Authors:
William Van Gordon Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby, Derby, Derbyshire, UK
Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, Nottinghamshire, UK

Search for other papers by William Van Gordon in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Edo Shonin Awake to Wisdom Centre for Meditation and Mindfulness Research, Ragusa, Italy

Search for other papers by Edo Shonin in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Thomas J. Dunn Psychology Division, Bishop Grosseteste University, Lincoln, UK

Search for other papers by Thomas J. Dunn in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Javier Garcia-Campayo Miguel Servet University Hospital, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain

Search for other papers by Javier Garcia-Campayo in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Marcelo M. P. Demarzo Department of Preventive Medicine, Federal University of Sao Paulo (UNIFESP), “Mente Aberta” – Brazilian Centre for Mindfulness and Health Promotion, Sao Paulo, Brazil
Hospital Israelita Albert Einstein, Sao Paulo, Brazil

Search for other papers by Marcelo M. P. Demarzo in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Mark D. Griffiths Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, Nottinghamshire, UK

Search for other papers by Mark D. Griffiths in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

Workaholism is a form of behavioral addiction that can lead to reduced life and job satisfaction, anxiety, depression, burnout, work–family conflict, and impaired productivity. Given the number of people affected, there is a need for more targeted workaholism treatments. Findings from previous case studies successfully utilizing second-generation mindfulness-based interventions (SG-MBIs) for treating behavioral addiction suggest that SG-MBIs may be suitable for treating workaholism. This study conducted a controlled trial to investigate the effects of an SG-MBI known as meditation awareness training (MAT) on workaholism.

Methods

Male and female adults suffering from workaholism (n = 73) were allocated to MAT or a waiting-list control group. Assessments were performed at pre-, post-, and 3-month follow-up phases.

Results

MAT participants demonstrated significant and sustained improvements over control-group participants in workaholism symptomatology, job satisfaction, work engagement, work duration, and psychological distress. Furthermore, compared to the control group, MAT participants demonstrated a significant reduction in hours spent working but without a decline in job performance.

Discussion and conclusions

MAT may be a suitable intervention for treating workaholism. Further controlled intervention studies investigating the effects of SG-MBIs on workaholism are warranted.

Abstract

Background and aims

Workaholism is a form of behavioral addiction that can lead to reduced life and job satisfaction, anxiety, depression, burnout, work–family conflict, and impaired productivity. Given the number of people affected, there is a need for more targeted workaholism treatments. Findings from previous case studies successfully utilizing second-generation mindfulness-based interventions (SG-MBIs) for treating behavioral addiction suggest that SG-MBIs may be suitable for treating workaholism. This study conducted a controlled trial to investigate the effects of an SG-MBI known as meditation awareness training (MAT) on workaholism.

Methods

Male and female adults suffering from workaholism (n = 73) were allocated to MAT or a waiting-list control group. Assessments were performed at pre-, post-, and 3-month follow-up phases.

Results

MAT participants demonstrated significant and sustained improvements over control-group participants in workaholism symptomatology, job satisfaction, work engagement, work duration, and psychological distress. Furthermore, compared to the control group, MAT participants demonstrated a significant reduction in hours spent working but without a decline in job performance.

Discussion and conclusions

MAT may be a suitable intervention for treating workaholism. Further controlled intervention studies investigating the effects of SG-MBIs on workaholism are warranted.

Introduction

Workaholism has been defined as “being overly concerned about work, driven by an uncontrollable work motivation, and to investing so much time and effort to work that it impairs other important life areas” (Andreassen, Hetland, & Pallesen, 2014, p. 8). Prevalence rates for workaholism in Western counties are typically in the order of 8%–10% (Sussman, Lisha, & Griffiths, 2011). However, only one study (i.e., Andreassen, Griffiths, et al., 2014) has ever carried out a nationally representative survey of workaholism, and it reported that 8.3% of Norwegian adults were addicted to work. Despite this relatively high figure, there is a concern that prevalence rates could increase even further with the proliferation of technology-driven modern working styles (e.g., use of laptops, tablets, and smartphones) that blur the work–leisure boundary (Andreassen, Griffiths, Sinha, Hetland, & Pallesen, 2016; Quinones, Griffiths, & Kakabadse, 2016). Workaholism has been empirically demonstrated to be a distinct construct compared with enthusiastic working and work engagement (Schaufeli, Taris, & van Rhenen, 2008). Indeed, whereas enthusiastic and engaged working styles are associated with increases in life satisfaction and job performance, workaholism is associated with stress and incivility (e.g., Lanzo, Aziz, & Wuensch, 2016), anxiety and depression (e.g., Andreassen et al., 2016), and reduced life and job satisfaction (e.g., Karanika-Murray, Pontes, Griffiths & Biron, 2015; Shimazu, Schaufeli, Kamiyama, & Kawakami, 2015). Workaholism can also lead to burnout, work–family conflict, and impaired productivity (Griffiths & Karanika-Murray, 2012; Sussman, 2012).

Workaholism is a form of behavioral addiction, of which other examples include addictions to gambling, video gaming, shopping, exercise, and sex (Demetrovics & Griffiths, 2012). According to Griffiths’ (2005a) components model of addiction, an individual is deemed to suffer from a behavioral addiction when concerning the object of their addiction, they satisfy six criteria. In relation to work, this would be: (a) salience (i.e., work is the single most important activity in their life), (b) mood modification (i.e., work is used to alleviate emotional stress and/or to engender euphoric or arousing states), (c) tolerance (i.e., needing to work longer hours or at greater intensity to derive the same mood-modifying effects), (d) withdrawal (i.e., suffering emotional and/or physical distress when not being able to work), (e) conflict (i.e., interpersonal conflict with family members and other individuals, conflict with non-work activities, such as socializing and exercising, and intrapsychic conflict), and (f) relapse (i.e., reverting to earlier patterns of excessive working following periods of being in control) (Griffiths, 2005b).

