Authors:
Seung-Yup Lee Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Uijeongbu St. Mary’s Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Seung-Yup Lee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Donghwan Lee Department of Statistics, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Donghwan Lee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Cho Rong Nam Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Cho Rong Nam in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Da Yea Kim Department of Statistics, Ewha Womans University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Da Yea Kim in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Sera Park I Will Center, Seoul Metropolitan Boramae Youth Center, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Sera Park in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jun-Gun Kwon I Will Center, Seoul Metropolitan Boramae Youth Center, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Jun-Gun Kwon in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Yong-Sil Kweon Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Uijeongbu St. Mary’s Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Yong-Sil Kweon in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Youngjo Lee Department of Statistics, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Youngjo Lee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Dai Jin Kim Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Seoul St. Mary’s Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Dai Jin Kim in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Jung-Seok Choi Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center, Seoul, Republic of Korea
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, College of Medicine, Seoul National University, Seoul, Republic of Korea

Search for other papers by Jung-Seok Choi in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and objectives

The ubiquitous Internet connections by smartphones weakened the traditional boundaries between computers and mobile phones. We sought to explore whether smartphone-related problems differ from those of computer use according to gender using latent class analysis (LCA).

Methods

After informed consents, 555 Korean middle-school students completed surveys on gaming, Internet use, and smartphone usage patterns. They also completed various psychosocial instruments. LCA was performed for the whole group and by gender. In addition to ANOVA and χ2 tests, post-hoc tests were conducted to examine differences among the LCA subgroups.

Results

In the whole group (n = 555), four subtypes were identified: dual-problem users (49.5%), problematic Internet users (7.7%), problematic smartphone users (32.1%), and “healthy” users (10.6%). Dual-problem users scored highest for addictive behaviors and other psychopathologies. The gender-stratified LCA revealed three subtypes for each gender. With dual-problem and healthy subgroup as common, problematic Internet subgroup was classified in the males, whereas problematic smartphone subgroup was classified in the females in the gender-stratified LCA. Thus, distinct patterns were observed according to gender with higher proportion of dual-problem present in males. While gaming was associated with problematic Internet use in males, aggression and impulsivity demonstrated associations with problematic smartphone use in females.

Conclusions

An increase in the number of digital media-related problems was associated with worse outcomes in various psychosocial scales. Gaming may play a crucial role in males solely displaying Internet-related problems. The heightened impulsivity and aggression seen in our female problematic smartphone users requires further research.

Abstract

Background and objectives

The ubiquitous Internet connections by smartphones weakened the traditional boundaries between computers and mobile phones. We sought to explore whether smartphone-related problems differ from those of computer use according to gender using latent class analysis (LCA).

Methods

After informed consents, 555 Korean middle-school students completed surveys on gaming, Internet use, and smartphone usage patterns. They also completed various psychosocial instruments. LCA was performed for the whole group and by gender. In addition to ANOVA and χ2 tests, post-hoc tests were conducted to examine differences among the LCA subgroups.

Results

In the whole group (n = 555), four subtypes were identified: dual-problem users (49.5%), problematic Internet users (7.7%), problematic smartphone users (32.1%), and “healthy” users (10.6%). Dual-problem users scored highest for addictive behaviors and other psychopathologies. The gender-stratified LCA revealed three subtypes for each gender. With dual-problem and healthy subgroup as common, problematic Internet subgroup was classified in the males, whereas problematic smartphone subgroup was classified in the females in the gender-stratified LCA. Thus, distinct patterns were observed according to gender with higher proportion of dual-problem present in males. While gaming was associated with problematic Internet use in males, aggression and impulsivity demonstrated associations with problematic smartphone use in females.

Conclusions

An increase in the number of digital media-related problems was associated with worse outcomes in various psychosocial scales. Gaming may play a crucial role in males solely displaying Internet-related problems. The heightened impulsivity and aggression seen in our female problematic smartphone users requires further research.

Introduction

Information technologies and wireless communications have brought rapid changes in everyday life. Communication, education, entertainment, and commercial transactions are increasingly taking place online. In Europe, household Internet access rose from 55% in 2007 to 85% in 2015, and Internet access through a mobile device also increased from 36% in 2012 to 59% in 2016 (Eurostat, 2017). Personal computers and smartphones have also become popular among children and adolescents. Indeed, even mentioning the increased use by adolescents appears as a cliché in the current climate.

Despite effecting many convenient and positive changes in education and recreation, excessive or inappropriate use of the new digital media by children and adolescents has heightened the concerns of parents and the wider community, including schools, with respect to potential disruption of normal development and academic achievement, and exposure to inappropriate content, such as pornography. The negative consequences of the new digital media on physical health may range from obesity to even death by accident, with the greatest danger associated with smartphones posed by use while crossing the road, as reported for younger age groups (Ayers et al., 2016; Hull, Draghici, & Sargent, 2012; Lennon, Oviedo-Trespalacios, & Matthews, 2017; Vandewater, Shim, & Caplovitz, 2004). Prior research has indicated that such misuse may also negatively impact mental health, by increasing sleep disturbance, depression, anxiety, impulsivity, and even suicidality (Achab et al., 2011; Gentile et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2006; Messias, Castro, Saini, Usman, & Peeples, 2011; Wei, Chen, Huang, & Bai, 2012; Weinstein & Weizman, 2012; Wu, Tao, Zhang, Zhang, & Tao, 2015).

During their youth, today’s parents may have used computers or conventional mobile phones, but were not exposed to the high-speed Internet connections and smartphones that their offspring use. Thus, they may experience difficulties in recognizing a problematic use or features associated with it. Although concerned, parents and the wider community could be confused by these new electronic media phenomena and may also experience difficulties in guiding their children in their use.

As per the famous old saying of “time will tell,” longitudinal studies and the passage of time will provide clarity regarding the consequences of new media usage, especially for highly excessive patterns of use. To achieve this, adequate classification of the problem is essential. However, it is also uncertain whether the problems related to smartphones are distinct from the Internet-related problems associated with the computer use. Most previous studies were concerned either with smartphones or computers but not both, despite this uncertainty. In addition to the device-related factors, individual characteristics may also influence the usage patterns. Thus, any exploration of these relatively new phenomena faces many challenges.

Latent class analysis (LCA) may represent a valuable tool, because it requires no structural modeling. Rather than relying on fixed assignments, LCA derives a set of latent variables from a series of observed variables and allocates them to a latent class. Thus, this analytical method may be particularly useful in revealing patterns of use of new digital media and, in turn, for adequate classification by usage pattern. A few studies used LCA as a statistical method to explore potential gaming (Borsari et al., 2013; Carras et al., 2017; Kiraly et al., 2017; Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Gentile, 2015), Internet addiction (Rumpf et al., 2014), and social network service-using subgroups (Dantlgraber, Wetzel, Schutzenberger, Stieger, & Reips, 2016). However, only one study, which was conducted by some of the present authors, simultaneously analyzed both Internet- and smartphone-related problems (Mok et al., 2014).

In the previous study, Internet and smartphone usage patterns were found to differ by gender among university students. Three distinct Internet and smartphone user groups were identified for each gender. Males were classified into similar low, moderate, and high addiction groups for both Internet and smartphone use. However, females showed different usage patterns. While two female subgroups displayed low and high levels of addiction to both Internet and smartphone usage, the remaining subgroup demonstrated low addiction to the Internet but high addiction to smartphones, rather than simply a moderate level of addiction to both.

Thus, the previous study first revealed a different pattern of addictive use of the Internet and/or smartphones by gender according to LCA (Mok et al., 2014). Although the demonstration of such a gender difference made a meaningful contribution to the literature, there was also a major limitation; specifically, no plausible explanation was provided for the gender difference.

To overcome the shortcomings of prior studies, we explored Internet and smartphone usage patterns in a larger sample, and captured more detailed information on digital media use and a broader range of psychosocial variables that are known to be associated with problematic use, such as depression, anxiety, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), aggression, and impulsivity. Considering the fact that the fronto-striatal mismatch in neurodevelopment of the juvenile brains renders the adolescents to increased impulsivity (Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover, & Casey, 2007), this study employed a range of instruments that measure various aspects of dysregulated cognitive control or reward-seeking. It was postulated that such comprehensive assessment of multiple domains may better reveal of which psychosocial characteristics are more related to excessive use pattern of the Internet or smartphones. Moreover, no study has examined patterns of addictive use of the Internet and/or smartphones by gender in adolescents using LCA. Thus, the primary objective of this study was to examine, in a relatively large sample, whether the previously demonstrated gender differences in electronic media usage also applied to adolescents. Second, we sought to identify psychosocial and media-related factors contributing to such gender differences in adolescents.

