Authors:
Hyoun S. Kim Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Ottawa, ON, Canada

Search for other papers by Hyoun S. Kim in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Alexandra Shifrin Department of Psychology, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada

Search for other papers by Alexandra Shifrin in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Travis Sztainert Gambling Research Exchange Ontario, Guelph, ON, Canada

Search for other papers by Travis Sztainert in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Michael J. A. Wohl Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Ottawa, ON, Canada

Search for other papers by Michael J. A. Wohl in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

We examined the potential role religious beliefs may play in disordered gambling. Specifically, we tested the idea that religiosity primes people to place their faith in good fortune or a higher power. In the context of gambling, however, this may lead to gambling fallacies (e.g., erroneous beliefs that one has control over a random outcome). People who are high in religiosity may be more at risk of developing gambling fallacies, as they may believe that a higher power can influence a game of chance. Thus, this research investigated the relationship between religiosity and gambling problems and whether gambling fallacies mediated this relationship.

Methods

In Study 1, we recruited an online sample from Amazon's Mechanical Turk to complete measures that assessed the central constructs (religiosity, disordered gambling, and gambling fallacies). In Study 2, we conducted a secondary analysis of a large data set of representative adults (N = 4,121) from a Canadian province, which contained measures that assessed the constructs of interest.

Results

In Study 1, religiosity significantly predicted gambling problem. Conversely, there was no direct relationship between religiosity and gambling in Study 2. Importantly, a significant indirect effect of religiosity on disordered gambling severity through gambling fallacies was found in both studies, thus establishing mediation. The results remained the same when controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status for both studies.

Discussion and conclusion

These findings suggest religiosity and its propensity to be associated with gambling fallacies, which should be considered in the progression (and possibly treatment) of gambling.

Abstract

Background and aims

We examined the potential role religious beliefs may play in disordered gambling. Specifically, we tested the idea that religiosity primes people to place their faith in good fortune or a higher power. In the context of gambling, however, this may lead to gambling fallacies (e.g., erroneous beliefs that one has control over a random outcome). People who are high in religiosity may be more at risk of developing gambling fallacies, as they may believe that a higher power can influence a game of chance. Thus, this research investigated the relationship between religiosity and gambling problems and whether gambling fallacies mediated this relationship.

Methods

In Study 1, we recruited an online sample from Amazon's Mechanical Turk to complete measures that assessed the central constructs (religiosity, disordered gambling, and gambling fallacies). In Study 2, we conducted a secondary analysis of a large data set of representative adults (N = 4,121) from a Canadian province, which contained measures that assessed the constructs of interest.

Results

In Study 1, religiosity significantly predicted gambling problem. Conversely, there was no direct relationship between religiosity and gambling in Study 2. Importantly, a significant indirect effect of religiosity on disordered gambling severity through gambling fallacies was found in both studies, thus establishing mediation. The results remained the same when controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status for both studies.

Discussion and conclusion

These findings suggest religiosity and its propensity to be associated with gambling fallacies, which should be considered in the progression (and possibly treatment) of gambling.

Introduction

Being devoted to the beliefs of one’s religion (i.e., religiosity; Esso & Dibb, 2004) has been shown to be a protective factor for engaging in addictive behaviors. Indeed, a growing body of literature suggests that people who are religious are less apt to smoke or gamble (Ellison & McFarland, 2011; Hodge, Andereck, & Montoya, 2007; Hoffmann, 2000; Islam & Johnson, 2003). One reason put forth for the positive relationship between religiosity and health is that the religious doctrine provides people with a sense of meaning in life, which facilitates positive living (Krause, 2003; Pargament, 1997). Contrary to this favorable view of religiosity, as it relates to health and well-being, is the evidence that suggests symptoms of disordered gambling elevated among the religious (Binde, 2007; Kaplan, 1978; Lam, 2006). Thus, although religiosity buffers against initiating gambling, problems may be more likely to develop among the religious once gambling is initiated.

The purpose of this research was twofold. First, we directly examined whether religiosity is positively associated with disordered gambling. Second, we examined a possible mechanism for the religiosity–disordered gambling link. Specifically, we hypothesized that people who are religious may place their faith in a higher power – a higher power that will intervene to ensure they will experience positive events in their life. For example, people who place their faith in higher power believe that it is possible for that higher power to intervene in cause–effect relationships (e.g., asking God to heal a terminal cancer patient; Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006). In the context of gambling, however, a belief that a higher power will intervene in games of chance may manifest as gambling fallacies (e.g., a belief that luck can be harnessed to maximized outcomes). As such, we tested a model in which the association between religiosity and symptoms of disordered gambling is mediated by the belief that a higher power may intervene to influence the outcome of a game of chance (i.e., gambling fallacies).

The Complex Relationship Between Religiosity and Gambling

Gambling has a long history of being in the “unhealthy” books of many religious groups (Aasved, 2003). Gambling tends to be perceived as an activity that is associated with greed and temptation, the misuse of money, and having a poor work ethic (Binde, 2007; Ellison & McFarland, 2011). Given the negative light that is placed on gambling among religions, it should be expected that the rate of gambling is low among religious people. Indeed, a small body of research suggests that religiosity has a prophylactic effect on disordered gambling (Ellison & McFarland, 2011; Hodge et al., 2007; Hoffmann, 2000; Koorn, 2011). That is, religiosity shields people from disordered gambling by placing psychological barriers on engaging in gambling in the first place. In support of this contention, Hoffmann (2000) found that people high in religious participation were less likely to report gambling problems.

Herein, we argue that religiosity may facilitate problem gambling among those who already engage in play. Circumstantial evidence for our general hypothesis comes from Kaplan (1978), who examined the religiosity of American lottery winners of the grand prizes. He found that one third of the lottery winners believed that divine or mystical forces positively influenced their success. Similarly, Lam (2006) found a positive relationship between importance of faith and gambling frequency among individuals who engaged in lottery gambling. Binde (2007) argued that religiosity may increase gambling, because people who are religious place their faith in a positive outcome on a higher power. Gamblers who are religious may feel that higher power will help maximize their success at gambling, despite the objective odds of success. In other words, people who are religious may be more susceptible to developing gambling fallacies.