Despite the significant health and economic burden imposed by workaholism, there are very few peer-reviewed studies examining its treatment (Shonin, Van Gordon, & Griffiths, 2014c). Consequently, guidelines for treating workaholism are largely based on theoretical proposals and/or anecdotal clinical reports that are unsupported by empirical evidence. Consistent with the need for more targeted treatments, there has been growing interest in the use of mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) for treating behavioral addiction, including workaholism (Shonin, Van Gordon, & Griffiths, 2014b). Mindfulness is a form of meditation that derives from Buddhist practice and can be defined as “the process of engaging a full, direct, and active awareness of experienced phenomena that is (i) spiritual in aspect and, (ii) maintained from one moment to the next” (Van Gordon, Shonin, & Griffiths, 2015, p. 592). A handful of intervention studies have been conducted indicating that mindfulness has applications for treating gambling addiction (see reviews by Griffiths, Shonin, & Van Gordon, 2016; Maynard, Wilson, Labuzienski, & Whiting, 2015; Shonin, Van Gordon, & Griffiths, 2013, 2014a). In addition, two separate clinical case studies (each incorporating a quantitative data-assessment component) have demonstrated that mindfulness can lead to clinically significant positive change (maintained through 6-month follow-up) in sex addiction (Van Gordon, Shonin, & Griffiths, 2016c) and workaholism (Shonin et al., 2014c).

The latter case study involved a director of a blue-chip company working more than 65 hr a week and who presented with complaints of (a) work-related sensation-seeking (e.g., obsessing over winning high-value contracts), (b) non-restorative sleep, (c) frequent migraines, (d) irritability and incivility when not working, (e) exhaustion, (f) dysphoric mood episodes, (g) work–family conflict, and (h) impaired concentration (Shonin et al., 2014c). The participant received an MBI known as meditation awareness training (MAT) (the same intervention was also administered in the aforementioned sex-addiction case study and in one of the aforementioned studies on the treatment of gambling addiction). MAT belongs to what have been termed the “second-generation” of MBIs because although mindfulness is a central feature of the program, compared with first-generation MBIs (FG-MBIs; such as mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy), MAT incorporates a greater range of meditative practices, ethics as a key component of the taught program, and an instructor training program that requires instructors to have undergone a minimum of 3 years supervised training (i.e., as opposed to a training period of just 1 year completed by some instructors of FG-MBIs) (Van Gordon et al., 2015).

As with many FG-MBIs, MAT introduces mindfulness techniques aimed at increasing perceptual distance from mental urges. However, MAT is deemed to be particularly suited to treating behavioral addictions because the additional meditation techniques that it incorporates are believed to invoke the following mechanisms of action: (a) reducing relapse and withdrawal symptoms by substituting maladaptive addictive behaviors with a “positive addiction” to the blissful and tranquil states associated with specific forms of meditation (Glasser, 1976) (i.e., while acknowledging that an addiction to meditation could potentially become maladaptive over the long term; Van Gordon et al., 2016c); (b) regulating dysphoric mood states and addiction-related shameful and self-disparaging schemas through the cultivation of compassion and self-compassion; (c) reducing myopic focus on reward by undermining the intrinsic value that individuals assign to the object of addiction (a central view in Buddhism is that phenomena are in fact “empty” of inherent existence; Van Gordon, Shonin, & Griffiths, 2016a); (d) reducing salience by fostering a better understanding of the “impermanent” nature of existence (e.g., a senior/lucrative occupational role must 1 day be relinquished, etc.); (e) growth in spiritual awareness that broadens perspective and prompts a reevaluation of life priorities; and (f) increased capacity to defer gratitude due to improved patience (Shonin et al., 2014b).

Building upon preliminary (but promising) findings observed during the use of MAT in the aforementioned workaholism clinical case study (and also during studies of MAT involving other forms of behavioral addiction), the purpose of this study was to conduct a controlled trial to investigate whether the salutary effects of MAT are observed in a larger sample of individuals suffering from workaholism. The primary study outcome was reduction in workaholism, and the secondary study outcomes were improved job satisfaction, improved job performance, decreased psychological distress, decreased work duration, and decreased work involvement.

Methods

Participants

Participants were male and female English-speaking adults working in full-time employment. Participation was on a voluntary basis, and participants were recruited through (a) posters and flyers strategically located across city and out-of-city work locations (e.g., business parks, factories, etc.) in the East Midlands region of the UK, (b) the client database of a local meditation center (limited to individuals who had expressed an interest in receiving meditation training but had not yet done so), and (c) presentations by the first two authors to personnel of key local employers. Furthermore, some East Midlands occupational physicians and occupational health nurse advisors were made aware of the study and were invited to informally raise awareness among relevant employees by suggesting that they could contact the research team for further information.

