Authors:
Svenja Orlowski Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Search for other papers by Svenja Orlowski in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Anja Bischof Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Search for other papers by Anja Bischof in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Bettina Besser Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Search for other papers by Bettina Besser in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Gallus Bischof Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Search for other papers by Gallus Bischof in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Hans-Jürgen Rumpf Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Lübeck, Lübeck, Germany

Search for other papers by Hans-Jürgen Rumpf in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

Deficits in emotion regulation (ER) are associated with mental disorders. To date, there are hardly any studies focusing on the role of ER strategies in the context of gambling behavior. The aim of this study was to investigate the association between specific ER strategies and pathological as well as problematic gambling in a proactively recruited sample.

Methods

A large and unselected sample (n = 4,928) has been screened proactively and systematically in vocational schools. We assessed the Affective Style Questionnaire to measure ER strategies and the Stinchfield questionnaire for assessing problematic and pathological gambling. Associations were investigated with linear and multinomial logistic regression analyses.

Results

The analyses showed a significant negative correlation between the subscales “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” and the Stinchfield sum score. Lower scores on these subscales were associated with a higher number of endorsed Stinchfield items. A lower score on the ER strategies “Adjusting” [conditional odds ratio (COR) = 0.95, confidence interval (CI) = 0.91–0.99] and “Tolerating” [COR = 0.95, CI = 0.92–99] led to a higher chance of being classified as a pathological gambler. In problematic gambling, on a subthreshold level, only “Tolerating” turned out to be significant [COR = 0.96, CI = 0.93–0.99].

Discussion and conclusions

For the first time, deficits in specific ER strategies were identified as independent risk factors for problematic and pathological gambling in a large and proactively recruited sample. ER skills, especially acceptance-focused strategies, should be considered in prevention and psychotherapy.

Abstract

Background and aims

Deficits in emotion regulation (ER) are associated with mental disorders. To date, there are hardly any studies focusing on the role of ER strategies in the context of gambling behavior. The aim of this study was to investigate the association between specific ER strategies and pathological as well as problematic gambling in a proactively recruited sample.

Methods

A large and unselected sample (n = 4,928) has been screened proactively and systematically in vocational schools. We assessed the Affective Style Questionnaire to measure ER strategies and the Stinchfield questionnaire for assessing problematic and pathological gambling. Associations were investigated with linear and multinomial logistic regression analyses.

Results

The analyses showed a significant negative correlation between the subscales “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” and the Stinchfield sum score. Lower scores on these subscales were associated with a higher number of endorsed Stinchfield items. A lower score on the ER strategies “Adjusting” [conditional odds ratio (COR) = 0.95, confidence interval (CI) = 0.91–0.99] and “Tolerating” [COR = 0.95, CI = 0.92–99] led to a higher chance of being classified as a pathological gambler. In problematic gambling, on a subthreshold level, only “Tolerating” turned out to be significant [COR = 0.96, CI = 0.93–0.99].

Discussion and conclusions

For the first time, deficits in specific ER strategies were identified as independent risk factors for problematic and pathological gambling in a large and proactively recruited sample. ER skills, especially acceptance-focused strategies, should be considered in prevention and psychotherapy.

Introduction

In recent years, the regulation of emotions has received increased interest in the context of psychotherapy and clinical–psychological research of mental health (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Berking & Wupperman, 2012; Gross & John, 2003). The number of publications in the field of emotion regulation (ER) and psychopathology has grown enormously. According to the definition by Gross (1998), ER refers to the process by which people influence which emotion they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express these emotions.

In accordance with the Process Model of ER (Gross & Thompson, 2007), antecedent-focused ER strategies and response-focused ER strategies can be distinguished. Antecedent-focused strategies refer to the subject’s behavior before the emotion response tendencies have become fully activated, such as situation selection, attention control, or cognitive concepts regarding the emotional situation (Gross & John, 2003; Gross & Thompson, 2007). Response-focused strategies, such as modification of the emotional response, refer to behavior that occurs after emotional response tendencies have already been generated and the emotion is underway (Gross & John, 2003). Three different styles of ER strategies have been identified consistently: concealing or suppression, adjusting or reappraisal, and tolerating (Hofmann & Kashdan, 2010).

Processes regulating emotions have a high relevance for mental health. The association between the use of dysfunctional ER strategies and psychopathology was described by several studies, and the concept of ER is part of many models of psychopathology (Aldao et al., 2010; Berking & Wupperman, 2012). These studies demonstrated that people suffering from psychiatric disorders often have difficulties with using functional and adaptive ER strategies (Aldao, Jazaieri, Goldin, & Gross, 2014; Aldao et al., 2010; Werner & Gross, 2010).