The Potential Mediating Role of Gambling Fallacies

Religion and gambling have certain elements in common, such as the belief that a person has control over external and uncontrollable events, either through one’s own ability or a deity (Binde, 2007). People who engage in these types of thinking are more likely to have gambling fallacies (e.g., belief that a lucky person can positively influence the odds of success), which are the risk factors in the onset of gambling problems (Toneatto, Blitz-Miller, Calderwood, Dragonetti, & Tsanos, 1997; Wohl & Enzle, 2002; Wohl, Young, & Hart, 2007). Indeed, gambling fallacies (also referred to as erroneous cognitions about gambling) have long been proposed to have an etiological role in the development and maintenance of problem gambling (e.g., Delfabbro & Winefeld, 2000; Gaboury & Ladouceur, 1989; Leonard & Williams, 2016). For example, disordered gamblers are more apt to ignore statistical probabilities of gambling (e.g., base rate neglect and insensitivity to sample size) and believe that the outcomes of games of pure chance can be influenced to maximize positive outcomes (Leonard, Williams, & Vokey, 2015; Wohl & Enzle, 2002; Wohl et al., 2007). Importantly, the long-term impact of such illusions of control on gambling has been substantiated with longitudinal methods (Cowie et al., 2017; Yakovenko et al., 2016). Specifically, the risk of developing disordered patterns of gambling is heightened among players who have false beliefs about their ability to control the uncontrollable elements in gambling games.

Although the extant literature has elucidated the important role that both religiosity and gambling fallacies play in the development of disordered gambling, to our knowledge, no research has examined the interplay between religiosity, gambling fallacies, and disordered gambling. Specifically, whether relation between religiosity and disordered gambling is mediated by gambling fallacies is unknown. In this research, we tested the idea that a belief that a higher power can intervene in casually related events will manifest in gambling as a related belief that a higher power can intervene in gambling games to control the objectively uncontrollable. The downstream consequence is likely elevated risk of disordered gambling symptomatology. Herein, we use the umbrella term “gambling fallacies,” which include illusion of control. Our hypothesis was tested in two community-based studies. The first study examined the proposed mediation model in a sample of community-based gamblers in the United States. The second study capitalized on existing data from a large representative sample of community gamblers in a Canadian province.

Study 1

The purpose of Study 1 was to test our general hypothesis that religiosity was positively associated with disordered gambling among a sample of community-based gamblers. We also tested whether belief in a higher power manifests in the context of gambling as gambling fallacies. In this way, gambling fallacies would serve as the proximal predictor of disordered gambling. Thus, ultimately, we contend that the relationship between religiosity and disordered gambling is indirect such that it is mediated by gambling fallacies.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The final sample consisted of 201 gamblers (96 females and 105 males), ranging in age from 18 to 73 years with a mean age of 37.96 (SD = 12.74). In regard to ethnicity, 144 (71.3%) participants self-identified as Caucasian. The majority of the sample was employed full time (n = 116, 57.4%) and earned between $15,000 and $75,000 USD (n = 137, 68.16%). About nine participants were eliminated due to missing data and 10 participants were eliminated as they failed two or more attention checks that were embedded within our questionnaire. Furthermore, we checked the data for outliers and none were identified on our variables of interest. Participants were recruited using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) system. MTurk is an online tool that allows “workers” to complete small tasks for monetary compensation and has become an immensely popular tool to recruit participants for psychological research (Chandler & Shapiro, 2016). Importantly, Kim and Hodgins (2017) found that MTurk is a reliable and valid method of collecting data from addictions populations, including gamblers. Participants were compensated $0.50 USD, which is a normative rate on MTurk for psychological studies of similar duration.

A recruitment notice was posted on MTurk inviting people who gamble to take part in an online survey. Interested participants were then redirected to Qualtrics, where participants provided informed consent. Thereafter, participants completed a battery of measures that collected data on societal risks of gambling, including measures of problem gambling severity, gambling fallacies, and the influence of a higher power on gambling.

Measures

Religiosity

Three items (α = 0.88) of our own design were used to assess religiosity via belief in the divine’s influence over one’s gambling outcomes. These items are as follows: “There is often a higher power looking over me when gambling,” “There is a divine presence that guides my gambling,” and “My gambling is not influenced by a higher power” (reverse-coded item). Items are anchored at 1 (strongly disagree) and 5 (strongly agree). An average score was created with a higher score reflecting greater religiosity.

Gambling fallacies

The Gambler’s Beliefs Questionnaire (Steenbergh, Meyers, May, & Whelan, 2002) is a 21-item scale that measures the extent to which people believe that gambling-related outcomes can be influenced to maximize success. For brevity – which is required for research on MTurk – we randomly selected the four items (α = 0.81) from the full scale. These items are as follows: “My choices or actions affect the game on which I am betting,” “I have a ‘lucky’ technique that I use when I gamble,” “There are certain things I do when I am betting (e.g., tapping a certain number of times, holding a lucky coin in my hand, crossing my fingers, etc.), which increase the chances that I will win” and “I am pretty accurate at predicting when a ‘win’ will occur.” Items are anchored at 1 (strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). An average score was created with a higher score reflecting greater belief that gambling-related outcomes can be influenced to maximize success.

Problem gambling severity

We used the 9-item (α = 0.91) Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI; Ferris & Wynne, 2001) to assess the severity of gambling problems. The items on the PGSI are anchored from 0 (never) to 3 (almost always) and assess gambling-related consequences in the past 12 months (e.g., “Have you felt guilty about the way you gamble or what happens when you gamble?”). A total score is calculated based on participants’ responses with higher scores indicating greater gambling severity.

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Carleton University Research Ethics Board approved the study. All participants were informed about the study and all provided informed consent.