Eligibility criteria

The presence of workaholism was confirmed using the Bergen Work Addiction Scale (BWAS; Andreassen, Griffiths, Hetland, & Pallesen, 2012). To be considered eligible for the study, participants were required to score 4 or more on at least four of the scale’s seven items (see below for a fuller description of the BWAS). Other eligibility criteria for participation in the study were (a) aged between 18 and 65 years, (b) not currently absent from work (e.g., due to leave of absence, maternity leave, sickness, etc.), (c) no periods of annual leave planned for the duration of the 8-week intervention, (d) not currently undergoing formal psychotherapy, (e) not currently practicing meditation, and (f) no changes in psychopharmacology type or dosage 1 month prior to intervention (although stable prescription medication was permitted). Furthermore, participants had to be working a minimum of 32 paid hours per week. Rather than an indication of workaholism (that is not necessarily a function of hours worked per week), the purpose of this latter inclusion criteria was to ensure that all participants were engaged in full-time employment and thus maximize homogeneity in terms of participant’s work characteristics.

Procedure

Allocation procedure

Randomization was not employed because some participants who met the eligibility criteria indicated that they would be unavailable to attend the requisite number of MAT sessions due to pre-planned work or family engagements scheduled to occur on the same day as delivery of the MAT intervention. These pre-planned engagements included training days, attending conferences/workshops, medical appointments, family engagements, and weekly team meetings, etc. Therefore, allocation to MAT or a waiting-list control group occurred first by assigning all participants reporting availability issues to the waiting-list control group, and then subsequently visually inspecting demographic data to match MAT and control-group participants as closely as possible on sex, age, education level, salary, and employment type (i.e., white-collar or blue-collar workers).

Program description

MAT is an 8-week second-generation mindfulness-based intervention (SG-MBI) in which mindfulness is an integral component, but is not the exclusive focus (Van Gordon, Shonin, Sumich, Sundin, & Griffiths, 2014). In addition to mindfulness, MAT incorporates a range of meditation techniques including shamatha meditation (concentrative meditation), vipassanā meditation (analytical meditation), loving-kindness meditation, and compassion meditation. During the weekly sessions, participants engage in guided sitting meditation, walking meditation, and working meditation (i.e., maintaining meditative awareness while engaged in individual and group problem-solving or work-based tasks). The various meditative techniques employed are intended to engender (a) citizenship, (b) perceptive clarity, (c) ethical and compassionate awareness, (d) meditative insight (e.g., into subtle concepts, such as non-self and impermanence), (e) patience, (f) generosity (e.g., of one’s time and energy), and (g) life perspective. The ethical component of MAT was taught by participants engaging in guided group exercises that involved contemplating how their thoughts, words, and actions can influence both themselves and others, as well as society and the world more generally.

The intervention is delivered by instructors who have undergone a 3-year supervised MAT program. Participants attend eight weekly workshops (each lasting 2 hr) and receive a CD of guided meditations to facilitate daily self-practice. The weekly sessions comprise three distinct phases: (a) a taught/presentation component (approximately 45 min), (b) a facilitated group-discussion component (approximately 35 min), and (c) guided meditation and/or mindfulness exercises (approximately 30 min). A 10-min break is scheduled prior to commencing the guided meditation exercises. In the third and eighth week of the program, participants attend one-to-one support sessions (each of 50-min duration) with the program instructor. For comprehensive information regarding the intervention protocol, see Van Gordon et al. (2014).

In this study, MAT was delivered by the second author (30 years of meditation teaching experience) and the first author provided supervision to identify any deviations from the standard intervention delivery format. Supervision was implemented by the first author (a) silently observing at least 15 min of each weekly session (not always following the same amount of elapsed time into the 2-hr session) and (b) engaging in discussion with the program facilitator on a weekly basis. No unplanned deviations from the standard intervention protocol were identified. The intervention was delivered using group sizes of approximately 18 participants. Attending a minimum of seven of the eight weekly MAT sessions was a prerequisite for course completion. Participants who did not attend the requisite number of sessions were classed as having dropped out and were excluded from (or where unavailable to attend) future assessment phases.

Measures

The study outcomes were assessed using the following well-established psychometric scales:

BWAS (Andreassen et al., 2012; Orosz et al., 2016): The BWAS is embedded within general addiction theory and contains seven items reflecting core characteristics of addiction (i.e., salience, conflict, mood modification, withdrawal, tolerance, problems, and relapse). Questions are answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Examples of BWAS items are “How often during the last year have you worked so much that it has negatively influenced your health?” and “How often during the last year have you experienced that others have told you to cut down on work without listening to them?” High scores indicate greater levels of workaholism and scoring 4 or more on at least four of the scale’s seven items indicates that the individual is suffering from workaholism (Andreassen et al., 2012).

Abridged Job in General Scale (AJIGS; Russel et al., 2004): The AJIGS is a measure of job satisfaction and includes the following eight adjectives or short phrases: “makes me content,” “better than most,” “good,” “disagreeable,” “excellent,” “enjoyable,” “poor,” and “undesirable.” For each item, respondents are asked if they agree, are unsure, or disagree. A score of 3 is assigned for agree, 1 for unsure, and 0 for disagree. Individual items are summed to give a global score and negatively worded items are reverse-scored. Higher scores indicate greater levels of job satisfaction.

Role-Based Performance Scale (RBPS; Welbourne, Johnson, & Erez, 1998): The 20-item RBPS is a measure of general work performance. The RBPS assesses performance across five different aspects of an individual’s roles: (a) job (e.g., quantity and quality of work output and standard of internal and external customer services), (b) career (e.g., skill development and personal career goal attainment), (c) innovator (e.g., improving processes and routines, and generating and implementing new ideas), (d) team member (e.g., responding to others’ needs in his/her work group and ensuring his/her work group succeeds), and (e) organizational citizen (e.g., working for the overall benefit of the company). Scoring is on a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from “1 = needs much improvement” to “5 = excellent”) and each role typology contains four items. When summed together, scores for each role typology provide an overall indication of job performance. The RBPS is completed by participants’ direct line manager and thus provides a more objective measure of work performance. The RBPS was submitted directly to the research team by participants’ line managers.

Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995): The 21-item DASS assesses psychological distress and comprises three subscales that focus on depression, anxiety, and stress. The scale is scored on a 4-point Likert scale (ranging from “0 = did not apply to me at all” to “3 = applied to me very much, or most of the time”) and includes items, such as “I found it hard to wind down” and “I felt that life was meaningless.” Scores from the three subscales can be summed together to provide an overall indication of psychological distress. The DASS is completed in respect of the foregoing 7-day period. According to the DASS manual (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995), the percentile cut-offs and corresponding mean scores for symptom severity are as follows: 0–78 (M ≤ 13) = normal, 78–87 (M = 14–18) = mild, 87–95 (M = 19–28) = moderate, and >95 (M ≥ 28) = severe.

Work Duration and Work Involvement: Participants were asked to keep a diary and record (a) the total number of hours worked each week and (b) how many of these hours were accumulated while working during non-work hours (e.g., evenings and weekends).

Statistical analysis

A significance level of < .05 and two-tailed tests were employed throughout. Independent sample t-tests (for continuous variables) and chi-square (χ2) tests with Yates’s correction (for categorical variables) were used to identify any significant differences between groups in demographic characteristics or baseline-dependent variable mean scores. Mixed effects models (also known as multilevel models, random effects model, and hierarchical models) were used to examine the effect of intervention (MAT) and control on all six outcome measures (i.e., BWAS, AJIGS, DASS, RBPS, Work Engagement, and Work Involvement). Mixed effects modeling accounts for shared variance within-participants while modeling between-participant differences (Baguley, 2012b). The benefits of mixed effects models include fewer assumptions (i.e., homoscedasticity, sphericity, and compound symmetry) and greater statistical power over traditional approaches (Baguley, 2012a; Gelman & Hill, 2007; Quené & van der Bergh, 2004; Snijders & Bosker, 1999). Furthermore, mixed effects models account for baseline differences in outcome scores by modeling (per participant) the change in outcome score relative to baseline across all measurement phases (i.e., pre-, post-, and 3-month follow-up) (Van Gordon, Shonin, Dunn, Garcia-Campayo, & Griffiths, 2017). Prior to model estimation, distributions of all outcome variables and random effects residuals were inspected and deemed to be close approximations of normality. Using the absolute median deviation method to detect outliers (Leys, Ley, Klein, Bernard, & Licata, 2013), no data points were deemed to be extreme in this data set. The trial was conducted on an “intent-to-treat” basis with missing data at end point substituted on a last observation-carried-forward basis.

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Institutional Review Board of Nottingham Trent University (UK) approved the study. All participants were informed about the study and all provided informed consent. For ethical and transparency reasons, participants were required to acknowledge (i.e., as part of the informed consent procedure) that they understood that MAT (a) is deemed by its founders to be both a psychological and spiritual intervention, (b) is not intended to be a course on Buddhism (i.e., it is secular in context) but makes use of Buddhist meditative techniques and principles. This step was implemented because there are reports that some FG-MBIs have inappropriately emphasized or concealed their affiliation with Buddhism and/or spiritual practice to suit their needs (Van Gordon et al., 2017).

Results

Recruitment and allocation

Participants’ demographic characteristics are summarized in Table 1. A total of 108 individuals completed the screening questionnaire and 35 of them were excluded on the grounds of ineligibility. The main reasons for exclusion were: (a) did not meet the BWAS criteria for workaholism (15 individuals), (b) not in full-time paid employment (six individuals), (c) currently absent from work (five individuals), (d) currently receiving structured psychotherapy (four individuals), (e) recent change in psychopharmacology type or dosage (three individuals), and (f) already attending meditation or mindfulness classes (two individuals). Of the 73 remaining participants, 37 were allocated to the intervention group and the remainder to the waiting-list control group (see Figure 1).

Table 1.

Baseline demographic characteristics for each allocation condition

Characteristic MAT (n = 37) Control (n = 36)
Age, mean (SD) 38.60 (7.80) 38.83 (8.98)
Male (%) 59.50 58.33
Employment type (%)
 Blue collar 32.43 36.11
 White collar 67.57 63.89
Salary range (£1000s/year; %)
 <20 2.70 5.56
 20–40 54.05 58.33
 40–60 35.14 27.78
 60–80 5.41 2.78
 >80 2.70 5.56
Education (%)
 School leaver 21.62 22.22
 Vocational 29.73 33.33
 University 48.65 44.44
Marital status (%)
 Married 62.16 66.67
 Single 13.51 11.11
 Divorced 21.62 19.44
 Widow 2.70 2.78
Ethnicity (%)
 White (British) 48.65 50.00
 White (non-British) 18.92 27.78
 Asian 16.22 8.33
 Black (Caribbean) 16.22 13.89

            Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Flow of participants through recruitment and assessment phases

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.021

Non-completion

A total of five MAT and nine control-group participants dropped out of the study prior to the post-intervention assessment phase. Using a χ2 test of independence, differences in frequencies of drop-out and completion were not statistically significant [χ2(1) = 1.71; p = .19]. The reasons provided for non-completion were changed job (six individuals), sickness (five individuals), and became too busy due to work demands (three individuals). Of those participants who attended the post-intervention assessment phase, a further three MAT and five control-group participants were lost to follow-up. The response rate by participants’ line-managers on the RBPS was approximately 80%. All MAT participants who completed post-intervention assessments attended at least seven of the eight weekly group sessions. MAT participants practiced meditation for an average of 40.08 min per day (SD = 15.91).