Methods

Participants

In total, 714 students from five public middle schools in Seoul, Republic of Korea participated in this study. Different school years may act as confounders due to differences in developmental stage or culture. Therefore, we recruited all the participants from same school year of year 9 to ensure the homogeneity of our sample. The participating students completed the questionnaires at their own school and gift vouchers were provided as a reward for participation. We excluded 159 samples due to missing values on either the Internet or Smartphone Addiction Scales (SASs). Consequently, 555 students (295 males and 260 females) were included in the final analysis.

Measures

Internet Addiction Test (IAT)

The IAT is a 20-item scale, developed by Young to assess the severity of addiction to the Internet. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale from ranging 1 (very rarely) to 5 (very frequently). Total scores on the instrument can range from 20 to 100, with a higher total scores indicating more severe Internet addiction (Young, 1999). The overall Cronbach’s α coefficient was .889 in a meta-analysis (Frangos, Frangos, & Sotiropoulos, 2012).

Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS)

The SAS consists of 33 items that evaluate problematic smartphone use across six domains: daily-life disturbance, positive anticipation, withdrawal, cyberspace-oriented relationship, overuse, and tolerance. Each item is rated on a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). A higher total SAS score indicates more severe smartphone addiction (Kwon et al., 2013). It previously demonstrated a good internal consistency with the Cronbach’s α coefficient of .967 (Kwon et al., 2013).

Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scales (BIS/BAS)

The BIS and BAS Scales were developed based on Gray’s biopsychological theory of personality to assess sensitivity to punishment and rewards, respectively (Carver & White, 1994; Gray, 1990). Altogether, they consist of 20 items, which are rated on 4-point scales ranging from 1 (very true for me) to 4 (very false for me). The BIS contains seven items that measure attitudes toward potentially unpleasant stimuli/anticipated punishments. The BAS consists of 13 items that measure the degree of motivation for rewards/goals/pleasure, which can further be subdivided into three subscales: reward responsiveness (BAS-R), drive to pursue goals (BAS-D), and fun-seeking (BAS-F; Carver & White, 1994). The Cronbach’s α coefficient for BIS was .74, while the coefficient values for BAS-R, BAS-D, and BAS-F were .73, .76, and .66, respectively (Carver & White, 1994).

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)

The BDI is a 21-item self-rated questionnaire assessing the severity of depressive symptoms experienced in the past week on a 4-point scale (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961). The validated Korean version was used, which showed the Cronbach’s α coefficient of .85 (Rhee et al., 1995).

Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)

The BAI is a 21-item self-rated questionnaire assessing the severity of anxiety symptoms experienced during the past week on a 4-point scale (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988). In Korean validation, it displayed a good internal consistency with the Cronbach’s α coefficient of .93 (Kim et al., 2016).

Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale – short version (CASS-S)

The CASS-S consists of 27 items that assess ADHD symptoms, focusing on cognitive, hyperactivity-related, and conduct problems in adolescents based on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (not true) to 3 (very often) (Conners, 1997). The CASS-S demonstrated good internal consistency with the Cronbach’s α coefficient of .88 (Bahn, Shin, Cho, & Hong, 2001).

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 is a 30-item self-report questionnaire that assesses impulsivity. Originally developed in 1959, it is currently in its 11th iteration. It assesses three facets of impulsivity – attentional, motor, and non-planning – which are also measured by the Korean version (Lee et al., 2012; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995). The BIS-11 displayed a fair internal consistency with the Cronbach’s α coefficient ranging from .79 to .83 (Patton et al., 1995).

Aggression Questionnaires (AQs)

The AQ consists of 29 items that assess physical aggression, verbal aggression, hostile aggression, and anger. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (uncharacteristic of me) to 5 (very characteristic of me) (Buss & Perry, 1992). The AQ showed a good internal consistency with the Cronbach’s α coefficient of .889 (Buss & Perry, 1992).

Anger Expression Scale (AX)

The AX is a 24-item scale with eight questions each on anger-in, anger-out, and anger-control (Spielberger et al., 1985). It measures the frequency of anger experience and expression, and has been validated for use in Korean populations with Cronbach’s α coefficient ranging from .73 to .81 (Hahn, Lee, & Chon, 1998).

Internet/smartphone usage patterns survey

We also investigated specific patterns of Internet usage. First, the adolescents indicated the age at which they started using the Internet, the nature of their Internet use, including games and other activities (e.g., social networking sites, messenger, and web surfing), and daily amount of Internet use on weekdays and weekends. They also indicated their reasons for using the Internet. We also determined the daily amount of time spent using smartphones on weekdays and weekends.

Data analyses

Data analyses were performed according to the following four steps: (a) descriptive statistics were generated to examine the overall distributions of the variables; (b) independent t-tests were performed to identify gender differences with regard to Internet and smartphone addiction; (c) LCA was used for classifying the subgroups (i.e., latent classes) based on the extent of Internet and smartphone addiction, for both males and females; and (d) analysis of variance (ANOVA) and χ2 tests were carried out to examine difference in variables, such as personality, clinical features, and usage patterns among the subgroups.

LCA is a statistical method to identify homogeneous classes, where the homogeneity may not be observed directly, but instead through latent variables derived from a series of observed variables (Hagenaars & McCutcheon, 2002). Because LCA assesses the probability of a given case belonging to a particular latent class, instead of making a prior assignment to a model, the method could be particularly useful for classifying new phenomena without potentially subjective biases from modeling, and may overcome the shortcomings associated with cluster analysis. In this study, we used log-transformed data in LCA for the normal approximation.

To determine the best-fitting LCA model, the following objectives of statistical indices were considered: the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), Akaike information criterion (AIC), and the Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT). The BIC and AIC are descriptive indices of the fitness of the LCA model, with lower values indicating a better fit. The BLRT statistically tests whether the current model (n classes) is better than the former model (n−1 classes). When the current model significantly fits better than the former model, the BLRT also yields a significant p value (Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007).

After determining the best-fitting model with the optimal number of classes, according to these reliable and widely used indices, cross-class comparisons were performed. Psychosocial and digital media-related variables, such as IAT and SAS scores, and electronic media usage survey data were compared among the latent classes. These interclass comparisons were carried out using ANOVA and χ2 tests for continuous and categorical values, respectively. When significant differences were found, post-hoc tests were performed using Tukey’s honest significant difference tests. The R software package was used for all statistical procedures (R Development Core Team, 2011). All analyses were conducted taking p < .05 as the level of significance.

Ethics

All subjects received an explanation about the research and provided written informed consent prior to participation. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Seoul St. Mary’s Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea (KC15EISI0103) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

For the LCA of all students (N = 555), a four-class model was determined as the best-fitting model according to the estimated BIC (Table 1).

Table 1.

Indicators of fit for models in the whole subjects (N = 555)

Model BIC AIC BLRT p value
Two classes 614.6392 571.4495 .001
Three classes 583.8394 519.0549 .001
Four classes 583.1744 496.7950 .001
Five classes 607.9873 500.0131 .390

Note. Bold value represents the lowest BIC value and signifies that the model composed by four class is most fit. AIC: Akaike information criterion; BIC: Bayesian information criterion; BLRT: Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test.

The four classes were as follows: (a) dual-problem group, demonstrating high severity for both Internet- and smartphone-related problems; (b) problematic Internet group, displaying high severity for Internet-related problems but low severity for smartphone-related problems; (c) problematic smartphone group, showing low severity for Internet-related problems but high severity for smartphone-related problems; and (d) a “healthy” group scoring low for both Internet- and smartphone-related problems (Figure 1).


          Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Latent class analysis of the whole subjects. SAS: Smartphone Addiction Scale; IAT: Internet Addiction Test

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 2; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.28

Post-hoc interclass comparisons revealed that the dual-problematic group was more likely to score higher on the Internet and smartphone problem questionnaires, as well as worse psychosocial outcomes in general (Table 2).

Table 2.