Although many psychiatric disorders such as the borderline personality disorder (Levine, Marziali, & Hood, 1997), major depression (Rottenberg, Gross, & Gotlib, 2005), anxiety disorder (Campell-Sills & Barlow, 2007), or eating disorders (Bydlowski et al., 2005; Harrison, Sullivan, Tchanturia, & Treasure, 2010) are associated with deficits in ER, only a few studies have investigated the relevance of ER in substance-related disorders. Theoretical approaches suggest that emotions play an essential role especially in the context of addictive disorders and should be incorporated into models of psychopathology. Khantzian (1985) postulated the self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders, assuming that people are using drugs to treat their negative emotions, which is one of today’s most popular models. In addition, numerous studies suggest that both positive and negative affects play an important role in the development and maintenance of substance-related disorders (Baker, Piper, McCarthy, Majeskie, & Fiore, 2004; Cheetham, Allen, Yucel, & Lubman, 2010; Kassel et al., 2007).

A variety of studies showed that, in addictive disorders, specific ER strategies could be observed. In a meta-analysis, Aldao et al. (2010) summarized studies of ER strategies for different psychiatric disorders including addictive disorders. They only found significant positive correlations between substance abuse and the two ER strategies “avoidance” and “rumination.” No significant correlations were found regarding further ER strategies, for example, expressive suppression.

Some studies have shown that people with substance-related disorders reveal deficits in ER compared to healthy controls, for example, in adaptive regulation of emotions and inability to tolerate negative emotions (Berking et al., 2011), strategies of emotional awareness and impulse control (Fox, Axelrod, Paliwal, Sleeper, & Sinha, 2007; Fox, Hong, & Sinha, 2008), and rumination (Nolen-Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007). On the contrary, the ER strategies “suppression of emotions” and “experiential avoidance” seem to be rather dysfunctional strategies (Levin et al., 2012; Wong et al., 2013). In the aforementioned studies, maladaptive ER strategies predicted alcohol consumption during therapy, increased substance-related problems, or relapse.

To date, most of the available studies regarding specific ER strategies and addiction are from the field of substance-related disorders, mainly based on alcohol-dependent patients. Studies in the field of gambling are scarce. Nevertheless, first studies focusing on pathological gambling looked at ER in general rather than the functional or dysfunctional use of specific ER strategies (Estevez, Jauregui, Sanchez-Marcos, Lopez-Gonzalez, & Griffiths, 2017; Kapsomenakis, Simos, Konstantakopoulos, & Kasselimis, 2018; Kopera et al., 2018). It was suggested that ER was predictive of substance-related and behavioral addictions (alcohol and drug abuse, gambling disorder, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use; Estevez et al., 2017).

Only few studies investigate the association between difficulties in specific ER strategies and pathological gambling. Navas, Verdejo-García, López-Gómez, Maldonado, and Perales (2016) explored the differences between gambling disorder patients and healthy controls in gambling-related cognitions and use of ER strategies, revealing that gambling disorder patients used “self-blame” and “catastrophizing,” but also “positive refocusing,” more often than controls. However, the study was limited as the sample consisted only of men. Williams, Grisham, Erskine, and Cassedy (2012) assessed the use of maladaptive and adaptive ER strategies in a sample of pathological gamblers. The study aimed at examining deficits in ER among individuals being treated in a specialist gambling clinic and to compare the use of strategies to a mixed clinical comparison group and a sample of healthy community controls. Findings show that pathological gamblers and patients of the clinical comparison group reported significantly fewer use of the ER strategy reappraisal, a greater lack of emotional clarity, and more impulse-control difficulties than individuals in the healthy control group. Compared to the healthy control group, pathological gamblers reported less emotional awareness. The authors concluded that pathological gamblers have difficulties in the use of various ER strategies, compared to clinical and healthy controls. Despite the valuable information this study provides, there are some limitations. The subjects were treatment-seeking, suggesting a greater awareness of their problems and higher severity of the disorder. Therefore, the results found in this study cannot be generalized. Furthermore, the authors exclusively included pathological gamblers in their study. However, it might be interesting to examine whether disturbed ER occurs in subthreshold cases as well.

