcohol intended to enhance placebo-information credibility.

After returning the completed questionnaires, participants were served their drinks in disposable cups. They were given a maximum of 20 min to drink their beverages. Each participant’s BAC was tested immediately after beverage intake prior to participating in the simulated gambling task. They were also tested 10 min after beverage intake, and at termination of the gambling task. This procedure resulted in an experimental setup with four different conditions, based on a 2 × 2 matrix reflecting two between-group factors, each with two levels. Figure 2 presents a framework of the experimental design.


            Figure 2.
Figure 2.

The recruitment and randomization process

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.031

Each laboratory cubicle had a computer with speakers for running the simulation. Each participant was given 200 credits and instructed to play the Fat Cat game for as long as he or she wanted. After playing the game for as many sessions as they wanted, the remaining game credit was registered. Participants then completed the second batch of instruments including the VAS, the BAES, and the BEGS. Debriefing then took place. Participants in the low alcohol-ingesting conditions (IA-GLA and IP-GLA) were informed about the deception and allowed to leave the laboratory.

Participants in the moderate alcohol-ingesting conditions (IA-GMA and IP-GMA) were also informed about the alcohol intake and asked to depart for home advisably using public transport. All participants were informed about their BAC at the debriefing. Prior to testing, participants were informed that they would get to maintain their winnings. After terminating the gambling session, they received Norwegian Kroner (NOK) 150 (≈US$ 17) as compensation for their contribution to the study in addition to their remaining credits in NOK.

Statistical analysis

We relied on BACs from the third measurement (at termination of the gambling task). Using a BAC cutoff score of 0.40 mg/L, six participants in the moderate alcohol conditions (IA-GMA and IP-GMA) displaying unusually low BACs (<0.40 mg/L) were excluded from the analysis. Descriptive statistics were used to determine sample characteristics. Chi-square analysis and one-way between-group analysis of variance (ANOVA) were conducted to examine between-group differences in sample characteristics. The effects of the experiment were analyzed using two-way between-group ANOVA. A previous study has shown an effect size (Cohen’s d; Cohen, 1988) for the effect of beverage on, for example, time spent playing of 0.75 (Breslin et al., 1999). Based on this, we estimated that a true effect of at least moderate size (d = 0.50) exists, implying a true effect of meaningful magnitude. Thus, an a priori power analysis was conducted based on the G*Power 3.1.7 (Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner, & Lang, 2009). Setting α to .05 (two-tailed), power to .80, and Cohen’s d to 0.50 (medium effect size), a minimum of 179 subjects in total were required to detect significant main and interaction effects. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 23.0 (IBM Inc., NY, USA) using a 2 (low vs. moderate alcohol) × 2 [true vs. false (information/expectancy)] design.

Ethics

The study procedures were conducted in line with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Western Norway provided ethical approval of the study (2013/119/REK).

RESULTS

Between-group pre-gambling comparison

The four groups did not differ on pre-gambling (demographic variables, subjective intoxication, gambling problems, and alcohol concentration) variables with the exception of BAC [F(3, 180) = 362.35; p = .000]. Expectedly, the moderate alcohol-ingesting groups (IA-GMA and IP-GMA) displayed higher BAC than the low alcohol-ingesting groups (IA-GLA and IP-GLA) (see Table 1).

Main effect of information/expectancy

There was no significant effect of information/expectancy on subjective intoxication, alcohol effects, number of gambling sessions, bet size, bet size variation, remaining credits, reaction time, and gambling evaluation (see Table 2). These results were robust adjusting for sex.

Table 2.