Interclass comparison of the whole subjects (N = 555)

Variables Dual (n = 275) (49.5%) Internet (n = 43) (7.7%) Smartphone (n = 178) (32.1%) Healthy (n = 59) (10.6%) p value F Post hoc
Psychosocial measures
 Internet Addiction Test 42.8 (12.0) 35.8 (9.8) 23.8 (2.8) 21.1 (1.2) <.001 203.9 D > I > S ≒ H
 Smartphone Addiction Scale 92.6 (25.1) 46.5 (4.3) 80.7 (23.5) 46.0 (4.0) <.001 108.4 D > S > I ≒ H
 BIS Scale 17.8 (3.6) 15.6 (3.5) 17.5 (3.7) 15.7 (4.1) <.001 8.6 D ≒ S > H ≒ I
 Total BAS Scale 30.1 (8.1) 26.5 (10.3) 27.5 (9.1) 24.6 (9.7) <.001 8.3 D > S ≒ H
 BAS-R 12.2 (3.5) 10.8 (4.3) 11.6 (4.0) 10.0 (4.3) <.001 6.3 D ≒ S > H
 BAS-DG 8.9 (2.7) 8.0 (3.2) 8.1 (3.0) 7.7 (3.4) .003 4.7 D > S ≒ H
 BAS-F 9.0 (2.8) 7.7 (3.2) 7.8 (2.9) 6.9 (3.0) <.001 11.3 D > S ≒ I ≒ H
 BDI 8.9 (8.2) 2.8 (3.6) 7.6 (7.6) 4.2 (6.9) <.001 11.6 D ≒ S > H ≒ I
 BAI 8.9 (9.7) 2.7 (4.1) 5.0 (7.2) 2.2 (4.3) <.001 18.2 D > S ≒ I ≒ H
 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 65.8 (7.2) 61.5 (7.7) 62.7 (8.3) 57.1 (9.8) <.001 21.4 D > S ≒ I > H
 CASS-S 21.7 (12.5) 14.3 (12.0) 15.1 (11.4) 11.5 (11.5) <.001 18.7 D > S ≒ I ≒ H
 AQ 70.4 (19.7) 52.4 (16.3) 60.1 (18.9) 48.0 (11.7) <.001 32.0 D > S ≒ I ≒ H, S > H
 AX 56.2 (10.3) 49.5 (12.9) 51.8 (11.1) 47.1 (12.1) <.001 15.2 D > S ≒ I ≒ H, S > H
Patterns survey
 Gaming hours per play (A) 1.8 (1.7) 2.3 (1.6) 0.9 (1.0) 0.6 (0.8) <.001 25.6 I ≒ D > S ≒ H
 Weekday daily gaming hours (A) 1.3 (1.5) 2.0 (1.9) 0.6 (1.0) 0.4 (0.9) <.001 23.1 I > D > S ≒ H
 Weekend daily gaming hours (A) 2.5 (2.5) 3.1 (2.9) 1.1 (1.5) 0.7 (1.3) <.001 25.2 I ≒ D > S ≒ H
 Internet café as the main gaming  place 0.2 (0.4) 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.3) 0.2 (0.4) .018 10.1 D > S
 Non-gaming Internet usage hours  per use (A) 1.5 (1.1) 1.2 (0.9) 1.4 (1.4) 0.9 (0.8) .004 4.4 D ≒ S > H
 Weekday daily non-gaming  Internet hours (A) 1.3 (1.2) 1.0 (0.8) 1.0 (1.0) 1.0 (1.5) .011 3.8 D > S
 Weekend daily non-gaming  Internet hours (A) 1.7 (1.5) 1.1 (1.1) 1.4 (1.4) 1.1 (1.1) .002 4.9 D > H
 Use Internet whenever possible 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4) 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.3) .002 14.5 D > S ≒ H
 Use smartphone whenever  possible 0.5 (0.5) 0.3 (0.4) 0.5 (0.5) 0.2 (0.4) <.001 26.0 D ≒ S > H, S > I

Note. A: average; AQ: Aggression Questionnaire; AX: Anger Expression Scale; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; BIS: Behavioral Inhibition System; BAS: Behavioral Activation System (R: reward responsiveness; DG: drive to pursuit goals; F: fun-seeking); CASS-S: Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale – short version; D: dual-problematic group; H: healthy group; I: Internet problem group; S: smartphone problem group.

The dual-problem group significantly demonstrated higher scores than the problematic Internet user group on the IAT (42.8 vs. 35.8, p < .001), SAS (92.6 vs. 46.5, p < .001), BIS (17.8 vs. 15.6, p = .002), BAS-F (9.0 vs. 7.7, p = .041), BDI (8.9 vs. 2.8, p < .001), BAI (8.9 vs. 2.7, p < .001), BIS-11 (65.8 vs. 61.5, p = .005), CASS-S (21.7 vs. 14.3, p = .002), AQ (70.4 vs. 52.4, p < .001), and AX (56.2 vs. 49.5, p = .002). However, there was no significant difference in the average number of hours spent gaming in one session, or the average number of hours spent gaming at the weekend, between the dual-problem group and the problematic Internet group.

The dual-problem group also significantly showed higher scores than the problematic smartphone group on the IAT (42.8 vs. 23.8, p < .001), SAS (92.6 vs. 80.7, p < .001), BAS (30.1 vs. 27.5, p = .013), BAS-D (8.9 vs. 8.1, p = .019), BAS-F (9.0 vs. 7.8, p < .001), BAI (8.9 vs. 5.0, p < .001), BIS-11 (65.8 vs. 62.7, p < .001), CASS-S (21.7 vs. 15.1, p < .001), AQ (70.4 vs. 60.1, p < .001), and AX (56.2 vs. 51.8, p < .001). In addition, the average hours spent gaming (1.8 vs. 0.9 hr, p < .001), average daily gaming hours on weekday (1.3 vs. 0.6 hr, p < .001) and weekend (2.5 vs. 1.1 hr, p < .001), hours of daily non-gaming Internet usage on weekdays (1.3 vs. 1.0 hr, p = .017), preference for Internet cafés as a gaming venue (0.2 vs. 0.1, p = .013), and positive response rate to the use of the Internet whenever possible (0.2 vs. 0.1, p = .010) were significantly greater in the dual group than the problematic smartphone group. However, the dual-problem and the problematic smartphone groups showed no statistical difference in BIS, BAS-R, or BDI scores, or in the number of hours of non-gaming Internet use or use of smartphone whenever possible in the post-hoc analysis.

The problematic Internet group played games for significantly longer time than the dual-problem group on average on weekdays (2.0 vs. 1.3 hr, p = .010). When compared with the problematic smartphone user group, the problematic Internet user group had significantly higher IAT scores (35.8 vs. 23.8, p < .001), a higher average number of hours spent gaming in one session (2.3 vs. 0.9 hr, p < .001), and a higher average number of hours spent gaming on weekdays (2.0 vs. 0.6 hr, p < .001) and at the weekends (3.1 vs. 1.1 hr, p < .001).

On the contrary, the problematic smartphone group scored significantly higher on the SAS (80.7 vs. 46.5, p < .001), BIS (17.5 vs. 15.6, p = .013), and BDI (7.6 vs. 2.8, p = .002), and were more likely to use smartphone whenever possible (0.5 vs. 0.3, p = .027) than the problematic Internet user group. No significant differences existed between the two groups for BAS-F, BAI, CASS-S, AQ, or AX scores.

As shown in Table 3, the male (n = 295) and female (n = 260) students differed significantly with respect to numerous variables related to gaming, and in terms of Internet and smartphone usage. However, the demographic variables of age and household income did not demonstrate significant differences between the genders. The males displayed higher alcohol consumption experience than the females (0.2% vs. 0.1%, p = .05), but smoking or psychiatric treatment histories did not differ by the gender. The females demonstrated more severe addiction than the males according to the SAS (85.9 vs. 74.9, respectively, p < .001), whereas males showed higher IAT scores than females (36.4 vs. 31.0, respectively, p < .001). The average daily number of hours of use, of the non-gaming Internet and smartphones on weekdays and at the weekend, was higher in females. On the contrary, the males on average spent more time gaming on weekdays and at weekends. While the duration of Internet use per session was significantly longer in females, individual gaming sessions were significantly longer in males. Significant differences in psychosocial variables were also observed between genders. Scores on the psychometric instruments were generally higher in females, except for the CASS-S, BIS-11, and AQ (Table 3).

Table 3.