Based on the mentioned theoretical assumptions and on the previous research on ER and addiction, the aim of this study was to investigate difficulties in three specific ER strategies in a proactively recruited vocational school sample of individuals with different types of gambling behavior including full-blown gambling disorder as well as subthreshold diagnoses. We hypothesized that a lower use of functional ER strategies and a higher use of maladaptive ER strategies are strongly related to gambling severity. Accordingly, we further hypothesized that pathological and problematic gamblers have more difficulties in the use of functional ER strategies compared to non-problematic gamblers. Less emotional awareness in pathological gamblers (Williams et al., 2012) might indicate problems in emotion recognition. This induces difficulties for adjusting or even tolerating emotions. Together with the findings of increased impulse-control difficulties in pathological gamblers (Williams et al., 2012) and in other addictive behavior (Fox et al., 2007, 2008), pathological gambling might be used as a behavior to relief from emotional tension increases. As concealing emotions might be not functional, we hypothesized that pathological and maybe problematical gamblers increasingly conceal their negative emotions.

Methods

Participants and procedure

This study was part of the project “Migration and gambling: investigation of the development in a longitudinal study” (MIGUEL) conducted with a sample of students of vocational schools in the federal state Schleswig-Holstein (Germany) between July 2016 and March 2017. Data in this study were collected proactively, which means every student in the classroom was approached systematically with questionnaires by trained study staff. The aim of the survey was to screen for subjects eligible for a diagnostic interview on risk and protective factors for problematic and pathological gambling. This analysis focuses on the screening sample. The questionnaires were handed out to the participants by trained interviewers in the classroom. Because every student was asked to participate, the data were not preselected. Answering all questions took about 20–30 min. A total of 6,781 participants completed the questionnaire, 63 of the screenings had to be excluded due to insufficient knowledge of the German language or double participation, leaving a sample of 6,718 valid screenings. Of those, 1,790 had missing data in one of the variables that have been included in the analyses as independent variables. These were excluded from the analyses, leaving a final sample of 4,928 valid cases.

Measures

Gambling

A German translation of the Stinchfield self-reporting questionnaire was utilized for the classification of pathological or problematic gambling (Stinchfield, 2002). Because it had been developed based on DSM-IV, we modified the instrument to fit DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) criteria by removing the “illegal acts” criterion. Later, the questionnaire included 17 items. Every DSM-5 criterion was assessed by two items, except criterion 2 “craving,” which was measured with only one item. The instrument is provided with excellent internal consistency in a general population-based sample (α = .81), in a sample of treatment-seeking individuals (α = .77) as well as in the combination of the two samples (α = .97) (Stinchfield, 2002, 2003). In this study, the instrument showed a good internal consistency with Cronbach’s α .87. Lesieur and Blume (1987) assessed the convergent validity of the questionnaire with the South Oaks Gambling Scale as reference and proved it with r = .77 (p < .01) in the general population-based sample and with r = .75 (p < .01) in the sample of treatment-seeking individuals satisfactory. The number of criteria based on DSM-5 criteria were calculated for defining groups (see “Statistical analyses” section).

Emotion regulation

In this study, we decided to use the Affective Style Questionnaire (ASQ; Hofmann & Kashdan, 2010), which includes three strategies of ER. In addition to the antecedent-focused strategy “Concealing” and the response-focused strategy “Adjusting,” the ASQ includes the ER strategy “Tolerating.” The questionnaire consists of 20 items measuring individual differences in ER. Each of these three ER strategies is being measured by one subscale: The scale “Concealing” includes eight items regarding avoiding and concealing aspects of emotions (e.g., “I am good at hiding my feelings”). The subscale “Adjusting” includes seven items on skills that influence and modify one’s own emotions by cognitive strategies (e.g., “I can avoid getting upset by taking a different perspective on things”). A tolerating or accepting style of dealing with emotions is assessed by the subscale “Tolerating” that refers to a high emotional stress tolerance (e.g., “It’s OK to feel negative emotions at times”).

In this study, the German version of the ASQ was used. Graser et al. (2012) translated and validated the ASQ with a German sample of students. Based on the results of factor analyses, the authors decided to rearrange two items on the scale “Adjusting.” Thus, the German version of the ASQ assessed the ER strategies “Concealing” with nine items, “Adjusting” with five items, and “Tolerating” with six items. The authors found a satisfactory (to good) internal consistency for the three subscales. Cronbach’s α was .84 for “Adjusting,” .75 for “Concealing,” and .72 for “Tolerating.” In our sample, Cronbach’s α was .79 for “Adjusting,” .79 for “Concealing,” and .69 for “Tolerating.”

Mental health

The brief questionnaire “Mental Health Inventory” (MHI-5; Berwick et al., 1991) was used to assess mental health. This short instrument consists of five items and was derived from the 38-item Mental Health Inventory. The MHI-5 includes items referring to positive and negative aspects of mental health, and items referring to depression and anxiety (Berwick et al., 1991). With a Cronbach’s α .74, the internal consistency of the MHI-5 is satisfactory, especially for depression and anxiety (Rumpf, Meyer, Hapke, & John, 2001). High values refer to a better mental health.