Results

Regression analyses were performed with gambling problems as the dependent variable and religiosity and gambling fallacies as the predictor variables (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics and Table 2 for regression analyses). As hypothesized, the results of the regression analysis indicated that religiosity significantly predicted the gambling problems, B = 0.07, F(1, 196) = 12.64, p < .001, R2 = .06. We also found support for our hypothesis that religiosity is associated with greater gambling fallacies, B = 0.47, F(1, 199) = 58.63, p < .001, R2 = .23. Similarly, gambling fallacies significantly predicted gambling problems, B = 0.12, F(1, 196) = 37.71, p < .001, R2 = .16, such that greater levels of gambling fallacies were associated with increased severity of gambling problems.

Table 1.

Correlations between measured variable with means and standard deviations on the diagonal: Study 1

Religiosity Gambling fallacies Problem gambling severity
Religiosity 2.32 (1.46)
Gambling fallacies 0.48** 3.47 (1.44)
Problem gambling severity 0.25** 0.40** 3.73 (4.79)

Note. Non-parametric Tau B’s correlations were calculated due to the skew of problem gambling severity and the pattern of results remained the same.

p < .01 level (two-tailed).

Table 2.

Summary of regression analyses predicting problem gambling severity

Variable B SE R 2 p F
Study 1
Religiosity 0.07 0.02 .06 <.001 12.64
Gambling fallacies 0.12 0.02 .16 <.001 37.71
Study 2
Religiosity −0.06 0.20 .01 .77 0.09
Gambling fallacies 0.25 0.05 .09 <.001 28.97

Note. B: unstandardized regression coefficient; SE: standard error; R2: squared multiple correlation coefficient; p: significance; F: F statistic.

To test our mediation model, we used Preacher and Hayes’ (2004) PROCESS Model 4 bootstrapping method with 5,000 iterations to obtain 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs). The results provided support for our hypothesized mediation as religiosity had a significant indirect effect on gambling problems through gambling fallacies, b = 0.56, SE = 0.14, 95% CI [0.32, 0.90] (Figure 1). In fact, the direct effect between religiosity and gambling problems was no longer significant when gambling fallacies were included in the model.


            Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Mediation model with religiosity as the independent variable, gambling fallacies as the mediator, and disordered gambling as the dependent variable among a sample of community-based gamblers: Study 1. The unstandardized coefficients and standard error shown in parentheses reflect the inclusion of the mediator in the equation. Two asterisks indicate a significant path, p < .001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 2; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.23

Discussion

The results of Study 1 replicated previous findings (Binde, 2007; Kaplan, 1978; Lam, 2006) by demonstrating that belief in a higher power is associated with elevated symptoms of disordered gambling. Importantly, however, we found that the religiosity–disordered gambling link is indirect. Specifically, belief in a higher power is positively associated with gambling fallacies that reflect a belief in the controllability of objectively uncontrollable outcomes in games of chance. A potential reason for this finding could be due to that people who are willing to believe that life events can be positively influenced by a higher power are also more apt to believe that the same higher power can intervene in games of chance. In other words, willingness to suspend traditional cause–effect logic in one’s religious life is correlated with willingness to suspend traditional cause–effect logic in games of chance. Unfortunately, when such logic is suspended while gambling, problematic gambling behaviors are likely to develop.

Although Study 1 provided support for our hypothesized mediation model, it was limited that our sample was recruited from MTurk, which is not a representative sample (Chandler & Shapiro, 2016). A further limitation of Study 1 was that our measure of religiosity was specific to religious beliefs within a gambling context (i.e., belief that a higher power can help maximize gambling-related outcomes), which may have inflated our hypothesized associations between religiosity, gambling fallacies, and disordered gambling. Thus, Study 2 aimed to address these limitations through a secondary analysis of representative adults from a large Canadian province, which used a general measure of religiosity (i.e., a measure that did not associate religiosity to the gambling context).

Study 2

The purpose of Study 2 was to conceptually replicate and address the limitations of Study 1. To this end, we capitalized on having access to data from a large-scale study of gambling and problem gambling conducted in the Quinte region of Ontario, Canada. The sample was representative of the demographic profile of the Canadian adult population, and the geographic region had similar gambling opportunities to the rest of Canada. Importantly, the survey used in the Quinte Longitudinal Study of Gambling and Problem Gambling (QLS; Williams et al., 2015) contained measures that assessed the central constructs examined in Study 1 (religiosity, gambling fallacies, and symptoms of disordered gambling).

Methods

Sample

The QLS is a 5-year longitudinal study that followed a sample of 4,121 adults in Ontario, Canada from 2006 to 2011. The sample was recruited through random digit telephone dialing and age and gender quotas were used to ensure a representative sample of the Canadian adult population. About 26% of the sample was recruited based on their high levels of gambling involvement. These at-risk gamblers were oversampled to ensure that a sufficient number of participants in the sample developed gambling-related problems during the duration of the study. The sample consisted of 1,867 males and 2,254 females ranging in age from 18 to 80 years (M = 46.07, SD = 14.11). Similar to Study 1, the majority of the sample reported being married (n = 2,393, 58.1%), being employed full-time (n = 2,141, 52.0%), and reported an annual income between $20,000 and $70,000 CDN (n = 2,395, 58.12%). Participants completed an online questionnaire once a year either online, at home, or at the QLS office. A total of 88.7% participants completed all assessments. For information regarding data cleaning, please refer to the QLS Data User Manual (Williams et al., 2014).

In this research, we restricted analysis to the first wave of assessments. This was done because religiosity was measured only once throughout the study (at the initial assessment) and was not measured in subsequent waves. Therefore, the only time where all three measures were assessed was during the first wave of data collection.

Measures

Religiosity

A general measure of religiosity was assessed using the Rohrbaugh Jessor Religiosity Scale (RJRS; Rohrbaugh & Jessor, 1975). The RJRS contains nine items (α = 0.90) that assess the influence that religion has on a person’s life, the strength of their religious beliefs, and the extent of their religion participation. The first item determines the participants’ religious affiliation by providing nine possible options (e.g., Catholic, Muslim, Atheist, and other). The second item inquires how often the person has attended religious services during the past year; this item was anchored at 0 (not at all) to 6 (more than once a week). The rest of the items are multiple choice questions with five possible options that vary per question. Examples of these items include: “How much influence would you say that religion has on the way that you choose to act and the way you choose to spend each day?” and “Which of the following statements comes closest to your belief about God?” As items 1 and 2 are not scaled, to provide a measure of participants’ religiosity, items 3–9 were summed to provide a measure of religiosity, with higher scores indicating greater religiosity.