Psychosocial and digital media-related variables of the participants

Mean (SD)
Characteristics Total Male Female p value
Demographic variables
 Age 13.9 (0.3) 13.9 (0.3) 13.9 (0.4) .214
Monthly household income .107
 <$940 22 (5.7) 11 (5.0) 11 (6.8)
 $940–1,880 32 (8.4) 17 (7.7) 15 (9.2)
 $1,880–2,820 57 (14.9) 37 (16.8) 20 (12.3)
 $2,820–3,760 84 (21.9) 47 (21.4) 37 (22.7)
 $3,760–4,700 53 (13.8) 24 (10.9) 29 (17.8)
 $4,700–5,640 46 (12.0) 30 (13.6) 16 (9.8)
 $5,640–6,580 33 (8.6) 15 (6.8) 18 (11.0)
 ≥$6,580 56 (14.6) 39 (17.7) 17 (10.4)
Alcohol (+)a 0.04 (0.2) 0.06 (0.2) 0.02 (0.1) .050
Smoking (+)a 0.02 (0.1) 0.02 (0.1) 0.01 (0.1) .614
Psychiatric treatment history (+)a 0.02 (0.1) 0.03 (0.2) 0.01 (0.1) .154
Psychosocial variables
 Behavioral Inhibition System 17.3 (3.7) 16.5 (3.3) 18.3 (3.9) <.001
 Behavioral Activation System Scales 28.4 (9.0) 27.5 (9.5) 29.4 (8.3) .017
 Beck Depression Inventory 7.5 (7.8) 5.7 (6.5) 9.5 (8.7) <.001
 Beck Anxiety Inventory 6.5 (8.6) 5.7 (7.6) 7.3 (9.5) .034
 CASS-S 17.9 (12.6) 18.3 (13.2) 17.4 (11.9) .422
 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 63.6 (8.3) 63.6 (7.9) 63.5 (8.8) .910
 Aggression Questionnaires 59.0 (20.0) 57.5 (21.0) 60.6 (18.6) .070
 State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory 53.3 (11.4) 51.9 (12.3) 54.8 (10.2) .003
Digital media-related variables
 Internet Addiction Test 33.9 (13.1) 36.4 (13.1) 31.0 (12.5) <.001
 Smartphone Addiction Scale 80.1 (28.0) 74.9 (24.2) 85.9 (30.7) <.001
 Weekday non-gaming Internet hours per day (A) 1.1 (1.1) 1.0 (0.9) 1.3 (1.4) <.001
 Weekend non-gaming Internet hours per day (A) 1.5 (1.4) 1.2 (1.1) 1.9 (1.7) <.001
 Non-gaming Internet usage hours per use 1.4 (1.2) 1.2 (1.2) 1.6 (1.2) <.001
 Weekday smartphone usage hours per day (A) 2.9 (2.5) 2.5 (2.0) 3.4 (2.8) <.001
 Weekend smartphone usage hours per day (A) 3.8 (3.0) 3.1 (2.5) 4.6 (3.3) <.001
 Weekday gaming hours per day (A) 1.0 (1.4) 1.5 (1.5) 0.5 (1.0) <.001
 Weekend gaming hours per day (A) 1.9 (2.3) 2.8 (2.2) 0.8 (1.8) <.001
 Gaming hours per play 1.4 (1.5) 2.0 (1.4) 0.7 (1.4) <.001

Note. A: average; CASS-S: Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale – short version; SD: standard deviation.

Frequency (%).

Reflecting the large gender differences, separate LCAs were performed for each gender. Unlike the four-class model for the whole group, a three-class model showed the best fit, according to the estimated BIC, for both the male and female students (Table 4).

Table 4.

Indicators of fit for models in the males and the females

Model BIC AIC BLRT p value
Males (N = 295)
 Two classes 330.7431 293.8733 .009
 Three classes 261.9221 206.6174 .001
 Four classes 268.2498 194.5103 .004
Females (N = 260)
 Two classes 250.5642 214.9573 .001
 Three classes 230.1625 176.7522 .001
 Four classes 240.8185 169.6049 .030

Note. Bold values represent the lowest BIC values and signify that the models composed by three class are most fit. AIC: Akaike information criterion; BIC: Bayesian information criterion; BLRT: Bootstrap Likelihood Ratio Test.

However, the classification of the three subtypes differed by gender. The males were classified as (a) dual-problem users, (b) problematic Internet users, and (c) healthy users. For the females, however, a problematic smartphone group was identified instead of a problematic Internet group (Figure 2).


          Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Latent class analysis of the males and the females. SAS: Smartphone Addiction Scale; IAT: Internet Addiction Test

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 2; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.28

Interclass comparisons of the male subgroups showed similar results to those of the whole-group model. The dual-problem group displayed more severe psychopathology and addictive behaviors than the problematic Internet and the healthy groups (Table 5). The problematic Internet group showed significantly higher IAT scores (30.6 vs. 20.2, p = .013), more hours of gaming per session (2.1 vs. 0.8 hr, p = .004), and more hours of gaming on weekdays (1.7 vs. 0.5 hr, p = .026) and at the weekends (2.7 vs. 1.0 hr, p = .025), on average, than the healthy group in post-hoc analyses.

Table 5.

Interclass comparison of the males (n = 295)

Variables Dual (n = 211) (71.5%) Internet (n = 71) (24.1%) Healthy (n = 13) (4.4%) p value F Post hoc
Psychosocial measures
 Internet Addiction Test 39.4 (13.0) 30.6 (10.0) 20.2 (0.4) <.001 26.3 D > I > H
 Smartphone Addiction Scale 86.2 (19.4) 48.4 (5.1) 42.2 (0.4) <.001 163.8 D > I ≒ H
 BIS Scale 17.0 (3.2) 15.3 (3.4) 15.8 (3.4) .001 6.9 D > I
 Total BAS Scale 28.6 (9.0) 25.0 (9.5) 23.9 (12.6) .008 4.9 D > I
 BAS-R 11.6 (3.7) 10.4 (4.3) 9.1 (4.7) .011 4.5 D > H
 BAS-DG 8.5 (2.9) 7.5 (3.2) 7.6 (4.3) .042 3.2 D > I
 BAS-F 8.5 (3.0) 7.2 (2.9) 7.2 (3.8) .002 6.2 D > I
 BDI 6.9 (6.9) 2.9 (4.0) 2.5 (6.1) <.001 12.1 D > I ≒ H
 BAI 7.1 (8.2) 2.7 (4.8) 0.9 (2.3) <.001 12.2 D > I ≒ H
 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 64.9 (7.3) 60.0 (8.3) 62.7 (10.0) <.001 10.8 D > I
 CASS-S 20.7 (13.1) 11.5 (9.8) 16.8 (18.6) <.001 13.7 D > I
 AQ 66.2 (21.1) 52.3 (14.8) 48.9 (15.3) <.001 16.3 D > I ≒ H
 AX 53.3 (11.6) 48.7 (12.6) 46.9 (15.8) .008 5.0 D > I
Patterns survey
 Gaming hours per play (A) 2.1 (1.4) 2.1 (1.4) 0.8 (1.0) .003 5.9 D ≒ I > H
 Weekday daily gaming hours (A) 1.5 (1.4) 1.7 (1.9) 0.5 (0.9) .034 3.4 I > H
 Weekend daily gaming hours (A) 3.0 (2.1) 2.7 (2.5) 1.0 (1.5) .007 5.0 D ≒ I > H
 Internet café as the main gaming place 0.3 (0.5) 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4) .029 7.1 D > I
 Non-gaming Internet usage hours per  use (A) 1.2 (1.3) 1.0 (0.9) 0.7 (0.6) .158 1.9
 Weekday daily non-gaming Internet  hours (A) 1.0 (0.9) 0.8 (0.8) 1.0 (0.9) .268 1.3
 Weekend daily non-gaming Internet  hours (A) 1.3 (1.2) 0.9 (1.0) 1.4 (1.2) .032 3.5 D > I
 Use Internet whenever possible 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.4) .466 1.5
 Use smartphone whenever possible 0.4 (0.5) 0.3 (0.5) 0.2 (0.4) .017 8.1 D > I

Note. A: average; AQ: Aggression Questionnaire; AX: Anger Expression Scale; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; BIS: Behavioral Inhibition System; BAS: Behavioral Activation System (R: reward responsiveness; DG: drive to pursuit goals; F: fun-seeking); CASS-S: Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale – short version; D: dual-problematic group; H: healthy group; I: Internet problem group.

In the female LCA model, the dual-problem group demonstrated higher scores on psychosocial measurements and addictive behaviors than the problematic smartphone and the healthy groups (Table 6). The problematic smartphone group scored significantly higher on the SAS (83.0 vs. 47.0, p < .001), BIS-11 (63.3 vs. 55.1, p < .001), and AQ (61.9 vs. 48.9, p < .001) than the healthy group. Subjects in the problematic smartphone group were also more likely to respond positively to the question regarding the use of a smartphone whenever possible than the healthy group in the post-hoc analysis (0.6 vs. 0.1, p < .001).

Table 6.