Alcohol use

We used the “Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption” (AUDIT-C) to assess problematic alcohol consumption, alcohol abuse, and alcohol addiction (Bush, Kivlahan, McDonell, Fihn, & Bradley, 1998). The short version of the AUDIT includes the first three items of the 10 items original test and provides information on drinking amount and frequency. Sensitivity and specificity proved to be good (Reinert & Allen, 2002). In addition, test–retest reliability was high (Dybek et al., 2006). High values refer to higher alcohol consumption.

Statistical analyses

To investigate the association between the three ER strategies and gambling, a linear regression analysis was conducted for dimensional analyses first and then multinomial logistic regression analyses for categorical analyses. For the identification of gambling-related problems, a sum score of the Stinchfield questionnaire was calculated. Individuals who fulfilled four or more criteria were classified as pathological gamblers according to DSM-5 (APA, 2013). In addition, we classified a subthreshold category for individuals who did not meet enough criteria for a diagnosis of dependence: Problematic gambling is defined by meeting two or three criteria. The third category was described as non-problematic gamblers who met one criterion or none at all. Based on these definitions, we classified 4,440 individuals as non-problematic gamblers, 285 as problematic gamblers, and 203 as pathological gamblers. According to the hypotheses, we expected lower scores for the ASQ subscales “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” and a higher score for “Concealing” for pathological gamblers compared to non-problematic gamblers, which may already be evident in subthreshold cases of problematic gamblers.

For ER strategies, sum scores of each subscale were calculated to analyze differences on the three subscales of the ASQ (Concealing, Adjusting, and Tolerating). Missing values in the ASQ have been replaced by the series mean. All analyses have been adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics and risk factors. The regression coefficient [Exp (B)] was interpreted in terms of a conditional odds ratio (COR; Gould, 2000) in all multinomial logistic regression analyses.

Associations of lower mental health (Aldao et al., 2010; Gross & Munoz, 1995) as well as alcohol use (Berking et al., 2011) and dysfunctional ER can consistently be found throughout the ER literature. Furthermore, it has been previously shown that male sex and migration proved as risk factors for problematic and pathological gambling behavior (Petry, Armentano, Kuoch, Norinth, & Smith, 2003) and studies suggest gender differences in the use of ER strategies (McRae, Ochsner, Mauss, Gabrieli, & Gross, 2008; Nolen-Hoeksema & Aldao, 2011). Therefore, we checked possible confounding variables by Spearman’s correlations in advance. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 (Armonk, NY, USA).

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The local ethics committee of the University of Lübeck approved MIGUEL (reg. no. 16-102). All subjects were informed about the study and the participation was voluntary. Informed consent was not necessary because study participation was anonymous. This procedure was in accordance with the local ethics committee and the Ministry of Education of Schleswig-Holstein.

Results

Participants’ characteristics

The sample included 4,928 subjects of which 62.8% (n = 3,096) were male and 37.2% (n = 1,832) were female. The mean age was 19.9 [standard deviation (SD) = 3.72; range 15–52] and 20.8% (n = 1,023) of the individuals had a migration background (MB), meaning that either the individuals themselves or at least one of their parents were born outside of Germany.

Analyses for the 12-month prevalence are based on the items of the Stinchfield questionnaire. On average, individuals affirmed 1.22 (SD = 1.44) Stinchfield items in the Stinchfield questionnaire. The 12-month prevalence for pathological gambling (≥4 DSM-5 criteria) was 4.1% (n = 203; CI = 3.6–4.7) and for problematic gambling (2–3 DSM-5 criteria) 5.8% (n = 285; CI = 5.2–6.4). The sum score of the DSM-5 criteria was positively correlated with the subscale “Concealing” (r = .070; p < .001) and negatively correlated with the subscale “Tolerating” (r = −.060; p < .001). There was no correlation with the subscale “Adjusting.” An overview of the descriptive statistics of the variables can be found in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample description of the total sample and of the three subgroups