The effects of information, beverage intake, and their combined effect on study variables

IA-GMA (n = 37) IA-GLA (n = 51) IP-GMA (n = 49) IP-GLA (n = 47) Main effect Information Main effect Beverage Interaction effect Information × Beverage
Variable Range Mean

SD
Mean

SD
Mean

SD
Mean

SD
F(df = 1, 180) F(df = 1, 180) F(df = 1, 180)
Subjective intoxication (cm) 0–8.7 4.57 2.06 4.24 2.19 0.11a 59.70a,* 0.61a
2.05 1.95 1.97 1.96
Alcohol effects (BAES) 3–148 81.46 66.27 81.80 68.81 0.12 11.63* 0.07
26.06 28.40 28.34 27.90
Sessions 2–167 67.38 66.63 75.33 71.55 1.11 0.14 0.06
41.20 37.72 43.66 42.20
Average bet size 0.50–26.86 4.39 3.71 3.46 3.89 0.71 0.08 1.62
4.61 1.91 2.57 2.68
Bet size variation 0–77 16.00 17.20 16.69 17.55 0.04 0.17 0.01
16.84 17.00 15.60 17.93
Remaining credits 0–350 122.38 101.55 99.80 110.23 0.33 0.19 1.68
86.39 82.72 84.10 72.04
Average reaction time (s) 1.43–166.72 9.73 9.25 12.85 8.03 0.12 0.91 0.61
19.83 13.32 27.31 9.52
Gambling evaluation (BEGS) 8–56 29.97 29.16 29.08 28.87 0.11 0.08 0.03
13.33 11.10 12.32 12.28

Note. IA-GMA: Informed Alcohol-Given Moderate Alcohol; IA-GLA: Informed Alcohol-Given Low Alcohol; IP-GMA: Informed Placebo-Given Moderate Alcohol; IP-GLA: Informed Placebo-Given Low Alcohol; BAES: Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale; BEGS: Bergen Evaluation of Games Scale.

df = 1, 178.

p < .001.

Main effect of beverage intake

A significant effect of beverage intake was detected for subjective intoxication [F(1, 178) = 59.70; p < .001] as well as alcohol effects [F(1, 180) = 11.63; p < .001] as expected. There were no significant effects of beverage intake on the other study variables (see Table 2). The above results were robust controlling for sex.

Interaction effect of information/expectancy and beverage intake

There were no significant interaction effects of information/expectancy and beverage intake on the dependent variables (see Table 2). These results proved robust with adjustment for sex.

DISCUSSION

We conducted an experimental investigation of the effect of beverage intake (low or moderate alcohol) and information/expectancy (alcohol or placebo) on gambling behavior. The moderate alcohol-ingesting groups’ display of higher BAC compared with the low alcohol-ingesting groups, as well as the significant main effect of beverage intake on perceived intoxication and alcohol effects are consistent with previous findings (Breslin et al., 1999; Ellery & Stewart, 2014; Gundersen et al., 2008). In addition, the absence of main or interaction effects for gambling persistence, remaining credits at termination, reaction time, and gambling evaluation suggests that alcohol expectancy and intake do not influence the above factors. These results are in contrast to findings from some previous studies (Barrett et al., 2015; Ellery & Stewart, 2014; Ellery et al., 2005; Giacopassi et al., 1998), but in line with findings from others (Breslin et al., 1999; Cronce & Corbin, 2010; Ellery & Stewart, 2014).

Several factors may account for the incongruent findings. First, we compare moderate alcohol intake with low intake rather than a placebo. Theoretically, it is possible that even low alcohol intake affects gambling behavior and that the important behavioral difference exists between moderate alcohol vs. placebo intake, rather than moderate vs. low alcohol intake as observed in this study. Similarly, whereas we used a BAC of 0.08 g% for the moderate alcohol-ingesting conditions, other studies have used differing doses (e.g., 0.06 g%) (Ellery & Stewart, 2014; Ellery et al., 2005). The differences in alcohol dosages administered in previous studies may account for the dissimilarities in findings. Similarly, the paucity, or differences in levels, of expectancy control may also account for the dissimilarities in findings (Ellery & Stewart, 2014) as alcohol expectancy and intoxication produce differing behavioral (Bègue et al., 2013) and neuronal effects (Gundersen et al., 2008).