Demographic and baseline characteristics

χ2 tests showed no significant difference between allocation conditions in terms of sex [χ2(1) < 0.01; p = .91], education [χ2(3) = 0.69; p = .87], employment type [χ2(1) < 0.01; p = .93], salary range [χ2(4) = 1.40; p = .84], family status [χ2(4) = 2.1858; p = .70], and ethnicity [χ2(3) = 1.61; p = .65]. Results also showed no significant difference between mean age of the control (M = 38.83; SD = 8.97) and intervention (M = 38.59; SD = 7.79) groups [t(70) = 0.121; p = .90] or contracted hours of the control (M = 39.50; SD = 3.63) and intervention (M = 39.94; SD = 4.88) groups [t(70) = −0.44; p = .66].

Independent sample t-tests were carried out to assess differences at baseline between allocated conditions across all outcome measures. Results showed no significant difference in baseline scores between control and intervention groups for BWAS [t(70) = 0.22; p = .82], AJIGS [t(70) = −0.43; p = .67], DASS [t(70) = 0.51; p = .61], RBPS [t(56) = 0.79; p = .43], Work Engagement [t(70) = −0.41; p = .69], and Work Involvement [t(70) = −0.06; p = .96] (see Table 2 for means and SDs).

Table 2.

Means and standard deviations of outcome variable scores for control and intervention groups at all time periods

Group BWAS AJIGS DASS RBPS Work Engagement Work Involvement
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Pre Intervention 29.30 3.04 9.46 2.94 28.03 3.78 73.56 6.11 53.49 5.25 13.54 5.18
Control 29.44 2.61 9.17 2.83 28.44 3.23 74.86 6.51 52.97 5.54 13.47 5.16
Post Intervention 21.65 5.39 13.70 3.16 19.19 4.95 70.53 23.34 49.68 4.99 9.73 5.65
Control 29.27 3.15 9.17 2.71 28.22 3.08 65.67 22.56 53.03 5.62 13.53 4.92
Follow-up Intervention 21.32 5.57 14.27 3.67 18.16 5.64 70.58 24.01 49.27 5.32 9.32 5.77
Control 29.06 3.97 9.19 2.86 27.97 3.45 65.74 22.40 53.00 5.56 13.50 4.85

Analysis of outcome measures

A separate mixed effects model was estimated for each outcome measure. Each model included group (control and intervention) and measurement interval (pre-, post-, and follow-up) as fixed effects [i.e., in the form of an interaction predictor (Group × Interval)] and participant (within measurement interval) as a random effect. This allowed a unique regression model (i.e., intercept and slope) to be specified for every participant across measurement intervals (see Figure 2 for an exemplar modeling BWAS scores across measurement intervals). Results from the six estimated mixed effects models show an overall strong effect of intervention compared with control for all outcome measures, with the exception of RBPS (see Table 3 for summaries of each model).


            Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Mixed effects model for BWAS. Note: The plot shows each participant’s BWAS score trajectory across measurement intervals (pre-, post-, and follow-up). Narrow lines illustrate trajectories at the subject-level, whereas two fuller lines illustrate the predicted population estimates by group (control vs. intervention)

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 6, 2; 10.1556/2006.6.2017.021

Table 3.

Fixed effects estimates (at post- and follow-up assessment phases) with 95% CIs for all six outcome measures

Value CIs t-value p-value
BWAS
(Intercept) 29.44
Post −7.48 −9.11 to −5.86 −9.09 <.001
Follow-up −7.58 −9.41 to −5.76 −8.22 <.001
AJIGS
(Intercept) 9.17
Post 4.24 3.18 to 5.31 7.87 <.001
Follow-up 4.78 3.54 to 6.03 7.59 <.001
DASS
(Intercept) 28.44
Post −8.62 −10.46 to −6.77 −9.24 <.001
Follow-up −9.39 −11.41 to −7.37 −9.20 <.001
RBPS
(Intercept) 74.88
Post 5.90 0.97 .34
Follow-up 6.21 1.01 .31
Work engagement
(Intercept) 52.97
Post −3.87 −5.21 to −2.52 −5.69 <.001
Follow-up −4.24 −5.66 to −2.82 −5.90 <.001
Work involvement
(Intercept) 13.47
Post −3.87 −5.21 to −2.52 −5.69 <.001
Follow-up −4.24 −5.66 to −2.82 −5.90 <.001

Note. The reference category in all cases is the control group. This means a post-BWAS score of −7.48 can be interpreted as a −7.48 change in BWAS score in comparison with the control condition relative to baseline (i.e., pre-BWAS score).

Discussion

A non-randomized controlled trial compared MAT (an SG-MBI) with a waiting-list control group in full-time employed adults suffering from workaholism. MAT participants demonstrated significant improvements over control-group participants in levels of workaholism, job satisfaction, psychological distress, work duration, and work engagement. Furthermore, compared with the control group, MAT participants demonstrated a significant reduction in hours spent working but without a decline in job performance. Therapeutic gains were maintained through 3-month follow-up.