Feature Total (N = 4,928) Non-problematic gamblers (n = 4,440) Problematic gamblers (n = 285) Pathological gamblers (n = 203)
Age [M (SD)] 19.90 (3.7) 19.92 (3.8) 19.51 (2.7) 20.00 (2.8)
Gender [n (%)]
 Male 3,096 (62.8) 1,798 (40.5) 260 (91.2) 194 (95.6)
 Female 1,832 (37.2) 2,642 (59.5) 25 (8.8) 9 (4.4)
Migration background [n (%)]
 Yes 1,023 (20.8) 850 (19.1) 92 (32.3) 81 (39.9)
 No 3,905 (79.2) 3,590 (80.9) 193 (67.7) 122 (60.1)
Stinchfield sum score [M (SD)] 1.22 (2.4) 0.19 (0.4) 2.73 (0.8) 7.49 (3.1)
DSM criteria sum score [M (SD)] 0.43 (1.3) 0.06 (0.3) 2.44 (0.5) 5.60 (1.6)
MHI-5 sum score [M (SD)] 6.68 (3.4) 6.68 (3.4) 6.27 (3.3) 7.26 (3.8)
AUDIT-C sum score [M (SD)] 4.38 (2.5) 4.20 (2.4) 5.74 (2.4) 6.29 (2.6)
ASQ subscale sum score [M (SD)]
 Concealing 29.24 (6.1) 29.12 (6.0) 29.95 (6.3) 30.87 (6.3)
 Adjusting 16.42 (4.1) 16.43 (4.1) 16.62 (4.0) 15.85 (4.6)
 Tolerating 20.58 (4.1) 20.68 (4.0) 19.87 (4.3) 19.40 (4.6)

Note. n: valid values; M: mean; SD: standard deviation; Non-problematic gamblers: 0–1 DSM-5 criteria; Problematic gamblers: 2–3 DSM-5 criteria; Pathological gamblers: ≥4 DSM-5 criteria; MHI-5: Mental Health Inventory; AUDIT-C: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption; ASQ: Affective Style Questionnaire.

Controlling for confounding variables

The MHI-5 was positively correlated with the ER strategy “Adjusting” (r = .461; p < .001) and “Tolerating” (r = .222; p < .001). Therefore, a better use of these strategies is associated with better mental health. Furthermore, the analyses yielded a positive correlation between AUDIT-C and the ER strategy “Concealing” (r = .091; p < .001) and a negative correlation with “Tolerating” (r = −.043; p = .002), although the correlations were low. The higher the alcohol consumption, the more people concealed their negative emotions and the less they tolerated them.

Concerning sociodemographic characteristics, a Mann–Whitney U test showed significant differences between male and female subjects on the subscales “Concealing” (Mmale = 30.14, SD = 5.92; Mfemale = 28.34, SD = 6.22) with p < .001 and “Adjusting” (Mmale = 17.27, SD = 4.00; Mfemale = 15.17, SD = 4.09) with p < .001. Furthermore, there were significant differences between subjects with and without MB for the subscales “Concealing” (MMB = 30.00, SD = 6.20; MNoMB = 29.23, SD = 6.05) with p < .001 and “Tolerating” (MMB = 19.75, SD = 4.16; MNoMB = 20.67, SD = 4.05) with p < .001. Due to these findings, the variables sex, MB, AUDIT-C sum score, and MHI-5 sum score were included in the linear and multinomial regression analyses to control the influence of these factors.

Dimensional analysis

The results of the linear regression analysis are shown in Table 2. After adjusting the data for the covariates, there was no significant association between affirmed Stinchfield items and the subscale “Concealing” in any model. However, the analysis still revealed a significant correlation between the Stinchfield sum score and the subscales “Adjusting” (p = .014) and “Tolerating” (p = .001). A lower score on these subscales is related to a higher number of affirmed Stinchfield items.

Table 2.

Linear regression analysis with Stinchfield sum score as dependent variable

B SE (Bs) β p [95% CI]
Concealing Scale 0.007 0.008 0.018 .392 [−0.009 to 0.024]
Sex 1.063 0.127 0.185 <.001 [0.814 to 1.312]
MB 1.044 0.125 0.175 <.001 [0.800 to 1.289]
AUDIT-C 0.137 0.021 0.138 <.001 [0.096 to 0.179]
MHI-5 0.102 0.016 0.137 <.001 [0.071 to 0.133]
Adjusting Scale −0.035 0.014 −0.058 .014 [−0.063 to −0.007]
Sex 1.129 0.127 0.197 <.001 [0.880 to 1.378]
MB 1.049 0.124 0.176 <.001 [0.805 to 1.293]
AUDIT-C 0.136 0.021 0.137 <.001 [0.095 to 0.178]
MHI-5 0.083 0.018 0.111 <.001 [0.048 to 0.118]
Tolerating Scale −0.041 0.013 −0.069 .001 [−0.067 to −0.016]
Sex 1.069 0.125 0.187 <.001 [0.824 to 1.315]
MB 1.020 0.124 0.171 <.001 [0.776 to 1.264]
AUDIT-C 0.133 0.021 0.134 <.001 [0.092 to 0.175]
MHI-5 0.090 0.016 0.121 <.001 [0.058 to 0.122]

Note. Sex: 0 = female, 1 = male; Migration background (MB): 0 = no, 1 = yes; AUDIT-C: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption (score: 0–11); MHI-5: Mental Health Inventory (score: 0–20); B: regression coefficient; SE: standard error; CI: confidence interval.