Interclass comparison of the females (n = 260)

Variables Dual (n = 35) (13.5%) Smartphone (n = 128) (49.2%) Healthy (n = 97) (37.3%) p value F Post hoc
Psychosocial measures
 Internet Addiction Test 42.9 (13.2) 24.6 (3.4) 21.4 (1.3) <.001 156.6 D > S ≒ H
 Smartphone Addiction Scale 104.2 (29.2) 83.0 (24.3) 47.0 (4.3) <.001 70.3 D > S > H
 BIS Scale 19.2 (3.9) 17.9 (3.7) 17.2 (4.4) .014 4.3 D > S ≒ H
 Total BAS Scale 31.6 (7.5) 28.4 (8.5) 26.9 (8.3) .003 5.9 D > S ≒ H
 BAS-R 12.9 (3.5) 11.9 (3.8) 11.2 (3.9) .038 3.3
 BAS-DG 9.3 (2.7) 8.3 (2.8) 8.5 (3.1) .032 3.5 D > S
 BAS-F 9.2 (2.7) 8.1 (2.9) 7.3 (2.8) .001 7.6 D > S ≒ H
 BDI 11.6 (9.1) 9.1 (8.2) 5.4 (7.8) .001 6.8 D > H
 BAI 10.9 (11.6) 5.7 (7.4) 3.1 (5.1) <.001 13.5 D > S ≒ H
 Barratt Impulsiveness 66.8 (7.2) 63.3 (8.2) 55.1 (9.7) <.001 26.9 D > S > H
 CASS-S 22.0 (11.9) 15.8 (11.4) 10.8 (8.5) <.001 14.9 D > S ≒ H
 AQ 74.0 (18.5) 61.9 (18.5) 48.9 (10.3) <.001 27.1 D > S > H
 AX 57.8 (9.7) 53.5 (10.1) 51.0 (9.7) <.001 8.1 D > S ≒ H
Patterns survey
 Gaming hours per play (A) 1.0 (1.9) 0.5 (1.0) 0.3 (0.7) .008 4.9 D > S ≒ H
 Weekday daily gaming hours (A) 0.8 (1.4) 0.3 (0.7) 0.3 (0.8) .001 7.1 D > S ≒ H
 Weekend daily gaming hours (A) 1.3 (2.4) 0.6 (1.2) 0.4 (1.0) .006 5.2 D > S ≒ H
 Internet café as the main gaming place 0.0 (0.2) 0.0 (0.2) 0.1 (0.2) .594 1.0
 Non-gaming Internet usage hours  per use (A) 2.0 (1.3) 1.4 (1.0) 1.1 (0.9) <.001 11.0 D > S ≒ H
 Weekday daily non-gaming Internet  hours (A) 1.7 (1.4) 1.1 (1.1) 1.1 (1.9) .001 6.7 D > S
 Weekend daily non-gaming Internet  hours (A) 2.4 (1.9) 1.6 (1.5) 1.2 (1.1) <.001 10.9 D > S ≒ H
 Use Internet whenever possible 0.2 (0.4) 0.1 (0.3) 0.0 (0.2) .052 5.9
 Use smartphone whenever possible 0.5 (0.5) 0.6 (0.5) 0.1 (0.3) <.001 20.0 S ≒ D > H

Note. A: average; AQ: Aggression Questionnaire; AX: Anger Expression Scale; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; BAI: Beck Anxiety Inventory; BIS: Behavioral Inhibition System; BAS: Behavioral Activation System (R: reward responsiveness; DG: drive to pursuit goals; F: fun-seeking); CASS-S: Conners-Wells’ Adolescent Self-Report Scale – short version; D: dual-problematic group; H: healthy group; S: smartphone problem group.

Discussion

In this study, we empirically showed that a four-class model most effectively categorized adolescents presenting with different degrees of problematic Internet and/or smartphone usage. The four classes generated by the LCA were as follows: (a) dual-problematic users, (b) problematic Internet users, (c) problematic smartphone users, and (d) “healthy” users. When compared with the healthy group, the dual-problem group demonstrated the most severe Internet- and smartphone-related problems. They also showed a poorer psychosocial profile in terms of anxiety, impulsivity, attention difficulties, and aggression.

The subclasses with problematic use in a single domain (Internet or smartphone) were generally intermediate between the dual-problem users and the healthy users in the psychosocial measures. However, despite their significantly lower IAT scores, the problematic Internet users showed a higher average number of hours spent gaming on weekdays than the dual-problem group.

When compared with the problematic smartphone group, the dual-problem group demonstrated significantly higher BAS-D and BAS-F scores, as well as anxiety, impulsivity, attention difficulty, and aggression. However, the sensitivity to punishment, such as BIS, BAS-R, and depression scores of the problematic smartphone group did not significantly differ from those of the dual-problem group.

Considering the gender differences in many psychosocial and digital media-related variables, LCA was separately performed for each gender. In contrast to the four-class model that showed the best fit for the whole group, three-class models provided the best fit for the male and female students. The males were categorized as (a) dual-problem users, (b) problematic Internet users, and (c) healthy users. The females were categorized as (a) dual-problem users, (b) problematic smartphone users, and (c) healthy users. Although both genders were best described by three-class models, the males included a subclass comprising only problematic Internet users, whereas the females included a subclass with only problematic smartphone use. These findings are consistent with a previous study that demonstrated similar variation in problematic digital media usage by gender (Mok et al., 2014).

A secondary objective of this study was to identify the underlying factors contributing to the gender differences. The females were more depressed, anxious, and sensitive to punishment and reward than the males. They also scored higher on the SAS and spent more time on smartphones than the males (Table 3). These findings are in line with the literature results. Mood and anxiety disorders, as well as problematic mobile phone use, have been reported to be higher among the females (Choi et al., 2015; Jiang & Zhao, 2016; Kessler et al., 2012; Steel et al., 2014; Takao, Takahashi, & Kitamura, 2009).

In the female LCA model, the dual-problem group showed more severe psychopathology and digital media-related problems. Furthermore, the problematic smartphone group had significantly higher levels of impulsivity, aggression, and smartphone addiction, and used smartphones more frequently than the healthy users. Although it was previously reported that females are more likely to utilize mobile phones for socializing purposes (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005), impulsivity and aggression, rather than sociability, may drive females to use their smartphones to a problematic degree. This is in line with a previous report, in which low self-control predicted problematic mobile phone use (Jiang & Zhao, 2016).

However, the males in this study scored higher on the IAT and played games for longer periods. Although the males demonstrated a lower level of psychopathology, in general, they did not show significant differences from the females in terms of attention difficulty, impulsivity, or aggression (Table 3).

Closer inspection of the three male LCA clusters revealed that the dual-problem group had a higher degree of psychopathology, and more severe digital media-related problems, including Internet addiction, than the problematic Internet group. However, the two groups did not significantly differ in terms of the average number of hours of gaming per session, or the average number of hours spent gaming at the weekend. Thus, gaming appeared as the most important factor contributing to the relative preponderance of problematic Internet usage in the males. This view is in line with the literature, in which gaming has been reported as a major risk factor for Internet addiction (Kormas, Critselis, Janikian, Kafetzis, & Tsitsika, 2011; Wu, Lee, Liao, & Chang, 2015).

This study indicated that individuals with both problematic Internet and smartphone usage may experience higher difficulties from excessive use of new digital media platforms and more psychosocial distress. Considering that depression and anxiety have been related to problematic Internet use, gaming, and smartphone use (Elhai, Dvorak, Levine, & Hall, 2017; Wei et al., 2012; Yen, Ko, Yen, Wu, & Yang, 2007), a higher level of depression and anxiety among our subjects was not a surprise. However, such a dose-dependent relationship between more severe psychosocial problems and a greater number of digital media-related problems warrants additional clinical attention to people experiencing a greater number of digital-media use problems. In addition, further research should reveal whether more severe psychosocial problems lead to greater numbers of problematic digital media use, or the synergistic effect of ‘dual-addictions’ exert greater negative consequences on mental health.

This study demonstrated gender differences in the problematic use of digital media. Although these gender differences were partially explained by differences in digital media use patterns and psychosocial burden by gender, further studies should investigate whether such differences stem from biological or sociocultural differences, such as expected gender roles or neurodevelopment.

One of this study’s interesting findings is the disproportionately overpresented dual-problem group in the males (71.5%) compared with the female students (13.5%). With increasing age, brain activity responsible for response inhibition was also reported to increase but with different pattern by gender (Rubia et al., 2013). Females displayed significantly higher activation in left superior and inferior frontal and striatal regions compared with males (Rubia et al., 2013). This finding hints that the females could have higher cognitive control than the males in the same age. Since higher impulsivity was observed for the dual problem group, the gender difference in brain maturation provides a plausible explanation for the relatively lower proportion of dual problems in the females than males (Ko, Yen, Chen, Chen, & Yen, 2005; Mihara & Higuchi, 2017).

The study results highlight that gaming is an important factor in males with Internet usage problems. It is also noteworthy that aggression and impulsivity were associated with problematic smartphone use among the females. Although aggression and impulsivity have been found to be associated with Internet addiction (Cao, Su, Liu, & Gao, 2007; Ko, Yen, Liu, Huang, & Yen, 2009; Koo & Kwon, 2014; Yen et al., 2007), our findings contradict a report stating that females use mobile phones to socialize (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005). Considering the higher level of fear among females (Tulviste, Kiive, Akkermann, & Harro, 2015), smartphone use may allow them to express their anger or hostility in a way that they regard as being socially more acceptable. If this hypothesis is valid, a longitudinal study should examine whether such use would ultimately represent an adaptive or maladaptive coping strategy. However, reverse causality is also possible, with aggression being the result of excessive media usage. Nevertheless, aggression is an important factor to address in clinical assessments, because it may affect the persistence of problematic use (Ko, Yen, Yen, Lin, & Yang, 2007).