This study is the first controlled study to investigate the utility of mindfulness for treating workaholism. Findings were consistent with a previous single-participant case study, which showed that MAT led to clinically significant change in workaholism symptomatology (Shonin et al., 2014c). However, unlike the case study that involved a senior manager working in a large corporation, this study involved participants of wide ranging education levels (i.e., school leaver, vocational qualification, university education, etc.), salary profiles (salary range: less than £20,000 per year to more than £80,000 per year), and occupational backgrounds (i.e., blue-collar and white-collar workers). Therefore, findings from this study expand the range of worker profiles for which MAT is likely to be beneficial.

Compared with FG-MBIs, SG-MBIs such as MAT integrate a greater spirituality component that has been shown to increase life purpose and prompt a reevaluation of life priorities (Van Gordon, Shonin, & Griffiths, 2016b). It has been proposed that the growth in spiritual awareness associated with meditation can reduce the salience that individuals assign to work or another given behavior (Shonin et al., 2013, 2014b). Participation in MAT is also associated with reduced attachment to self and environment (Van Gordon et al., 2016b). According to Buddhist theory, being less attached to the self means that an individual also becomes less attached to desirous objects in their environment (i.e., on the basis that it is the selfhood of an individual that assigns value to an object, substance, or behavior rather than the object or behavior possessing intrinsic value per se) (Van Gordon et al., 2016a). Thus, reduced self-attachment associated with participation in MAT may help to reduce myopic focus on reward, including urges for feelings of elation or escape elicited by problematic working.

The observed improvements in secondary study outcomes support the consensual scientific opinion that workaholism contraindicates job satisfaction and psychological well-being more generally (Shimazu et al., 2015). Mindfulness requires participants to be less future- or goal-orientated and to focus attentional resources on the task at hand (i.e., the present moment). For individuals suffering from workaholism, it is conceivable that goal-based working could reinforce an addiction feedback loop due to the mood modification and reward effects experienced at the point of goal attainment. Therefore, being less goal-orientated as part of a mindful approach to working may reduce sensation-seeking along with its associated negative consequences.

The fact that this study did not find significant improvements for MAT versus control in job performance is not consistent with findings from (a) the aforementioned MAT workaholism case study (Shonin et al., 2014c) and (b) a randomized controlled trial that administered MAT to middle managers of healthy clinical status (Shonin, Van Gordon, Dunn, Singh, & Griffiths, 2014). A plausible explanation for this finding is that missing data for the RBPS (i.e., due to drop-out and the fact that 20% of participants’ line managers did not complete and return the scale) is likely to have increased the standard error. Indeed, the fact that there was no significant change in job performance between allocation conditions could reflect a more efficient use of time by MAT participants, particularly given that work involvement and work engagement decreased for MAT versus control (i.e., MAT led to a reduction in hours spent working without causing job performance to decline). Nevertheless, it remains unclear at present as to whether the reduced focus on accomplishing goals embodied by mindfulness can paradoxically facilitate goal attainment and improved job performance more generally.

Although there appears to be a role for MAT in the treatment of workaholism, several factors limit the generalizability of these findings. In particular, selection bias may have been introduced due to a lack of randomization, and the use of a waiting list rather than active control condition means that non-specific factors (e.g., group interaction, psycho-education, therapeutic alliance, intrinsic motivation, etc.) may have influenced outcomes (i.e., rather than meditation per se). The provision of relaxation training (i.e., without the use of meditation) as a control condition could be used to address this limitation in any follow-up studies. Other factors that may limit the findings are the fact that (a) the follow-up assessment occurred after only 3 months had expired following intervention completion (i.e., a follow-up assessment at 6 or 9 months would have provided a better indication of maintenance effects), (b) self-employed workers were not represented in this study, (c) the sample size was reduced due to the number of control-group participants dropping out of the study prior to the post-intervention assessment phase (pre–post drop-out rate of 27.8% for control vs. 13.5% for MAT), and (d) interested participants were required to contact the research team directly to be considered for recruitment (i.e., participants were “self-referring” and it is thus difficult to gauge whether outcomes would be as favorable for individuals directly referred by their employer or by an occupational health professional).

A primary focus of interventions, such as MAT, is to encourage participants to integrate mindfulness into all aspects of work and family life. Therefore, MAT may be a practical and cost-effective workaholism intervention compared with treatments that require a reduction of work responsibilities or segregation from the work environment (Shonin et al., 2014c). Furthermore, findings from this study support outcomes of other studies indicating that MAT may be a cost-effective and feasible intervention for improving work-related well-being and work effectiveness more generally (including in individuals of healthy clinical status demonstrating adaptive levels of work enthusiasm and/or work engagement). Further research addressing the aforementioned study limitations is required to augment the evidence-base in terms of MAT’s effectiveness as a workaholism treatment. However, the present controlled trial adds further empirical support for the applications of SG-MBIs for treating workaholism and other forms of behavioral addiction.

Authors’ contribution

We confirm that all authors of this paper had access to the study data, are responsible for all contents of the article, and had authority over manuscript preparation and the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interest to declare.

References

  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Hetland, J. , Kravina, L. , Jensen, F. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). The prevalence of workaholism: A survey study in a nationally representative sample of Norwegian employees. PLoS One, 9(8), e102446. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102446

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a work addiction scale. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 265272. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00947.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Sinha, R. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2016). The relationships between workaholism and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 11(5), e0152978. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152978

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). Psychometric assessment of workaholism measures. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 29, 724. doi:10.1108/JMP-05-2013-0143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baguley, T. (2012a). Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 158175. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0123-7

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baguley, T. (2012b). Serious stats: A guide to advanced statistics for the behavioral sciences. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Demetrovics, Z. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Behavioral addictions: Past, present and future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(1), 12. doi:10.1556/JBA.1.2012.1.0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gelman, A. , & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical model. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glasser, W. (1976). Positive addictions. New York, NY, USA: Harper & Row.