Categorical analyses

We performed separate multinomial logistic regression analyses for each subscale of the ASQ and regression coefficients (interpreted as CORs).

Table 3 shows the results of the subscale “Concealing.” Having included the covariates, no significant group differences were found, but a statistical tendency could be observed between pathological and non-problematic gamblers (p = .084).

Table 3.

Multinomial logistic regression analysis for the ER strategy “Concealing” among non-problematic, problematic, and pathological gamblers

  Problematic gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. problematic gamblersa
Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value
Concealing 1.00 [0.98–1.02] .872 1.02 [1.00–1.05] .084 1.02 [0.99–1.05] .200
Sex 5.71 [3.72–8.76] <.001 13.04 [6.57–25.89] <.001 2.28 [1.03–5.08] .043
MB 2.10 [1.61–2.75] <.001 2.74 [2.02–3.73] <.001 1.31 [0.89–1.91] .169
AUDIT-C 1.23 [1.17–1.29] <.001 1.33 [1.25–1.41] <.001 1.09 [1.01–1.17] .029
MHI-5 1.01 [0.97–1.05] .673 1.10 [1.06–1.15] <.001 1.09 [1.04–1.15] .001

Note. Sex: 0 = female, 1 = male; Migration background (MB): 0 = no, 1 = yes; AUDIT-C: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption (score: 0–11); MHI-5: Mental Health Inventory (score: 0–20); CI: confidence interval; ER: emotion regulation.

aThe last of the two compared categories is always the reference category. bExp (B) = regression coefficients are interpreted in terms of conditional odds ratios.

The results of the subscale “Adjusting” are shown in Table 4. No significant differences in CORs could be found between problematic gamblers and non-problematic gamblers, but a statistical tendency could be observed (p = .079). The comparison of pathological gamblers with non-problematic gamblers revealed a significant lower COR for the subscale sum score (p = .011). Subjects were more likely to be classified as a pathological gambler when they scored lower on the subscale “Adjusting.” There were no significant differences between the groups of pathological and problematic gamblers.

Table 4.

Multinomial logistic regression analysis for the ER strategy “Adjusting” among non-problematic, problematic, and pathological gamblers

Problematic gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. problematic gamblersa
Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value
Adjusting 0.97 [0.94–1.00] .079 0.95 [0.91–0.99] .011 0.98 [0.93–1.03] .400
Sex 5.95 [3.87–9.15] <.001 14.29 [7.19–28.40] <.001 2.40 [1.08–5.35] .032
MB 2.12 [1.62–2.77] <.001 2.79 [2.05–3.79] <.001 1.31 [0.90–1.92] .158
AUDIT-C 1.22 [1.17–1.29] <.001 1.33 [1.25–1.41] <.001 1.09 [1.01–1.17] .026
MHI-5 0.99 [0.95–1.04] .730 1.08 [1.03–1.13] .002 1.08 [1.02–1.15] .008

Note. Sex: 0 = female, 1 = male; Migration background (MB): 0 = no, 1 = yes; AUDIT-C: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption (score: 0–11); MHI-5: Mental Health Inventory (score: 0–20); CI: confidence interval; ER: emotion regulation.

aThe last of the two compared categories is always the reference category. bRegression coefficients are interpreted in terms of conditional odds ratios.

Table 5 shows the results of the third subscale “Tolerating.” Comparing problematic with non-problematic gamblers, the data revealed a significant lower COR for the subscale sum score (p = .005). Comparing pathological with non-problematic gamblers, the COR was significantly lower, even when all other covariates were controlled (p = .010). In both cases, a lower score on the subscale “Tolerating” led to a higher possibility to be classified as a problematic or as a pathological gambler. No significant differences could be identified for this ER strategy by comparing problematic with pathological gamblers.

Table 5.