This study had some limitations. First, the cross-sectional design allowed us to investigate only the current pattern of use and psychosocial variables of users, and their precise causal relationships could not be confirmed. For example, a problematic Internet user with high impulsivity could have become a pathological user due to his/her impulsivity, or vice versa. However, we cannot make inference on causality in this study; therefore, a longitudinal study is required. There were also limitations in terms of the lack of detailed information on the external environment of the subjects, such as parent–child and peer relationships. Furthermore, the psychosocial measurements relied on self-report measures with no objective assessments. However, because extensive psychosocial and digital media-related questionnaires were administered to the students, collecting too many details beyond the capacity that an adolescent could tolerate may actually undermine the overall study accuracy. Even though statistically higher scores in psychosocial measurements were displayed by the dual-problematic users, caution is required to interpret clinical significance. For instance, the mean values of BDI and BAI of this group were both 8.9 in the whole sample. Since the scores of 0–9 are regarded as normal, those average scores are situated in subclinical level (Beck, Steer, & Carbin, 1988; Julian, 2011). Sill, such subclinical distress by the dual excessive media usage may act as a background predisposing vulnerability and grant a clinical significance when the individual encounters other stressors in life. Considering that the study participants were not clinical sample, further studies in clinical populations are also required to ascertain the full clinical value of these findings.

In conclusion, this study revealed different patterns of problematic use of new digital media platforms among adolescents by gender. Four user subgroups were identified among the whole population. Although having dual-problem and healthy groups in common, distinct classes of problematic use, i.e., problematic Internet users versus problematic smartphone users, were identified in the males and the females, respectively, in the gender-stratified LCA. Gaming was associated with problematic Internet usage in males. On the contrary, higher levels of impulsivity and aggression were observed in the female problematic smartphone users; this group may use their smartphones as a means of releasing their impulsivity or frustration. Future research is required to confirm this hypothesis and to test whether such actions are useful for alleviating psychosocial burden or worsens it. Regardless of gender, in general, the dual-problem group demonstrated the most severe psychosocial and digital media-related problems. Thus, evaluating whether there are multiple types of addictive behavior in the context of digital media use could help us acquire a clearer clinical picture, similar to evaluating additional substance misuse behaviors among patients with a substance-use disorder.

Authors’ contribution

S-YL led the drafting of the manuscript and DL, CRN, SP, and J-GK also took part in drafting. DL, DYK, and YL conducted the analyses. Y-SK and J-SC developed the concept and supervised. DJK reviewed the scientific and further developed the concept. All authors contributed to the editorial comments.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

  • Achab, S. , Nicolier, M. , Mauny, F. , Monnin, J. , Trojak, B. , Vandel, P. , Sechter, D. , Gorwood, P. , & Haffen, E. (2011). Massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Comparing characteristics of addict vs non-addict online recruited gamers in a French adult population. BMC Psychiatry, 11(1), 144. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-144

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ayers, J. W. , Leas, E. C. , Dredze, M. , Allem, J. , Grabowski, J. G. , & Hill, L. (2016). Pokémon go – A new distraction for drivers and pedestrians. JAMA Internal Medicine, 176(12), 18651866. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6274

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bahn, G. H. , Shin, M. S. , Cho, S. C. , & Hong, K. E. (2001). A preliminary study for the development of the assessment scale for ADHD in adolescents: Reliability and validity for CASS(S). Korean Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12(2), 218224.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Epstein, N. , Brown, G. , & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.56.6.893

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77100. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Ward, C. H. , Mendelson, M. , Mock, J. , & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bianchi, A. , & Phillips, J. G. (2005). Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 8(1), 3951. doi:10.1089/cpb.2005.8.39

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Borsari, B. , Zamboanga, B. L. , Correia, C. , Olthuis, J. V. , Van Tyne, K. , Zadworny, Z. , Grossbard, J. R. , & Horton, N. J. (2013). Characterizing high school students who play drinking games using latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 38(10), 25322540. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.04.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buss, A. H. , & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452459. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cao, F. , Su, L. , Liu, T. , & Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. European Psychiatry, 22(7), 466471. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carras, M. C. , Van Rooij, A. J. , Van de Mheen, D. , Musci, R. , Xue, Q. L. , & Mendelson, T. (2017). Video gaming in a hyperconnected world: A cross-sectional study of heavy gaming, problematic gaming symptoms, and online socializing in adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 472479. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carver, C. S. , & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, S. W. , Kim, D. J. , Choi, J. S. , Ahn, H. , Choi, E. J. , Song, W. Y. , Kim, S. , & Youn, H. (2015). Comparison of risk and protective factors associated with smartphone addiction and Internet addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(4), 308314. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.043

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Conners, C. (1997). Conners’ Rating Scales – Revised technical manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

  • Dantlgraber, M. , Wetzel, E. , Schutzenberger, P. , Stieger, S. , & Reips, U. D. (2016). Simple construct evaluation with latent class analysis: An investigation of Facebook addiction and the development of a short form of the Facebook Addiction Test (F-AT). Behavior Research Methods, 48(3), 869879. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0716-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elhai, J. D. , Dvorak, R. D. , Levine, J. C. , & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251259. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eurostat. (2017). Digital economy and society statistics – Households and individuals. Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Digital_economy_and_society_statistics_-_households_and_individuals

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frangos, K. , Frangos, C. , & Sotiropoulos, I. (2012). A meta-analysis of the reliabilty of Young’s Internet Addiction Test (Vol. 1). London, UK: Proceedings of the World Congress on Engineering.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Galvan, A. , Hare, T. , Voss, H. , Glover, G. , & Casey, B. J. (2007). Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Science, 10(2), F8F14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00579.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. A. , Choo, H. , Liau, A. , Sim, T. , Li, D. , Fung, D. , & Khoo, A. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319e329. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-1353

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition & Emotion, 4(3), 269288. doi:10.1080/02699939008410799

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hagenaars, J. A. , & McCutcheon, A. L. (2002). Applied latent class analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • Hahn, D. W. , Lee, C. H. , & Chon, K. K . (1998). Korean adaptation of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI-K): The case of college students. Korean Journal of Health Psychology, 3(1), 1832.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. , Draghici, A. M. , & Sargent, J. D. (2012). A longitudinal study of risk-glorifying video games and reckless driving. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(4), 244253. doi:10.1037/a0029510

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jiang, Z. , & Zhao, X. (2016). Self-control and problematic mobile phone use in Chinese college students: The mediating role of mobile phone use patterns. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 416. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-1131-z

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Julian, L. J. (2011). Measures of anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis Care & Research, 63(S11), S467S472. doi:10.1002/acr.20561

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kessler, R. C. , Avenevoli, S. , Costello, E. J. , Georgiades, K. , Green, J. G. , Gruber, M. J. , He, J. P. , Koretz, D. , McLaughlin, K. A. , Petukhova, M. , Sampson, N. A. , Zaslavsky, A. M. , & Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemographic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(4), 372380. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.160

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, K. , Ryu, E. , Chon, M.-Y. , Yeun, E.-J. , Choi, S.-Y. , Seo, J.-S. , & Nam, B.-W. (2006). Internet addiction in Korean adolescents and its relation to depression and suicidal ideation: A questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 43(2), 185192. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2005.02.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, S. H. , Jung, S. , Park, K. , Jaekal, E. , Lee, S. H. , Choi, Y. , Lee, W. H. , & Choi, K. H. (2016). Development of the Korean screening tool for anxiety disorders: Review of current anxiety scales and development of preliminary item pools. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35(3), 630644. doi:10.15842/kjcp.2016.35.3.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kiraly, O. , Sleczka, P. , Pontes, H. M. , Urban, R. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2017). Validation of the Ten-Item Internet Gaming Disorder Test (IGDT-10) and evaluation of the nine DSM-5 Internet gaming disorder criteria. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 253260. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Yen, J. Y. , Chen, C. C. , Chen, S. H. , & Yen, C. F. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 193(4), 273277. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000158373.85150.57

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C.-H. , Yen, J.-Y. , Liu, S.-C. , Huang, C.-F. , & Yen, C.-F. (2009). The associations between aggressive behaviors and Internet addiction and online activities in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 44(6), 598605. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.11.011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Yen, J. Y. , Yen, C. F. , Lin, H. C. , & Yang, M. J. (2007). Factors predictive for incidence and remission of Internet addiction in young adolescents: A prospective study. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(4), 545551. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.9992

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koo, H. J. , & Kwon, J. H. (2014). Risk and protective factors of Internet addiction: A meta-analysis of empirical studies in Korea. Yonsei Medical Journal, 55(6), 16911711. doi:10.3349/ymj.2014.55.6.1691

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kormas, G. , Critselis, E. , Janikian, M. , Kafetzis, D. , & Tsitsika, A. (2011). Risk factors and psychosocial characteristics of potential problematic and problematic Internet use among adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 595. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-11-595