Between-study differences in sample characteristics may also account for the varying results. Whereas our sample of volunteers comprised novice gamblers (mainly students), other studies have included regular gamblers and alcohol drinkers (Cronce & Corbin 2010), and pathological gamblers (Ellery & Stewart, 2014; Ellery et al., 2005). Again, it is theoretically possible that in student samples, a higher BAC is required to observe any meaningful effect of alcohol intake on gambling behavior. Furthermore, differences in the structural characteristics of simulated gambling tasks, such as the number of paylines (Dixon et al., 2014; Haw, 2008; Livingstone, Woolley, Zazryn, Bakacs, & Shami, 2008), are also a plausible explanation for the dissimilarities in findings.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

As far as we are aware, this is the first study to examine the effects of alcohol expectancy and use on gambling behavior among novice gamblers. Nonetheless, this study is not precluded from ecological challenges associated with laboratory studies in general and gambling studies in particular, such as the laboratory cubicle-casino ambiance variance and the absence of direct or personal monetary risk or loss (Gainsbury & Blaszczynski, 2011; Lyons, 2006). To enhance ecological validity, future studies in natural gambling environments are recommended. In addition, to reduce the possibility of participant and experimenter bias, we did not include an additional control task or manipulation check (Kidd, 1976). Future studies are encouraged to include other control tasks or manipulation checks to ascertain blindness. Moreover, uniformity of experimental equipment (e.g., simulators) in future studies may facilitate the comparison of experimental findings. Finally, further investigations in this area using diverse methods and samples may provide further elucidation.

CONCLUSION

There is conflicting evidence on the influence of alcohol expectancy and intake on gambling behavior. Our findings show that the expectancy and intake of low or moderate amounts of alcohol do not affect gambling persistence, dissipation of funds, reaction time, and gambling evaluation. Differences between this study and previous ones, such as control for alcohol expectancy, alcohol dosages administered, experimental equipment, and sample type may account for the dissimilarities in findings. Further investigations using diverse methods and samples may provide further elucidation.

Authors’ contribution

SP, RAM, DH, TL, and DS contributed to study concept and design. SP obtained funding. SH and MFG contributed to data collection. DS, TL, and SP contributed to statistical analysis and interpretation of data. SP conducted study supervision. DS, SP, RAM, TL, HM, SH, MFG, and DH contributed to writing.

All authors had full access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • Barnes, G. M. , Welte, J. W. , Tidwell, M. C. O. , & Hoffman, J. H. (2015). Gambling and substance use: Co-occurrence among adults in a recent general population study in the United States. International Gambling Studies, 15(1), 5571. doi:10.1080/14459795.2014.990396

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barrett, S. P. , Collins, P. , & Stewart, S. H. (2015). The acute effects of tobacco smoking and alcohol consumption on video-lottery terminal gambling. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 130, 3439. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2014.12.015

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bègue, L. , Bushman, B. J. , Zerhouni, O. , Subra, B. , & Ourabah, M. (2013). ‘Beauty is in the eye of the beer holder’: People who think they are drunk also think they are attractive. British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 225234. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2012.02114.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Breslin, F. C. , Sobell, M. B. , Cappell, H. , Vakili, S. , & Poulos, C. X. (1999). The effects of alcohol, gender, and sensation seeking on the gambling choices of social drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13(3), 243252. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.13.3.243

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chou, K. L. , & Afifi, T. O. (2011). Disordered (pathologic or problem) gambling and axis I psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. American Journal of Epidemiology, 173(11), 12891297. doi:10.1093/aje/kwr017

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Corbin, W. R. , & Cronce, J. M. (2007). Alcohol effects on behavioral control: The impact of likelihood and magnitude of negative consequences. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(6), 955964. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00389.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cronce, J. M. , & Corbin, W. R. (2010). Effects of alcohol and initial gambling outcomes on within-session gambling behavior. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(2), 145157. doi:10.1037/a0019114

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dixon, M. J. , Graydon, C. , Harrigan, K. A. , Wojtowicz, L. , Siu, V. , & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). The allure of multi-line games in modern slot machines. Addiction, 109(11), 19201928. doi:10.1111/add.12675

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Easdon, C. M. , & Vogel-Sprott, M. (2000). Alcohol and behavioral control: Impaired response inhibition and flexibility in social drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8(3), 387394. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.8.3.387