  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005a). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10, 191197. doi:10.1080/14659890500114359

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005b). Workaholism is still a useful construct. Addiction Research and Theory, 13, 97100. doi:10.1080/16066350500057290

  • Griffiths, M. D. , & Karanika-Murray, M. (2012). Contextualising over-engagement in work: Towards a more global understanding of workaholism as an addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(3), 8795. doi:10.1556/JBA.1.2012.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. , Shonin, E. , & Van Gordon, W. (2016). Mindfulness as a treatment for gambling disorder. Journal of Gambling and Commercial Gaming Research, 1, 4752. doi:10.17536/jgcgr.2016.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karanika-Murray, M. , Pontes, H. M. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Biron, C. (2015). Sickness presenteeism determines job satisfaction via affective-motivational states. Social Science and Medicine, 139, 100106. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.06.035

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lanzo, L. , Aziz, S. , & Wuensch, K. (2016). Workaholism and incivility: Stress and psychological capital’s role. International Journal of Workplace Health Management, 9, 165183. doi:10.1108/IJWHM-08-2015-0051

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leys, C. , Ley, C. , Klein, O. , Bernard, P. , & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 764766. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lovibond, S. H. , & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. Sydney, NSW, Australia: Psychology Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maynard, B. R. , Wilson, A. N. , Labuzienski, E. , & Whiting, S. W. (2015). Mindfulness-based approaches in the treatment of disordered gambling: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Research on Social Work Practice. Advance Online Publication. doi:10.1177/1049731515606977

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orosz, G. , Loránd, E. , Dombi, E. , Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2016). Analyzing models of work addiction: Single factor and bi-factor models of the Bergen Work Addiction Scale. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14, 662671. doi:10.1007/s11469-015-9613-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quené, H. , & van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modelling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43, 103121. doi:10.1016/j.specom.2004.02.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quinones, C. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Kakabadse, N. (2016). Compulsive Internet use and workaholism: An exploratory two-wave longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 492499. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russel, S. S. , Spitzmuller, C. , Lin, L. F. , Stanton, J. M. , Smith, P. C. , & Ironson, G. H. (2004). Shorter can also be better: The abridged job in general scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 878893. doi:10.1177/0013164404264841

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schaufeli, W. B. , Taris, T. W. , & van Rhenen, W. (2008). Workaholism, burnout, and work engagement: Three of a kind or three different kinds of employee well-being? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 173203. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00285.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shimazu, A. , Schaufeli, W. B. , Kamiyama, K. , & Kawakami, N. (2015). Workaholism vs. work engagement: The two different predictors of future well-being and performance. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 1823. doi:10.1007/s12529-014-9410-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , Dunn, T. , Singh, N. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Meditation awareness training for work-related wellbeing and job performance: A randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12, 806823. doi:10.1007/s11469-014-9513-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Buddhist philosophy for the treatment of problem gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2, 6371. doi:10.1556/JBA.2.2013.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014a). Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and meditation awareness training (MAT) for the treatment of co-occurring schizophrenia with pathological gambling: A case study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12, 181196. doi:10.1007/s11469-013-9460-3

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014b). Mindfulness as a treatment for behavioral addiction. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy, 5, e122. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.1000e122

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014c). The treatment of workaholism with meditation awareness training: A case study. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing, 10, 193195. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2014.02.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snijders, T. A. B. , & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modelling. London, UK: Sage.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sussman, S. (2012). Workaholism: A review. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy, S6, 4120. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.S6-001

  • Sussman, S. , Lisha, N. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Evaluation and the Health Professionals, 34, 356. doi:10.1177/0163278710380124

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , Dunn, T. , Garcia-Campayo, J. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Meditation awareness training for the treatment of fibromyalgia: A randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Health Psychology, 22, 186206. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Towards a second-generation of mindfulness-based interventions. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 591592. doi:10.1177/0004867415577437

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016a). Buddhist emptiness theory: Implications for psychology. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. Advance Online Publication. doi:10.1037/rel0000079

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016b). Meditation awareness training for individuals with fibromyalgia syndrome: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of participant’s experiences. Mindfulness, 7(2), 409419. doi:10.1007/s12671-015-0458-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016c). Meditation awareness training for the treatment of sex addiction: A case study. Journal of Behavioral Addiction, 5, 363372. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.034

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , Sumich, A. , Sundin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Meditation awareness training (MAT) for psychological well-being in a sub-clinical sample of university students: A controlled pilot study. Mindfulness, 5, 381391. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0191-5

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welbourne, T. M. , Johnson, D. E. , & Erez, A. (1998). The role-based performance scale: Validity analysis of a theory-based measure. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 540555. doi:10.2307/256941

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Hetland, J. , Kravina, L. , Jensen, F. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). The prevalence of workaholism: A survey study in a nationally representative sample of Norwegian employees. PLoS One, 9(8), e102446. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102446

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a work addiction scale. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 53, 265272. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00947.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , Sinha, R. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2016). The relationships between workaholism and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. PLoS One, 11(5), e0152978. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0152978

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S. , Hetland, J. , & Pallesen, S. (2014). Psychometric assessment of workaholism measures. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 29, 724. doi:10.1108/JMP-05-2013-0143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baguley, T. (2012a). Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 158175. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0123-7

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baguley, T. (2012b). Serious stats: A guide to advanced statistics for the behavioral sciences. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Demetrovics, Z. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Behavioral addictions: Past, present and future. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(1), 12. doi:10.1556/JBA.1.2012.1.0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gelman, A. , & Hill, J. (2007). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical model. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glasser, W. (1976). Positive addictions. New York, NY, USA: Harper & Row.