Gambling fallacies

The Gambling Fallacy Measure (GFM; Leonard & Williams, 2016) was used to assess gambling fallacies. The GFM contains 10 items (α = .88) that assess several gambling fallacies, including the belief that one is luckier than others (“How lucky are you? If 10 people’s names were put into a hat and one name was drawn for a prize, how likely is it that your name would be chosen?”) and illusion of control (“Do you think your chances of winning a lottery are better if you are able to choose your own numbers?”). Each item is a multiple-choice question that contains only one correct answer; the more items the participant answers correctly, the less amount of gambling fallacies they have. For ease of interpretability, an average score was created and inversed so that a higher score reflecting greater belief that gambling-related outcomes can be influenced to maximize success.

Problem gambling severity

To assess symptoms of problem gambling, the QLS contained the 14-item (α = 0.81) Problem and Pathological Gambling Measure (PPGM; Williams & Volberg, 2010, 2014). The PPGM classifies participants into five categories: non-gambler, recreational gambler, at-risk gambler, problem gambler, and pathological gambler. Items are dichotomous with the options “yes” or “no.” In this research, participants’ scores were summed to provide a continuous measure of gambling severity.

Results

Regression analyses were performed with gambling problems as the dependent variable and religiosity and gambling fallacies as the predictor variables (see Table 2 for descriptive statistics and Table 3 for regression analyses). Contrary to Study 1, religiosity was not significantly associated with disordered gambling, B = −0.06, F(1, 3205) = 0.09, p = .77, R2 = .001. However, religiosity significantly predicted gambling fallacies, B = 0.02, F(1, 3957) = 33.52, p < .001, R2 = .01, such that individuals high in religiosity reported greater amount of gambling fallacies. Additionally, gambling fallacies were significantly associated with disordered gambling, B = 0.25, F(1, 3328) = 28.97, p < .001, R2 = .01. As the hypothesized model specified that the effect of the religiosity (the independent variable) on symptoms of disordered gambling (the dependant variable) is indirect, we proceeded to test the full mediation model. Doing so is in line with Hayes (2009, 2013), MacKinnon, Fairchild, and Fritz (2007), and others who argue that assessment of indirect effects via bootstrapping is the most valid test of mediation. To this end, we once again used Preacher and Hayes’ (2004) bootstrapping method with 5,000 iterations. As predicted, the indirect effect of religiosity on disordered gambling severity was estimated to lie between 0.001 and 0.002 with 95% CI (b = 0.001, SE < 0.001) (Figure 2).

Table 3.

Correlations between measured variable with means and standard deviations on the diagonal: Study 2

Religiosity Gambling fallacies Problem gambling severity
Religiosity 12.59 (6.64)
Gambling fallacies 0.09** 4.10 (1.48)
Problem gambling severity −0.01 0.09** 0.12 (0.56)

Note. Non-parametric Tau B’s correlations were calculated due to the skew of problem gambling severity and the pattern of results remained the same.

p < 0.01 level (two-tailed).


            Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Mediation model with religiosity as the independent variable, gambling fallacies as the mediator, and disordered gambling as the dependent variable among a sample of community-based gamblers: Study 2. The unstandardized coefficients and standard error shown in parentheses reflect the inclusion of the mediator in the equation. Two asterisks indicate a significant path, p < .001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 2; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.23

Discussion

Study 2 accomplished two main objectives. First, and most importantly, we replicated our hypothesized mediation model with a representative sample of adults. Second, Study 2 used different, yet related measures to assess our central constructs (religiosity, disordered gambling, and gambling fallacies) to corroborate the findings in Study 1. Altogether, our results provide converging support that religiosity may be an important etiological factor in the development of disordered gambling, insofar as religiosity may be associated with greater gambling fallacies.

Supplementary Analyses

To rule our alternative explanations for our results, we reran our mediational analyses controlling for potential confounding variables. Specifically, prior research has observed that disordered gambling tends to be more prevalent in young adults (Gupta & Derevensky, 1998; Welte, Barnes, Tidwell, & Hoffman, 2008), men (Williams, West, & Simpson, 2012), certain religious groups (e.g., Catholics; Welte et al., 2008), and people of lower socioeconomic status (Welte, Barnes, Wieczorek, Tidwell, & Parker, 2001). In addition, religiosity tends to be higher in older adults (Bengston, Silverstein, Putney, & Harris, 2015), women (Miller & Stark, 2002), and people of lower socioeconomic status (Schieman, 2010). As such, we reran our mediation analysis for Studies 1 and 2 controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status (as measured by personal income). Gambling fallacies remained a significant mediator of the relation between religiosity and disordered gambling in both Study 1, b = 0.24, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [0.09, 0.42], and Study 2, b = 0.0008, SE = 0.0003, 95% CI [0.0003, 0.0016], thus providing additional evidence for our hypothesized mediation model.

General Discussion

Having faith in one’s religion is associated with a myriad of health benefits (McCullough, Hoyt, Larson, Koenig, & Thoresen, 2000), including buffering people from engaging in risky behaviors, such as addictions (Hodge et al., 2007). A potential exception to this rule are the findings that religiosity may increase the risk of disordered gambling (Lam, 2006). Herein, we assessed whether the association between religiosity and disordered gambling is indirect. It was hypothesized that people who are religious may be more likely to believe that they can control the outcome (i.e., through an intervening higher power), despite the objective probability of success, which in turn may increase the risk of disordered gambling. Thus, we contend that gambling fallacies are the more proximal mechanism that explains the religiosity–disordered gambling link.