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellery, M. , & Stewart, S. H. (2014). Alcohol affects video lottery terminal (VLT) gambling behaviors and cognitions differently. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 206216. doi:10.1037/a0035235

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellery, M. , Stewart, S. H. , & Loba, P. (2005). Alcohol’s effects on video lottery terminal (VLT) play among probable pathological and nonpathological gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(3), 299324. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-3101-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faul, F. , Erdfelder, E. , Buchner, A. , & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 11491160. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. , & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Fromme, K. , Katz, E. , & D’Amico, E. (1997). Effects of alcohol intoxication on the perceived consequences of risk taking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(1), 1423. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.5.1.14

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gainsbury, S. , & Blaszczynski, A. (2011). The appropriateness of using laboratories and student participants in gambling research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(1), 8397. doi:10.1007/s10899-010-9190-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Giacopassi, D. , Stitt, B. G. , & Vandiver, M. (1998). An analysis of the relationship of alcohol to casino gambling among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(2), 135149. doi:10.1023/A:1023094725055

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. (1994). An exploratory study of gambling cross addictions. Journal of Gambling Studies, 10(4), 371384. doi:10.1007/BF02104903

  • Gundersen, H. , Specht, K. , Grüner, R. , Ersland, L. , & Hugdahl, K. (2008). Separating the effects of alcohol and expectancy on brain activation: An fMRI working memory study. NeuroImage, 42(4), 15871596. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.037

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Haw, J. (2008). Random-ratio schedules of reinforcement: The role of early wins and unreinforced trials. Journal of Gambling Issues, 21, 5667. doi:10.4309/jgi.2008.21.6

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90(6), 586600. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.90.6.586

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. , Levenson, R. W. , Young, R. D. , & Sher, K. J. (1983). Self-awareness-reducing effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(3), 461473. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.3.461

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kidd, R. F. (1976). Manipulation checks: Advantage or disadvantage? Representative Research in Social Psychology, 7, 160165.

  • Kushner, M. G. , Mackenzie, T. B. , Fiszdon, J. , Valentiner, D. , Foa, E. , Anderson, N. , & Wangensteen, D. (1996). The effects of alcohol consumption on laboratory-induced panic and state anxiety. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(3), 264270. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830030086013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leino, T. , Molde, H. , Griffiths, M. D. , Mentzoni, R. A. , Sagoe, D. , & Pallesen, S. (2017). Gambling behavior in alcohol-serving and non-alcohol-serving-venues: A study of electronic gaming machine players using account records. Addiction Research & Theory, 25(3), 201207. doi:10.1080/16066359.2017.1288806

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Livingstone, C. , Woolley, R. , Zazryn, T. , Bakacs, L. , & Shami, R. (2008). The relevance and role of gaming machine games and game features on the play of problem gamblers. Adelaide, SA, Australia: Independent Gambling Authority of South Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lyons, C. A. (2006). Methodological considerations in the experimental analysis of gambling. In P. M. Ghezzi, C. A. Lyons, M. R. Dixon, & G. R. Wilson (Eds.), Gambling: Behavior theory, research, and application (pp. 91104). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Martin, C. S. , Earleywine, M. , Musty, R. E. , Perrine, M. W. , & Swift, R. M. (1993). Development and validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 17(1), 140146. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb00739.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Martins, S. S. , Ghandour, L. A. , Lee, G. P. , & Storr, C. L. (2010). Sociodemographic and substance use correlates of gambling behavior in the Canadian general population. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29(3), 338351. doi:10.1080/10550887.2010.489447

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mentzoni, R. , Laberg, J. , Brunborg, G. , Molde, H. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). Type of musical soundtrack affects behavior in gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3(2), 102106. doi:10.1556/JBA.3.2014.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Noël, X. , Tomberg, C. , Verbanck, P. , & Campanella, S. (2010). The influence of alcohol ingestion on cognitive response inhibition and error processing. Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(4), 253258. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000039

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17(11), 776783. doi:10.1037/h0043424

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M. , Stinson, F. S. , & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(5), 564574. doi:10.4088/JCP.v66n0504