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kwon, M. , Lee, J. Y. , Won, W. Y. , Park, J. W. , Min, J. A. , Hahn, C. , Gu, X. , Choi, J. H. , & Kim, D. J. (2013). Development and validation of a Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS). PLoS One, 8(2), e56936. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056936

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, S.-R. , Lee, W.-H. , Park, J.-S. , Kim, S.-M. , Kim, J.-W. , & Shim, J.-H. (2012). The study on reliability and validity of Korean version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-revised in nonclinical adult subjects. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 51(6), 378386. doi:10.4306/jknpa.2012.51.6.378

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lemmens, J. S. , Valkenburg, P. M. , & Gentile, D. A. (2015). The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 567582. doi:10.1037/pas0000062

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lennon, A. , Oviedo-Trespalacios, O. , & Matthews, S. (2017). Pedestrian self-reported use of smart phones: Positive attitudes and high exposure influence intentions to cross the road while distracted. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 98, 338347. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2016.10.028

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Messias, E. , Castro, J. , Saini, A. , Usman, M. , & Peeples, D. (2011). Sadness, suicide, and their association with video game and Internet overuse among teens: Results from the youth risk behavior survey 2007 and 2009. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 41(3), 307315. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00030.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mihara, S. , & Higuchi, S. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal epidemiological studies of Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 425444. doi:10.1111/pcn.12532

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rhee, M. K. , Lee, Y. H. , Park, S. H. , Sohn, C. H. , Chung, Y. C. , Hong, S. K. , Son, C. H. , Jeong, Y. J. , Chang, P. , & Yoon, A. R. (1995). A standardization study of Beck Depression Inventory 1 – Korean Version (K-BDI): Reliability and factor analysis. The Korean Journal of Psychopathology, 4(1), 7795.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mok, J. Y. , Choi, S. W. , Kim, D. J. , Choi, J. S. , Lee, J. , Ahn, H. , Choi, E. J. , & Song, W. Y. (2014). Latent class analysis on Internet and smartphone addiction in college students. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 817828. doi:10.2147/ndt.s59293

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nylund, K. L. , Asparouhov, T. , & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535569. doi:10.1080/10705510701575396

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Patton, J. H. , Stanford, M. S. , & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768774. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6%3C768::AID-JCLP2270510607%3E3.0.CO;2-1

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rubia, K. , Lim, L. , Ecker, C. , Halari, R. , Giampietro, V. , Simmons, A. , Brammer, M. , & Smith, A. (2013). Effects of age and gender on neural networks of motor response inhibition: From adolescence to mid-adulthood. Neuroimage, 83, 690703. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.078

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumpf, H. J. , Vermulst, A. A. , Bischof, A. , Kastirke, N. , Gurtler, D. , Bischof, G. , Meerkerk, G. J. , John, U. , & Meyer, C. (2014). Occurrence of Internet addiction in a general population sample: A latent class analysis. European Addiction Research, 20(4), 159166. doi:10.1159/000354321

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spielberger, C. D. , Johnson, E. H. , Russell, S. F. , Crane, R. J. , Jacobs, G. A. , & Worden, T. J. (1985). The experience and expression of anger: Construction and validation of an Anger Expression Scale. In M. A. Chesney, & R. H. Rosenman (Eds.), Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders. New York, NY: Hemisphere/McGraw-Hills.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steel, Z. , Marnane, C. , Iranpour, C. , Chey, T. , Jackson, J. W. , Patel, V. , & Silove, D. (2014). The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 476493. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu038

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Takao, M. , Takahashi, S. , & Kitamura, M. (2009). Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(5), 501507. doi:10.1089/cpb.2009.0022

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tulviste, T. , Kiive, E. , Akkermann, K. , & Harro, J. (2015). Fears in the general population: More frequent in females and associated with the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and perceived relationship with mothers. Journal of Child Neurology, 30(11), 14591465. doi:10.1177/0883073815570151

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vandewater, E. A. , Shim, M.-S. , & Caplovitz, A. G. (2004). Linking obesity and activity level with children’s television and video game use. Journal of Adolescence, 27(1), 7185. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wei, H. T. , Chen, M. H. , Huang, P. C. , & Bai, Y. M. (2012). The association between online gaming, social phobia, and depression: An Internet survey. BMC Psychiatry, 12(1), 92. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-12-92

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Weizman, A. (2012). Emerging association between addictive gaming and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14(5), 590597. doi:10.1007/s11920-012-0311-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu, C. Y. , Lee, M. B. , Liao, S. C. , & Chang, L. R. (2015). Risk factors of Internet addiction among Internet users: An online questionnaire survey. PLoS One, 10(10), e0137506. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0137506

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu, X. , Tao, S. , Zhang, Y. , Zhang, S. , & Tao, F. (2015). Low physical activity and high screen time can increase the risks of mental health problems and poor sleep quality among Chinese college students. PLoS One, 10(3), e0119607. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0119607

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, J. Y. , Ko, C. H. , Yen, C. F. , Wu, H. Y. , & Yang, M. J. (2007). The comorbid psychiatric symptoms of Internet addiction: Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(1), 9398. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (1999). The research and controversy surrounding Internet addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 2(5), 381383. doi:10.1089/cpb.1999.2.381

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Achab, S. , Nicolier, M. , Mauny, F. , Monnin, J. , Trojak, B. , Vandel, P. , Sechter, D. , Gorwood, P. , & Haffen, E. (2011). Massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Comparing characteristics of addict vs non-addict online recruited gamers in a French adult population. BMC Psychiatry, 11(1), 144. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-144

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ayers, J. W. , Leas, E. C. , Dredze, M. , Allem, J. , Grabowski, J. G. , & Hill, L. (2016). Pokémon go – A new distraction for drivers and pedestrians. JAMA Internal Medicine, 176(12), 18651866. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6274

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bahn, G. H. , Shin, M. S. , Cho, S. C. , & Hong, K. E. (2001). A preliminary study for the development of the assessment scale for ADHD in adolescents: Reliability and validity for CASS(S). Korean Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12(2), 218224.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Epstein, N. , Brown, G. , & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893897. doi:10.1037//0022-006X.56.6.893

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Carbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77100. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beck, A. T. , Ward, C. H. , Mendelson, M. , Mock, J. , & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561571. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bianchi, A. , & Phillips, J. G. (2005). Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 8(1), 3951. doi:10.1089/cpb.2005.8.39

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Borsari, B. , Zamboanga, B. L. , Correia, C. , Olthuis, J. V. , Van Tyne, K. , Zadworny, Z. , Grossbard, J. R. , & Horton, N. J. (2013). Characterizing high school students who play drinking games using latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 38(10), 25322540. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.04.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buss, A. H. , & Perry, M. (1992). The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452459. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cao, F. , Su, L. , Liu, T. , & Gao, X. (2007). The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction in a sample of Chinese adolescents. European Psychiatry, 22(7), 466471. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carras, M. C. , Van Rooij, A. J. , Van de Mheen, D. , Musci, R. , Xue, Q. L. , & Mendelson, T. (2017). Video gaming in a hyperconnected world: A cross-sectional study of heavy gaming, problematic gaming symptoms, and online socializing in adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 472479. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carver, C. S. , & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, S. W. , Kim, D. J. , Choi, J. S. , Ahn, H. , Choi, E. J. , Song, W. Y. , Kim, S. , & Youn, H. (2015). Comparison of risk and protective factors associated with smartphone addiction and Internet addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(4), 308314. doi:10.1556/2006.4.2015.043

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Conners, C. (1997). Conners’ Rating Scales – Revised technical manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.