In two studies, using community samples from two different sources (online, Study 1 and representative adults, Study 2) and different measures to assess the central constructs, we found support for our hypothesized mediation model. Importantly, the hypothesized model held when controlling variables known to vary as a function of disordered gambling symptomatology as well as religiosity (i.e., age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status). In Study 1, we found that religiosity was associated with gambling fallacies and disordered gambling. Similarly, gambling fallacies were associated with disordered gambling. Importantly, we found support that the relationship between religiosity and disordered gambling may be mediated by gambling fallacies. Study 2 corroborated the findings of Study 1, with the exception that religiosity was not associated with disordered gambling. Variation in the presence of a direct link between religiosity and disordered gambling may be due to the measures of religiosity used in each study. In Study 1, we used items that measured belief in a higher power, which could directly influence the gambling outcomes. In Study 2, we used a general measure of religiosity (i.e., the measure assessed significance of religion in an individual’s life, the strength of their religious beliefs, and the extent of their religious participation), which did not make specific reference to the divine’s power to influence gambling-related outcomes. It is possible that the religiosity measure used in Study 1 inflated the association between belief in a higher power and disordered gambling severity. Importantly, however, the hypothesized indirect effect was found regardless of the religiosity measures used.

Lastly, it is important to note that the effect size of the mediation model was small. In addition, the portion of the variance explained by the mediation model in Study 2 was smaller than in Study 1. It is possible that the difference between the observed effects in the reported studies can be attributed to the fact that the religiosity measure used differed across the studies. In Study 1, the items referred to the use of the divine while gambling. In contrast, in Study 2, the items assessed religiosity in general (i.e., not within with the context of gambling). Although the effect size of the indirect effects was small and the measure of religiosity differed in the two reported studies, we have confidence in the findings. This is because the hypothesized mediation model was observed in both Studies 1 and 2, despite the methodological differences. In fact, we believe that the difference in measures used demonstrate the robustness of the hypothesized model.

Implications

The results of the study suggest that certain aspects of religiosity may be a risk factor for gambling problems, whereas other aspects of religiosity may be a protective factor against gambling problems. For example, previous research has found that individuals high in religious participation were less likely to report gambling problems (Hoffmann, 2000). However, among people of faith who already engage in gambling activities, religiosity may increase the risk of disordered gambling. We speculated that the link between religiosity and disordered gambling may be the result of religiosity facilitating the development of gambling fallacies, which have been linked to the risk of disordered gambling. Inherent to religion is the belief that a higher power can intervene to positively influence the cause–effect relations on behalf of the devotee. Although a belief that cause–effect relations can be positively influenced may provide comfort to the religious, problems may develop when transferred to the gambling context. It would behoove researchers to determine which types of gambling fallacies people who are high in religiosity are likely to endorse (e.g., magical thinking and illusion of control). Such information may be helpful in the development of targeted interventions among this potentially vulnerable population.

Our findings may also have some important implications for addressing the development and progression of disordered gambling among people who are high in religiosity. For example, one potential way to reduce gambling fallacies among people of faith who engage in gambling activities is through educational initiatives. It is possible that by providing information about the probability of success in gambling outcomes may reduce the belief that a higher power would intervene during games of chance. However, this assertion requires empirical attention. While believing that a higher power may intervene during times of distress by providing comfort and a sense of control can be adaptive, in the context of gambling, this belief in a higher power may heighten the risk of disordered gambling.

Limitations and Future Directions

Some limitations should be noted. First, the sample in Study 1 consisted of only individuals who use Amazon’s MTurk system. This may affect the generalizability of findings as the sample was not entirely representative of the general community, which we addressed in Study 2. That said, Study 2 is also not without its limitations. The overall response rate for the QLS study was fairly low (21.3%), thus response bias may have affected our findings. Lastly, the results of our study were based on cross-sectional design, and as such we cannot infer causality between our variables.

Future research would do well to assess the relations between religiosity, gambling fallacies, and problem gambling longitudinally. Doing so would help determine the causal relation between the variables of interest. Second, it would be informative if future research determines which type(s) of gambling fallacies people who are high in religiosity are most likely to endorse (e.g., magical thinking and illusion of control). Previous research has suggested that gambling fallacies consist of two general factors, luck/chance and skill/attitude, and these fallacies have differing relations to future gambling (Cowie et al., 2017). Increased specificity may help the development of targeted interventions among this potentially vulnerable population. Lastly, it is likely that there are other important variables (e.g., psychological distress) that may influence the relationship between religiosity, gambling fallacies, and disordered gambling. As such, future studies may do well to test whether our hypothesized mediation model holds when controlling for other important confounding variables.

Conclusions

This research aimed to determine whether gambling fallacies mediated the religiosity–disordered gambling link. Across two studies, we found support for our hypothesized mediation model. Our findings add to the growing understanding of risk factors in the development of problem gambling by exploring religiosity (a potential risk factor that has received a paucity of empirical attention). These findings may have important implications in developing targeted prevention and interventions for people of faith who engage in gambling activities. Doing so may help people who are high in religiosity maintain the plethora of benefits associated with religiosity, while preventing the development of gambling fallacies and disordered gambling.

Authors’ contribution

HSK, TS, and MJAW conceptualized the study. AS and TS conducted the statistical analyses. AS wrote the first draft of the manuscript and HSK, TS, and MJAW revised subsequent versions. All authors have read and approved the final draft of the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of interest

HSK and MJAW have received research grants from the Gambling Research Exchange Ontario (GREO). HSK receives a doctoral scholarship from the Alberta Research Gambling Institute. TS is a Knowledge Broker and Content Specialist for (GREO). AS reports no conflict of interest.

References

  • Aasved, M. J. (2003). The sociology of gambling (Vol. 2). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher, Ltd.

  • Binde, P. (2007). Gambling and religion: Histories of concord and conflict. Journal of Gambling Issues, 20, 145165. doi:10.4309/jgi.2007.20.4

  • Chandler, J. , & Shapiro, D. (2016). Conducting clinical research using crowdsourced convenience samples. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 5381. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093623

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cowie, M. E. , Stewart, S. H. , Salmon, J. P. , Collins, P. , Al-Hamdani, M. , Boffo, M. , Salemink, E. , de Jong, D. , Smits, R. , & Wiers, R. W. (2017). Distorted beliefs about luck and skill and their relation to gambling problems and gambling behavior in Dutch gamblers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2245. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02245

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delfabbro, P. H. , & Winefeld, A. H. (2000). Predictors of irrational thinking in regular slot machine gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 134(2), 117128. doi:10.1080/00223980009600854

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellison, C. G. , & McFarland, M. J. (2011). Religion and gambling among US adults: Exploring the role of traditions, beliefs, practices, and networks. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 50(1), 82102. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2010.01553.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Esso, N. , & Dibb, S. (2004). Religious influence on shopping behavior: An exploratory study. Journal of Marketing Management, 20(7–8), 683712. doi:10.1362/0267257041838728

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. , & Wynne, H. J. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final report. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaboury, A. , & Ladouceur, R. (1989). Erroneous perceptions and gambling. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4(4), 411420.