Multinomial logistic regression analysis for the ER strategy “Tolerating” among non-problematic, problematic, and pathological gamblers

Problematic gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. non-problematic gamblersa Pathological gamblers vs. problematic gamblersa
Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value Exp (B)b [95% CI] p value
Tolerating 0.96 [0.93–0.99] .005 0.95 [0.92–0.99] .010 1.00 [0.95–1.04] .876
Sex 5.69 [3.71–8.73] <.001 13.32 [6.71–26.43] <.001 2.34 [1.05–5.20] .037
MB 2.06 [1.58–2.70] <.001 2.71 [2.00–3.68] <.001 1.31 [0.90–1.92] .159
AUDIT-C 1.22 [1.16–1.28] <.001 1.33 [1.25–1.41] <.001 1.09 [1.01–1.17] .027
MHI-5 1.00 [0.96–1.04] .847 1.09 [1.04–1.14] <.001 1.09 [1.04–1.15] .001

Note. Sex: 0 = female, 1 = male; Migration background (MB): 0: = no, 1 = yes; AUDIT-C: Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test – Consumption (score: 0–11); MHI-5: Mental Health Inventory (score: 0–20); CI: confidence interval; ER: emotion regulation.

aThe last of the two compared categories is always the reference category. bRegression coefficients are interpreted in terms of conditional odds ratios.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine ER strategies in a systematically and proactively recruited sample of gamblers. The aim of this study was to examine deficits in the use of functional ER strategies in a large and unselected sample of young individuals. We compared the use of three different ER strategies between self-reported pathological, problematic, and non-problematic gamblers.

As hypothesized, we found significant associations between self-reported gambling-related problems and ER strategies. The negative correlation between the ER strategies “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” and the number of affirmed Stinchfield-items suggests that a lack or a maladaptive use of these two strategies is associated with the number of DSM-5 symptoms. Even when data were adjusted for sociodemographic characteristics, mental health and alcohol use, the significant effect of these two ER strategies on gambling behavior was stable.

Furthermore, the multinomial logistic regression analyses provided detailed information on group differences for specific ER strategies. Concerning response-focused strategies, there was no significant difference in the use of the ER strategy “Concealing” between the groups. Even though there was no significant difference between pathological and non-problematic gamblers, a statistical tendency could be observed, and the descriptive statistic revealed that individuals with a higher score met more DSM-5 criteria. As there was no significance, we could not confirm our hypothesis. However, pathological gamblers tend to suppress their emotions more than non-pathological gamblers. This finding is in line with other studies showing that concealing or suppressing emotions is dysfunctional to regulate emotions (Levin et al., 2012; Wong et al., 2013).

Gross and John (2003) demonstrated that reappraisal is a much more functional strategy to regulate emotions than suppression. “Adjusting” means cognitively adapting to a new situation in order to change the emotional appraisal of the situation before an emotional response is activated, similar to reappraising emotions. Our results show that subjects who reported lower skills in this antecedent-focused strategy were more likely to be classified as pathological gamblers, which confirm our initial hypothesis. Indeed, no group difference could be found in our data between problematic gamblers on a subthreshold level (<4 DSM-5 criteria) and non-problematic gamblers. This finding supports the suggestion that difficulties in regulating emotions in advance by use of this cognitive strategy could be a risk factor especially for pathological gambling. On the other hand, the functional use of “Adjusting” was identified to be an independent protective factor for pathological gambling.

The data revealed group differences between problematic and non-problematic gamblers and pathological and non-problematic gamblers regarding the ability to tolerate own negative emotions. Individuals with problematic and/or pathological gambling behavior reported less use of the ER strategy “Tolerating,” so the hypothesis could be confirmed. The lack of an accepting attitude toward negative emotions could be identified as a risk factor to be a problematic or pathological gambler, even when the influence of other possible factors was controlled. No difference for the ability to tolerate negative emotions between individuals with the diagnosis of a gambling disorder and individuals meeting the criteria on a subthreshold level could be shown. Therefore, one could suggest that acceptance might be a helpful strategy to prevent gambling-related problems on a subthreshold level. Tolerating the own emotions reduces the probability to show pathological gambling behavior. In addition, lacking this ability increases the probability to have gambling-related problems. These findings are similar to the results by Berking et al. (2011), who identified the lack of tolerance as the most important risk factor for relapse in alcohol-dependent patients. The comparison between individuals classified as pathological gamblers and non-problematic gamblers demonstrates that a maladaptive use of the ER strategies “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” is a risk factor for pathological gambling. Furthermore, “Tolerating” is an independent risk factor even on an advanced level of gambling-related problems. The results of both the dimensional as well as the categorical analyses suggest that the skill to tolerate own negative emotions is a very constant protective factor for gambling-related problems as well as for meeting the DSM-5 criteria for a gambling disorder. Having a comfort and accepting attitude to arousing emotional experience and a strong tolerance of distress seemed to be associated with less criteria for gambling-related problems.

Interestingly, we found a difference between individuals with and without a MB regarding their ability to regulate emotions functionally. The described significant differences were stable in any regression model and they could not be better explained by other factors. The studies found a strong association between gambling disorder and MB (Petry et al., 2003) that cannot be explained by sociodemographic characteristics and gambling preferences (Kastirke, Rumpf, John, Bischof, & Meyer, 2014). Besides a significantly higher prevalence rate of pathological gambling in the population of individuals with a MB, this study identified that individuals with a MB showed lesser use of the strategies “Adjusting” and “Tolerating” and an increased use of the strategy “Concealing” compared to individuals without a MB.