  • Dantlgraber, M. , Wetzel, E. , Schutzenberger, P. , Stieger, S. , & Reips, U. D. (2016). Simple construct evaluation with latent class analysis: An investigation of Facebook addiction and the development of a short form of the Facebook Addiction Test (F-AT). Behavior Research Methods, 48(3), 869879. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0716-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elhai, J. D. , Dvorak, R. D. , Levine, J. C. , & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251259. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.030

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eurostat. (2017). Digital economy and society statistics – Households and individuals. Retrieved from http://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php/Digital_economy_and_society_statistics_-_households_and_individuals

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frangos, K. , Frangos, C. , & Sotiropoulos, I. (2012). A meta-analysis of the reliabilty of Young’s Internet Addiction Test (Vol. 1). London, UK: Proceedings of the World Congress on Engineering.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Galvan, A. , Hare, T. , Voss, H. , Glover, G. , & Casey, B. J. (2007). Risk-taking and the adolescent brain: Who is at risk? Developmental Science, 10(2), F8F14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00579.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. A. , Choo, H. , Liau, A. , Sim, T. , Li, D. , Fung, D. , & Khoo, A. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319e329. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-1353

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition & Emotion, 4(3), 269288. doi:10.1080/02699939008410799

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hagenaars, J. A. , & McCutcheon, A. L. (2002). Applied latent class analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • Hahn, D. W. , Lee, C. H. , & Chon, K. K . (1998). Korean adaptation of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI-K): The case of college students. Korean Journal of Health Psychology, 3(1), 1832.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hull, J. G. , Draghici, A. M. , & Sargent, J. D. (2012). A longitudinal study of risk-glorifying video games and reckless driving. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(4), 244253. doi:10.1037/a0029510

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jiang, Z. , & Zhao, X. (2016). Self-control and problematic mobile phone use in Chinese college students: The mediating role of mobile phone use patterns. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 416. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-1131-z

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Julian, L. J. (2011). Measures of anxiety: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale-Anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis Care & Research, 63(S11), S467S472. doi:10.1002/acr.20561

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kessler, R. C. , Avenevoli, S. , Costello, E. J. , Georgiades, K. , Green, J. G. , Gruber, M. J. , He, J. P. , Koretz, D. , McLaughlin, K. A. , Petukhova, M. , Sampson, N. A. , Zaslavsky, A. M. , & Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemographic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(4), 372380. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.160

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, K. , Ryu, E. , Chon, M.-Y. , Yeun, E.-J. , Choi, S.-Y. , Seo, J.-S. , & Nam, B.-W. (2006). Internet addiction in Korean adolescents and its relation to depression and suicidal ideation: A questionnaire survey. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 43(2), 185192. doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2005.02.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, S. H. , Jung, S. , Park, K. , Jaekal, E. , Lee, S. H. , Choi, Y. , Lee, W. H. , & Choi, K. H. (2016). Development of the Korean screening tool for anxiety disorders: Review of current anxiety scales and development of preliminary item pools. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35(3), 630644. doi:10.15842/kjcp.2016.35.3.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kiraly, O. , Sleczka, P. , Pontes, H. M. , Urban, R. , Griffiths, M. D. , & Demetrovics, Z. (2017). Validation of the Ten-Item Internet Gaming Disorder Test (IGDT-10) and evaluation of the nine DSM-5 Internet gaming disorder criteria. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 253260. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Yen, J. Y. , Chen, C. C. , Chen, S. H. , & Yen, C. F. (2005). Gender differences and related factors affecting online gaming addiction among Taiwanese adolescents. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 193(4), 273277. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000158373.85150.57

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C.-H. , Yen, J.-Y. , Liu, S.-C. , Huang, C.-F. , & Yen, C.-F. (2009). The associations between aggressive behaviors and Internet addiction and online activities in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 44(6), 598605. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.11.011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ko, C. H. , Yen, J. Y. , Yen, C. F. , Lin, H. C. , & Yang, M. J. (2007). Factors predictive for incidence and remission of Internet addiction in young adolescents: A prospective study. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(4), 545551. doi:10.1089/cpb.2007.9992

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koo, H. J. , & Kwon, J. H. (2014). Risk and protective factors of Internet addiction: A meta-analysis of empirical studies in Korea. Yonsei Medical Journal, 55(6), 16911711. doi:10.3349/ymj.2014.55.6.1691

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kormas, G. , Critselis, E. , Janikian, M. , Kafetzis, D. , & Tsitsika, A. (2011). Risk factors and psychosocial characteristics of potential problematic and problematic Internet use among adolescents: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 595. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-11-595

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kwon, M. , Lee, J. Y. , Won, W. Y. , Park, J. W. , Min, J. A. , Hahn, C. , Gu, X. , Choi, J. H. , & Kim, D. J. (2013). Development and validation of a Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS). PLoS One, 8(2), e56936. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0056936

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, S.-R. , Lee, W.-H. , Park, J.-S. , Kim, S.-M. , Kim, J.-W. , & Shim, J.-H. (2012). The study on reliability and validity of Korean version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-revised in nonclinical adult subjects. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 51(6), 378386. doi:10.4306/jknpa.2012.51.6.378

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lemmens, J. S. , Valkenburg, P. M. , & Gentile, D. A. (2015). The Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 567582. doi:10.1037/pas0000062

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lennon, A. , Oviedo-Trespalacios, O. , & Matthews, S. (2017). Pedestrian self-reported use of smart phones: Positive attitudes and high exposure influence intentions to cross the road while distracted. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 98, 338347. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2016.10.028

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Messias, E. , Castro, J. , Saini, A. , Usman, M. , & Peeples, D. (2011). Sadness, suicide, and their association with video game and Internet overuse among teens: Results from the youth risk behavior survey 2007 and 2009. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 41(3), 307315. doi:10.1111/j.1943-278X.2011.00030.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mihara, S. , & Higuchi, S. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal epidemiological studies of Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 425444. doi:10.1111/pcn.12532

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rhee, M. K. , Lee, Y. H. , Park, S. H. , Sohn, C. H. , Chung, Y. C. , Hong, S. K. , Son, C. H. , Jeong, Y. J. , Chang, P. , & Yoon, A. R. (1995). A standardization study of Beck Depression Inventory 1 – Korean Version (K-BDI): Reliability and factor analysis. The Korean Journal of Psychopathology, 4(1), 7795.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mok, J. Y. , Choi, S. W. , Kim, D. J. , Choi, J. S. , Lee, J. , Ahn, H. , Choi, E. J. , & Song, W. Y. (2014). Latent class analysis on Internet and smartphone addiction in college students. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 817828. doi:10.2147/ndt.s59293

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nylund, K. L. , Asparouhov, T. , & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535569. doi:10.1080/10705510701575396

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Patton, J. H. , Stanford, M. S. , & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(6), 768774. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6%3C768::AID-JCLP2270510607%3E3.0.CO;2-1

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rubia, K. , Lim, L. , Ecker, C. , Halari, R. , Giampietro, V. , Simmons, A. , Brammer, M. , & Smith, A. (2013). Effects of age and gender on neural networks of motor response inhibition: From adolescence to mid-adulthood. Neuroimage, 83, 690703. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.078

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumpf, H. J. , Vermulst, A. A. , Bischof, A. , Kastirke, N. , Gurtler, D. , Bischof, G. , Meerkerk, G. J. , John, U. , & Meyer, C. (2014). Occurrence of Internet addiction in a general population sample: A latent class analysis. European Addiction Research, 20(4), 159166. doi:10.1159/000354321

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spielberger, C. D. , Johnson, E. H. , Russell, S. F. , Crane, R. J. , Jacobs, G. A. , & Worden, T. J. (1985). The experience and expression of anger: Construction and validation of an Anger Expression Scale. In M. A. Chesney, & R. H. Rosenman (Eds.), Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders. New York, NY: Hemisphere/McGraw-Hills.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steel, Z. , Marnane, C. , Iranpour, C. , Chey, T. , Jackson, J. W. , Patel, V. , & Silove, D. (2014). The global prevalence of common mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis 1980–2013. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 476493. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu038

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Takao, M. , Takahashi, S. , & Kitamura, M. (2009). Addictive personality and problematic mobile phone use. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(5), 501507. doi:10.1089/cpb.2009.0022

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tulviste, T. , Kiive, E. , Akkermann, K. , & Harro, J. (2015). Fears in the general population: More frequent in females and associated with the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and perceived relationship with mothers. Journal of Child Neurology, 30(11), 14591465. doi:10.1177/0883073815570151

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vandewater, E. A. , Shim, M.-S. , & Caplovitz, A. G. (2004). Linking obesity and activity level with children’s television and video game use. Journal of Adolescence, 27(1), 7185. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wei, H. T. , Chen, M. H. , Huang, P. C. , & Bai, Y. M. (2012). The association between online gaming, social phobia, and depression: An Internet survey. BMC Psychiatry, 12(1), 92. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-12-92

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A. , & Weizman, A. (2012). Emerging association between addictive gaming and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14(5), 590597. doi:10.1007/s11920-012-0311-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu, C. Y. , Lee, M. B. , Liao, S. C. , & Chang, L. R. (2015). Risk factors of Internet addiction among Internet users: An online questionnaire survey. PLoS One, 10(10), e0137506. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0137506

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wu, X. , Tao, S. , Zhang, Y. , Zhang, S. , & Tao, F. (2015). Low physical activity and high screen time can increase the risks of mental health problems and poor sleep quality among Chinese college students. PLoS One, 10(3), e0119607. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0119607

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, J. Y. , Ko, C. H. , Yen, C. F. , Wu, H. Y. , & Yang, M. J. (2007). The comorbid psychiatric symptoms of Internet addiction: Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41(1), 9398. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Young, K. S. (1999). The research and controversy surrounding Internet addiction. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 2(5), 381383. doi:10.1089/cpb.1999.2.381

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2024 0 419 57
Nov 2024 0 214 49
Dec 2024 0 168 62
Jan 2025 0 197 67
Feb 2025 0 193 52
Mar 2025 0 131 41
Apr 2025 0 0 0