  • Gupta, R. , & Derevensky, J. (1998). Adolescent gambling behavior: A prevalence study and examination of the correlates associated with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(4), 319345. doi:10.1023/A:1023068925328

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408420. doi:10.1080/03637750903310360

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hodge, D. R. , Andereck, K. , & Montoya, H. (2007). The protective influence of spiritual-religious lifestyle profiles on tobacco use, alcohol use, and gambling. Social Work Research, 31(4), 211219. doi:10.1093/swr/31.4.211

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hoffmann, J. P. (2000). Religion and problem gambling in the US. Review of Religious Research, 41(4), 488509. doi:10.2307/3512317

  • Islam, S. M. , & Johnson, C. A. (2003). Correlates of smoking behavior among Muslim Arab-American adolescents. Ethnicity & Health, 8(4), 319337. doi:10.1080/13557850310001631722

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan, H. R. (1978). Lottery winners: How they won and how winning changed their lives. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

  • Kim, H. S. , & Hodgins, D. C. (2017). Reliability and validity of data obtained from alcohol, cannabis, and gambling populations on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31(1), 8594. doi:10.1037/adb0000219

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koorn, K. (2011). The roles of religious affiliation and family solidarity as protective factors against problem gambling risk in a Métis sample (Unpublished Master’s thesis). University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krause, N. (2003). Religious meaning and subjective well-being in late life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(3), S160S170. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.3.S160

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lam, D. (2006). The influence of religiosity on gambling participation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22(3), 305320. doi:10.1007/s10899-006-9018-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leonard, C. A. , & Williams, R. J. (2016). The relationship between gambling fallacies and problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(6), 694704. doi:10.1037/adb0000189

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leonard, C. A. , Williams, R. J. , & Vokey, J. (2015). Gambling fallacies: What are they and how are they best measured? Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy, 6(4), 256266. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.1000256

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacKinnon, D. P. , Fairchild, A. J. , & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 593614. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McCullough, M. E. , Hoyt, W. T. , Larson, D. B. , Koenig, H. G. , & Thoresen, C. (2000). Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 19(3), 211222. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.19.3.211

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miller, A. S. , & Stark, R. (2002). Gender and religiousness: Can socialization explanations be saved? American Journal of Sociology, 107(6), 13991423. doi:10.1086/342557

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norenzayan, A. , & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 174187. doi:10.1177/0146167205280251

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Preacher, K. , & Hayes, A. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 717731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rohrbaugh, J. , & Jessor, R. (1975). Religiosity in youth: A personal control against deviant behaviour. Journal of Personality, 43(1), 136155. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00577.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schieman, S. (2010). Socioeconomic status and beliefs about God’s influence in everyday life. Sociology of Religion, 71(1), 2551. doi:10.1093/socrel/srq004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Toneatto, T. , Blitz-Miller, T. , Calderwood, K. , Dragonetti, R. , & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253266. doi:10.1023/A:1024983300428

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welte, J. W. , Barnes, G. M. , Tidwell, M. C. O. , & Hoffman, J. H. (2008). The prevalence of problem gambling among US adolescents and young adults: Results from a national survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(2), 119133. doi:10.1007/s10899-007-9086-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welte, J. W. , Barnes, G. M. , Wieczorek, W. F. , Tidwell, M. , & Parker, J. (2001). Alcohol and gambling pathology among U.S. adults: Prevalence, demographic patterns and co morbidity. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62(5), 706712. doi:10.15288/jsa.2001.62.706

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , Hann, R. , McLaughlin, P. , White, N. , King, N. , Schopflocher, D. , West, B. , & Flexhaug, T. (2014). QLS data user manual. Retrieved from https://dataverse.scholarsportal.info/file.xhtml;jsessionid=62a8f5545e2d52041c82e6fe8819?fileId=8473&version=RELEASED&version=.0

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , Hann, R. , Schopflocher, D. , West, B. , McLaughlin, P. , White, N. , King, K. , & Flexhaug, T. (2015). Quinte longitudinal study of gambling and problem gambling. , Guelph, Ontario. Retrieved February 20, 2015, from http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3641

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2010). Best practices in the population assessment of problem gambling. Guelph, ON: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2014). Classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments. International Gambling Studies, 14(1), 1528. doi:10.1080/14459795.2013.839731

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , West, B. L. , & Simpson, R. I. (2012). Prevention of problem gambling: A comprehensive review of the evidence and identified best practices. Guelph, ON: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wohl, M. J. A. , & Enzle, M. E. (2002). The deployment of personal luck: Illusory control in games of pure chance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(10), 13881397. doi:10.1177/014616702236870

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wohl, M. J. A. , Young, M. M. , & Hart, K. E. (2007). Self-perceptions of dispositional luck: Relationship to DSM gambling symptoms, subjective enjoyment of gambling and treatment readiness. Substance Use & Misuse, 42(1), 4363. doi:10.1080/10826080601094223

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yakovenko, I. , Hodgins, D. C. , el-Guebaly, N. , Casey, D. M. , Currie, S. R. , Smith, G. J. , Williams, R. J. , & Schopflocher, D. P. (2016). Cognitive distortions predict future gambling involvement. International Gambling Studies, 16(2), 175192. doi:10.1080/14459795.2016.1147592

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aasved, M. J. (2003). The sociology of gambling (Vol. 2). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher, Ltd.