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rosenthal, R. (1967). Covert communication in the psychological experiment. Psychological Bulletin, 67(5), 356367. doi:10.1037/h0024529

  • Spoelder, M. , Lesscher, H. M. , Hesseling, P. , Baars, A. M. , Lozeman-van’t Klooster, J. G., Mijnsbergen, R., & Vanderschuren, L. J. (2015). Altered performance in a rat gambling task after acute and repeated alcohol exposure. Psychopharmacology, 232(19), 36493662. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4020-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steele, C. M. , & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45(8), 921933. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.921

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zoethout, R. W. , Delgado, W. L. , Ippel, A. E. , Dahan, A. , & van Gerven, J. (2011). Functional biomarkers for the acute effects of alcohol on the central nervous system in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 71(3), 331350. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.2010.03846.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barnes, G. M. , Welte, J. W. , Tidwell, M. C. O. , & Hoffman, J. H. (2015). Gambling and substance use: Co-occurrence among adults in a recent general population study in the United States. International Gambling Studies, 15(1), 5571. doi:10.1080/14459795.2014.990396

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barrett, S. P. , Collins, P. , & Stewart, S. H. (2015). The acute effects of tobacco smoking and alcohol consumption on video-lottery terminal gambling. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 130, 3439. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2014.12.015

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bègue, L. , Bushman, B. J. , Zerhouni, O. , Subra, B. , & Ourabah, M. (2013). ‘Beauty is in the eye of the beer holder’: People who think they are drunk also think they are attractive. British Journal of Psychology, 104(2), 225234. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2012.02114.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Breslin, F. C. , Sobell, M. B. , Cappell, H. , Vakili, S. , & Poulos, C. X. (1999). The effects of alcohol, gender, and sensation seeking on the gambling choices of social drinkers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 13(3), 243252. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.13.3.243

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chou, K. L. , & Afifi, T. O. (2011). Disordered (pathologic or problem) gambling and axis I psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. American Journal of Epidemiology, 173(11), 12891297. doi:10.1093/aje/kwr017

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Corbin, W. R. , & Cronce, J. M. (2007). Alcohol effects on behavioral control: The impact of likelihood and magnitude of negative consequences. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31(6), 955964. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00389.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cronce, J. M. , & Corbin, W. R. (2010). Effects of alcohol and initial gambling outcomes on within-session gambling behavior. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(2), 145157. doi:10.1037/a0019114

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dixon, M. J. , Graydon, C. , Harrigan, K. A. , Wojtowicz, L. , Siu, V. , & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). The allure of multi-line games in modern slot machines. Addiction, 109(11), 19201928. doi:10.1111/add.12675

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Easdon, C. M. , & Vogel-Sprott, M. (2000). Alcohol and behavioral control: Impaired response inhibition and flexibility in social drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8(3), 387394. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.8.3.387

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellery, M. , & Stewart, S. H. (2014). Alcohol affects video lottery terminal (VLT) gambling behaviors and cognitions differently. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 206216. doi:10.1037/a0035235

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellery, M. , Stewart, S. H. , & Loba, P. (2005). Alcohol’s effects on video lottery terminal (VLT) play among probable pathological and nonpathological gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 21(3), 299324. doi:10.1007/s10899-005-3101-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faul, F. , Erdfelder, E. , Buchner, A. , & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 11491160. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. , & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

  • Fromme, K. , Katz, E. , & D’Amico, E. (1997). Effects of alcohol intoxication on the perceived consequences of risk taking. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 5(1), 1423. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.5.1.14

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gainsbury, S. , & Blaszczynski, A. (2011). The appropriateness of using laboratories and student participants in gambling research. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(1), 8397. doi:10.1007/s10899-010-9190-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Giacopassi, D. , Stitt, B. G. , & Vandiver, M. (1998). An analysis of the relationship of alcohol to casino gambling among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(2), 135149. doi:10.1023/A:1023094725055

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. (1994). An exploratory study of gambling cross addictions. Journal of Gambling Studies, 10(4), 371384. doi:10.1007/BF02104903