  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005a). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10, 191197. doi:10.1080/14659890500114359

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005b). Workaholism is still a useful construct. Addiction Research and Theory, 13, 97100. doi:10.1080/16066350500057290

  • Griffiths, M. D. , & Karanika-Murray, M. (2012). Contextualising over-engagement in work: Towards a more global understanding of workaholism as an addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(3), 8795. doi:10.1556/JBA.1.2012.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. , Shonin, E. , & Van Gordon, W. (2016). Mindfulness as a treatment for gambling disorder. Journal of Gambling and Commercial Gaming Research, 1, 4752. doi:10.17536/jgcgr.2016.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karanika-Murray, M. , Pontes, H. M. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Biron, C. (2015). Sickness presenteeism determines job satisfaction via affective-motivational states. Social Science and Medicine, 139, 100106. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.06.035

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lanzo, L. , Aziz, S. , & Wuensch, K. (2016). Workaholism and incivility: Stress and psychological capital’s role. International Journal of Workplace Health Management, 9, 165183. doi:10.1108/IJWHM-08-2015-0051

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leys, C. , Ley, C. , Klein, O. , Bernard, P. , & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 764766. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lovibond, S. H. , & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales. Sydney, NSW, Australia: Psychology Foundation.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maynard, B. R. , Wilson, A. N. , Labuzienski, E. , & Whiting, S. W. (2015). Mindfulness-based approaches in the treatment of disordered gambling: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Research on Social Work Practice. Advance Online Publication. doi:10.1177/1049731515606977

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orosz, G. , Loránd, E. , Dombi, E. , Andreassen, C. S. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2016). Analyzing models of work addiction: Single factor and bi-factor models of the Bergen Work Addiction Scale. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14, 662671. doi:10.1007/s11469-015-9613-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quené, H. , & van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modelling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43, 103121. doi:10.1016/j.specom.2004.02.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Quinones, C. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Kakabadse, N. (2016). Compulsive Internet use and workaholism: An exploratory two-wave longitudinal study. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 492499. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Russel, S. S. , Spitzmuller, C. , Lin, L. F. , Stanton, J. M. , Smith, P. C. , & Ironson, G. H. (2004). Shorter can also be better: The abridged job in general scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 878893. doi:10.1177/0013164404264841

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schaufeli, W. B. , Taris, T. W. , & van Rhenen, W. (2008). Workaholism, burnout, and work engagement: Three of a kind or three different kinds of employee well-being? Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 173203. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00285.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shimazu, A. , Schaufeli, W. B. , Kamiyama, K. , & Kawakami, N. (2015). Workaholism vs. work engagement: The two different predictors of future well-being and performance. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 1823. doi:10.1007/s12529-014-9410-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , Dunn, T. , Singh, N. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Meditation awareness training for work-related wellbeing and job performance: A randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12, 806823. doi:10.1007/s11469-014-9513-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Buddhist philosophy for the treatment of problem gambling. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2, 6371. doi:10.1556/JBA.2.2013.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014a). Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and meditation awareness training (MAT) for the treatment of co-occurring schizophrenia with pathological gambling: A case study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 12, 181196. doi:10.1007/s11469-013-9460-3

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014b). Mindfulness as a treatment for behavioral addiction. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy, 5, e122. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.1000e122

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shonin, E. , Van Gordon, W. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014c). The treatment of workaholism with meditation awareness training: A case study. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing, 10, 193195. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2014.02.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snijders, T. A. B. , & Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modelling. London, UK: Sage.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sussman, S. (2012). Workaholism: A review. Journal of Addiction Research and Therapy, S6, 4120. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.S6-001

  • Sussman, S. , Lisha, N. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Evaluation and the Health Professionals, 34, 356. doi:10.1177/0163278710380124

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , Dunn, T. , Garcia-Campayo, J. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Meditation awareness training for the treatment of fibromyalgia: A randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Health Psychology, 22, 186206. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Towards a second-generation of mindfulness-based interventions. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 591592. doi:10.1177/0004867415577437

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016a). Buddhist emptiness theory: Implications for psychology. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. Advance Online Publication. doi:10.1037/rel0000079

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016b). Meditation awareness training for individuals with fibromyalgia syndrome: An interpretative phenomenological analysis of participant’s experiences. Mindfulness, 7(2), 409419. doi:10.1007/s12671-015-0458-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2016c). Meditation awareness training for the treatment of sex addiction: A case study. Journal of Behavioral Addiction, 5, 363372. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.034

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Gordon, W. , Shonin, E. , Sumich, A. , Sundin, E. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Meditation awareness training (MAT) for psychological well-being in a sub-clinical sample of university students: A controlled pilot study. Mindfulness, 5, 381391. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0191-5

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welbourne, T. M. , Johnson, D. E. , & Erez, A. (1998). The role-based performance scale: Validity analysis of a theory-based measure. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 540555. doi:10.2307/256941

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article for articles submitted after 30 April 2023 (850 EUR for articles submitted prior to this date)
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Apr 2024 0 108 93
May 2024 0 79 64
Jun 2024 0 97 32
Jul 2024 0 80 48
Aug 2024 0 127 39
Sep 2024 0 113 40
Oct 2024 0 79 30