  • Binde, P. (2007). Gambling and religion: Histories of concord and conflict. Journal of Gambling Issues, 20, 145165. doi:10.4309/jgi.2007.20.4

  • Chandler, J. , & Shapiro, D. (2016). Conducting clinical research using crowdsourced convenience samples. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 12(1), 5381. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093623

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cowie, M. E. , Stewart, S. H. , Salmon, J. P. , Collins, P. , Al-Hamdani, M. , Boffo, M. , Salemink, E. , de Jong, D. , Smits, R. , & Wiers, R. W. (2017). Distorted beliefs about luck and skill and their relation to gambling problems and gambling behavior in Dutch gamblers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2245. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02245

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delfabbro, P. H. , & Winefeld, A. H. (2000). Predictors of irrational thinking in regular slot machine gamblers. The Journal of Psychology, 134(2), 117128. doi:10.1080/00223980009600854

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ellison, C. G. , & McFarland, M. J. (2011). Religion and gambling among US adults: Exploring the role of traditions, beliefs, practices, and networks. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 50(1), 82102. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2010.01553.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Esso, N. , & Dibb, S. (2004). Religious influence on shopping behavior: An exploratory study. Journal of Marketing Management, 20(7–8), 683712. doi:10.1362/0267257041838728

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferris, J. , & Wynne, H. J. (2001). The Canadian Problem Gambling Index: Final report. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaboury, A. , & Ladouceur, R. (1989). Erroneous perceptions and gambling. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 4(4), 411420.

  • Gupta, R. , & Derevensky, J. (1998). Adolescent gambling behavior: A prevalence study and examination of the correlates associated with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(4), 319345. doi:10.1023/A:1023068925328

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408420. doi:10.1080/03637750903310360

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hodge, D. R. , Andereck, K. , & Montoya, H. (2007). The protective influence of spiritual-religious lifestyle profiles on tobacco use, alcohol use, and gambling. Social Work Research, 31(4), 211219. doi:10.1093/swr/31.4.211

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hoffmann, J. P. (2000). Religion and problem gambling in the US. Review of Religious Research, 41(4), 488509. doi:10.2307/3512317

  • Islam, S. M. , & Johnson, C. A. (2003). Correlates of smoking behavior among Muslim Arab-American adolescents. Ethnicity & Health, 8(4), 319337. doi:10.1080/13557850310001631722

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kaplan, H. R. (1978). Lottery winners: How they won and how winning changed their lives. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

  • Kim, H. S. , & Hodgins, D. C. (2017). Reliability and validity of data obtained from alcohol, cannabis, and gambling populations on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31(1), 8594. doi:10.1037/adb0000219

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koorn, K. (2011). The roles of religious affiliation and family solidarity as protective factors against problem gambling risk in a Métis sample (Unpublished Master’s thesis). University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krause, N. (2003). Religious meaning and subjective well-being in late life. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(3), S160S170. doi:10.1093/geronb/58.3.S160

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lam, D. (2006). The influence of religiosity on gambling participation. Journal of Gambling Studies, 22(3), 305320. doi:10.1007/s10899-006-9018-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leonard, C. A. , & Williams, R. J. (2016). The relationship between gambling fallacies and problem gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(6), 694704. doi:10.1037/adb0000189

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leonard, C. A. , Williams, R. J. , & Vokey, J. (2015). Gambling fallacies: What are they and how are they best measured? Journal of Addiction Research & Therapy, 6(4), 256266. doi:10.4172/2155-6105.1000256

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacKinnon, D. P. , Fairchild, A. J. , & Fritz, M. S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 593614. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McCullough, M. E. , Hoyt, W. T. , Larson, D. B. , Koenig, H. G. , & Thoresen, C. (2000). Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychology, 19(3), 211222. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.19.3.211

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miller, A. S. , & Stark, R. (2002). Gender and religiousness: Can socialization explanations be saved? American Journal of Sociology, 107(6), 13991423. doi:10.1086/342557

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norenzayan, A. , & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 174187. doi:10.1177/0146167205280251

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Preacher, K. , & Hayes, A. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 717731. doi:10.3758/BF03206553

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rohrbaugh, J. , & Jessor, R. (1975). Religiosity in youth: A personal control against deviant behaviour. Journal of Personality, 43(1), 136155. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1975.tb00577.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schieman, S. (2010). Socioeconomic status and beliefs about God’s influence in everyday life. Sociology of Religion, 71(1), 2551. doi:10.1093/socrel/srq004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steenbergh, T. A. , Meyers, A. W. , May, R. K. , & Whelan, J. P. (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers’ Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16(2), 143149. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.16.2.143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Toneatto, T. , Blitz-Miller, T. , Calderwood, K. , Dragonetti, R. , & Tsanos, A. (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 13(3), 253266. doi:10.1023/A:1024983300428

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welte, J. W. , Barnes, G. M. , Tidwell, M. C. O. , & Hoffman, J. H. (2008). The prevalence of problem gambling among US adolescents and young adults: Results from a national survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 24(2), 119133. doi:10.1007/s10899-007-9086-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Welte, J. W. , Barnes, G. M. , Wieczorek, W. F. , Tidwell, M. , & Parker, J. (2001). Alcohol and gambling pathology among U.S. adults: Prevalence, demographic patterns and co morbidity. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62(5), 706712. doi:10.15288/jsa.2001.62.706

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , Hann, R. , McLaughlin, P. , White, N. , King, N. , Schopflocher, D. , West, B. , & Flexhaug, T. (2014). QLS data user manual. Retrieved from https://dataverse.scholarsportal.info/file.xhtml;jsessionid=62a8f5545e2d52041c82e6fe8819?fileId=8473&version=RELEASED&version=.0

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , Hann, R. , Schopflocher, D. , West, B. , McLaughlin, P. , White, N. , King, K. , & Flexhaug, T. (2015). Quinte longitudinal study of gambling and problem gambling. , Guelph, Ontario. Retrieved February 20, 2015, from http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3641

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2010). Best practices in the population assessment of problem gambling. Guelph, ON: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , & Volberg, R. A. (2014). Classification accuracy of four problem gambling assessment instruments. International Gambling Studies, 14(1), 1528. doi:10.1080/14459795.2013.839731