  • Gundersen, H. , Specht, K. , Grüner, R. , Ersland, L. , & Hugdahl, K. (2008). Separating the effects of alcohol and expectancy on brain activation: An fMRI working memory study. NeuroImage, 42(4), 15871596. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.037

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Haw, J. (2008). Random-ratio schedules of reinforcement: The role of early wins and unreinforced trials. Journal of Gambling Issues, 21, 5667. doi:10.4309/jgi.2008.21.6

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes and effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90(6), 586600. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.90.6.586

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. , Levenson, R. W. , Young, R. D. , & Sher, K. J. (1983). Self-awareness-reducing effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(3), 461473. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.3.461

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kidd, R. F. (1976). Manipulation checks: Advantage or disadvantage? Representative Research in Social Psychology, 7, 160165.

  • Kushner, M. G. , Mackenzie, T. B. , Fiszdon, J. , Valentiner, D. , Foa, E. , Anderson, N. , & Wangensteen, D. (1996). The effects of alcohol consumption on laboratory-induced panic and state anxiety. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(3), 264270. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830030086013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leino, T. , Molde, H. , Griffiths, M. D. , Mentzoni, R. A. , Sagoe, D. , & Pallesen, S. (2017). Gambling behavior in alcohol-serving and non-alcohol-serving-venues: A study of electronic gaming machine players using account records. Addiction Research & Theory, 25(3), 201207. doi:10.1080/16066359.2017.1288806

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Livingstone, C. , Woolley, R. , Zazryn, T. , Bakacs, L. , & Shami, R. (2008). The relevance and role of gaming machine games and game features on the play of problem gamblers. Adelaide, SA, Australia: Independent Gambling Authority of South Australia.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lyons, C. A. (2006). Methodological considerations in the experimental analysis of gambling. In P. M. Ghezzi, C. A. Lyons, M. R. Dixon, & G. R. Wilson (Eds.), Gambling: Behavior theory, research, and application (pp. 91104). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Martin, C. S. , Earleywine, M. , Musty, R. E. , Perrine, M. W. , & Swift, R. M. (1993). Development and validation of the Biphasic Alcohol Effects Scale. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 17(1), 140146. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb00739.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Martins, S. S. , Ghandour, L. A. , Lee, G. P. , & Storr, C. L. (2010). Sociodemographic and substance use correlates of gambling behavior in the Canadian general population. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 29(3), 338351. doi:10.1080/10550887.2010.489447

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mentzoni, R. , Laberg, J. , Brunborg, G. , Molde, H. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). Type of musical soundtrack affects behavior in gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3(2), 102106. doi:10.1556/JBA.3.2014.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Noël, X. , Tomberg, C. , Verbanck, P. , & Campanella, S. (2010). The influence of alcohol ingestion on cognitive response inhibition and error processing. Journal of Psychophysiology, 24(4), 253258. doi:10.1027/0269-8803/a000039

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17(11), 776783. doi:10.1037/h0043424

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M. , Stinson, F. S. , & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 66(5), 564574. doi:10.4088/JCP.v66n0504

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rosenthal, R. (1967). Covert communication in the psychological experiment. Psychological Bulletin, 67(5), 356367. doi:10.1037/h0024529

  • Spoelder, M. , Lesscher, H. M. , Hesseling, P. , Baars, A. M. , Lozeman-van’t Klooster, J. G., Mijnsbergen, R., & Vanderschuren, L. J. (2015). Altered performance in a rat gambling task after acute and repeated alcohol exposure. Psychopharmacology, 232(19), 36493662. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4020-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steele, C. M. , & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45(8), 921933. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.921

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zoethout, R. W. , Delgado, W. L. , Ippel, A. E. , Dahan, A. , & van Gerven, J. (2011). Functional biomarkers for the acute effects of alcohol on the central nervous system in healthy volunteers. British Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 71(3), 331350. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.2010.03846.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jul 2024 0 34 5
Aug 2024 0 39 10
Sep 2024 0 28 10
Oct 2024 0 177 8
Nov 2024 0 119 12
Dec 2024 0 61 6
Jan 2025 0 30 6