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, R. J. , West, B. L. , & Simpson, R. I. (2012). Prevention of problem gambling: A comprehensive review of the evidence and identified best practices. Guelph, ON: Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wohl, M. J. A. , & Enzle, M. E. (2002). The deployment of personal luck: Illusory control in games of pure chance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(10), 13881397. doi:10.1177/014616702236870

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wohl, M. J. A. , Young, M. M. , & Hart, K. E. (2007). Self-perceptions of dispositional luck: Relationship to DSM gambling symptoms, subjective enjoyment of gambling and treatment readiness. Substance Use & Misuse, 42(1), 4363. doi:10.1080/10826080601094223

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yakovenko, I. , Hodgins, D. C. , el-Guebaly, N. , Casey, D. M. , Currie, S. R. , Smith, G. J. , Williams, R. J. , & Schopflocher, D. P. (2016). Cognitive distortions predict future gambling involvement. International Gambling Studies, 16(2), 175192. doi:10.1080/14459795.2016.1147592

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand
The author instruction is available in PDF.
Please, download the file from HERE

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2022  
Web of Science  
Total Cites
WoS
5713
Journal Impact Factor 7.8
Rank by Impact Factor

Psychiatry (SCIE) 18/155
Psychiatry (SSCI) 13/144

Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
7.2
5 Year
Impact Factor
8.9
Journal Citation Indicator 1.42
Rank by Journal Citation Indicator

Psychiatry 35/264

Scimago  
Scimago
H-index
69
Scimago
Journal Rank
1.918
Scimago Quartile Score Clinical Psychology Q1
Medicine (miscellaneous) Q1
Psychiatry and Mental Health Q1
Scopus  
Scopus
Cite Score
11.1
Scopus
Cite Score Rank
Clinical Psychology 10/292 (96th PCTL)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 30/531 (94th PCTL)
Medicine (miscellaneous) 25/309 (92th PCTL)
Scopus
SNIP
1.966

 

 
2021  
Web of Science  
Total Cites
WoS
5223
Journal Impact Factor 7,772
Rank by Impact Factor Psychiatry SCIE 26/155
Psychiatry SSCI 19/142
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
7,130
5 Year
Impact Factor
9,026
Journal Citation Indicator 1,39
Rank by Journal Citation Indicator

Psychiatry 34/257

Scimago  
Scimago
H-index
56
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,951
Scimago Quartile Score Clinical Psychology (Q1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health (Q1)
Scopus  
Scopus
Cite Score
11,5
Scopus
CIte Score Rank
Clinical Psychology 5/292 (D1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 20/529 (D1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) 17/276 (D1)
Scopus
SNIP
2,184

2020  
Total Cites 4024
WoS
Journal
Impact Factor
6,756
Rank by Psychiatry (SSCI) 12/143 (Q1)
Impact Factor Psychiatry 19/156 (Q1)
Impact Factor 6,052
without
Journal Self Cites
5 Year 8,735
Impact Factor
Journal  1,48
Citation Indicator  
Rank by Journal  Psychiatry 24/250 (Q1)
Citation Indicator   
Citable 86
Items
Total 74
Articles
Total 12
Reviews
Scimago 47
H-index
Scimago 2,265
Journal Rank
Scimago Clinical Psychology Q1
Quartile Score Psychiatry and Mental Health Q1
  Medicine (miscellaneous) Q1
Scopus 3593/367=9,8
Scite Score  
Scopus Clinical Psychology 7/283 (Q1)
Scite Score Rank Psychiatry and Mental Health 22/502 (Q1)
Scopus 2,026
SNIP  
Days from  38
submission  
to 1st decision  
Days from  37
acceptance  
to publication  
Acceptance 31%
Rate  

2019  
Total Cites
WoS
2 184
Impact Factor 5,143
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
4,346
5 Year
Impact Factor
5,758
Immediacy
Index
0,587
Citable
Items
75
Total
Articles
67
Total
Reviews
8
Cited
Half-Life
3,3
Citing
Half-Life
6,8
Eigenfactor
Score
0,00597
Article Influence
Score
1,447
% Articles
in
Citable Items
89,33
Normalized
Eigenfactor
0,7294
Average
IF
Percentile
87,923
Scimago
H-index
37
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,767
Scopus
Scite Score
2540/376=6,8
Scopus
Scite Score Rank
Cllinical Psychology 16/275 (Q1)
Medicine (miscellenous) 31/219 (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 47/506 (Q1)
Scopus
SNIP
1,441
Acceptance
Rate
32%

 

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article for articles submitted after 30 April 2023 (850 EUR for articles submitted prior to this date)
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Max W. ABBOTT (Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand)
  • Elias N. ABOUJAOUDE (Stanford University School of Medicine, USA)
  • Hojjat ADELI (Ohio State University, USA)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (University of Dundee, United Kingdom)
  • Alex BLASZCZYNSKI (University of Sidney, Australia)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Kenneth BLUM (University of Florida, USA)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Sam-Wook CHOI (Eulji University, Republic of Korea)
  • Damiaan DENYS (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Heather HAUSENBLAS (Jacksonville University, USA)
  • Tobias HAYER (University of Bremen, Germany)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Jaeseung JEONG (Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Emmanuel KUNTSCHE (La Trobe University, Australia)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Michel LEJOXEUX (Paris University, France)
  • Anikó MARÁZ (Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Astrid MÜLLER  (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Frederick GERARD MOELLER (University of Texas, USA)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Nancy PETRY (University of Connecticut, USA)
  • Bettina PIKÓ (University of Szeged, Hungary)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Rory C. REID (University of California Los Angeles, USA)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ferenc TÚRY (Semmelweis University, Hungary)
  • Alfred UHL (Austrian Federal Health Institute, Austria)
  • Róbert URBÁN  (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Johan VANDERLINDEN (University Psychiatric Center K.U.Leuven, Belgium)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN  (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Kimberly YOUNG (Center for Internet Addiction, USA)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jun 2023 0 18 18
Jul 2023 0 16 6
Aug 2023 0 22 10
Sep 2023 0 23 10
Oct 2023 0 31 15
Nov 2023 0 36 27
Dec 2023 0 4 0