Limitations

Despite many advantages, some limitations of this study have to be mentioned. Although the large sample consisted of proactively recruited unselected participants, our results refer to vocational students only and can thus not be generalized to the general population.

Furthermore, the study is limited by the use of the Stinchfield questionnaire that is a self-reporting instrument, which could possibly overestimate the prevalence of pathological gambling (Stinchfield, 2002).

Another point of discussion is the ASQ, which covers three different strategies of regulating emotions and which was chosen as measurement for ER because of its comprehensiveness and good internal consistence. We have used the validated German version of the ASQ, which could confirm the factor structure with only minor changes concerning two items (Graser et al., 2012). Although it has been used in different studies, there is still no available validation of the German translation of the ASQ on the basis of a clinical sample or with other populations. Furthermore, the ASQ concentrates on the regulation of negative emotions exclusively, disregarding the regulation of positive emotions as an important aspect of psychopathological models of addiction disorders (Cheetham et al., 2010). In their review, Cheetham et al. (2010) postulated that a high positive affect plays a major role especially in the early stages of an addictive disorder, whereas a low affect becomes relevant in later stages. On that account, further investigations are needed with regard to the regulation of positive emotions in the population of gamblers as well.

Furthermore, due to limited time to complete the survey, we were unable to include a questionnaire for measuring depression specifically. We therefore used the MHI-5 as a brief instrument to measure mental health.

One requirement for participation and filling out the questionnaires was a sufficient knowledge of the German language, so we could not include migrants with insufficient language skills. However, we are unsure whether all participants understood each question correctly.

Conclusion and implications for clinical practice

This study provides important new aspects regarding the association of ER strategies and pathological gambling. Our findings highly suggest that training of adaptive ER strategies is a candidate treatment that could potentially be integrated in psychotherapy for patients with gambling problems. Therefore, it will be necessary to further prove its effectiveness in a randomized controlled study and to investigate whether pathological gamblers could benefit from a therapeutic intervention that improves their ER skills. One possible treatment option could be the Dialectical Behavioral Therapy (DBT) developed by Linehan (2018). DBT is a form of cognitive behavioral therapy and works toward supporting patients to increase their ER abilities. A skill training for ER could be a helpful concept for individuals with a behavioral addiction like pathological gambling. Another option could be the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006), which integrates acceptance, mindfulness, and behavior-change strategies as psychological interventions to increase psychological flexibility. We found that tolerating negative emotions plays an important role even on a subthreshold level. Therefore, the data support the inclusion of a training into prevention measures that aim to develop a tolerating attitude toward unpleasant emotions in order to intervene at an early stage of the development of gambling-related problems

Authors’ contribution

SO: data gathering, statistical analysis, interpretation of findings, and preparation of manuscript draft. AB and GB: study concept and design, statistical analysis, and interpretation of findings. BB: Data gathering. H-JR: study supervision, study concept and design, and obtained funding. All authors had full access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • Aldao, A. , Jazaieri, H. , Goldin, P. R. , & Gross, J. J. (2014). Adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies: Interactive effects during CBT for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(4), 382389. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aldao, A. , Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association [APA]. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baker, T. B. , Piper, M. E. , McCarthy, D. E. , Majeskie, M. R. , & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111(1), 3351. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berking, M. , Margraf, M. , Ebert, D. , Wupperman, P. , Hofmann, S. G. , & Junghanns, K. (2011). Deficits in emotion-regulation skills predict alcohol use during and after cognitive-behavioral therapy for alcohol dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 307318. doi:10.1037/a0023421

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berking, M. , & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: Recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128134. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503669

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berwick, D. M. , Murphy, J. M. , Goldman, P. A. , Ware, J. E. , Barsky, A. J. , & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance of a 5-item Mental-Health Screening-Test. Medical Care, 29(2), 169176. doi:10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bush, K. , Kivlahan, D. R. , McDonell, M. B. , Fihn, S. D. , & Bradley, K. A. (1998). The AUDIT Alcohol Consumption Questions (AUDIT-C). An effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158(16), 17891795. doi:10.1001/archinte.158.16.1789

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bydlowski, S. , Corcos, M. , Jeammet, P. , Paterniti, S. , Berthoz, S. , Laurier, C. , Chambry, J. , & Consoli, S. M. (2005). Emotion-processing deficits in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37(4), 321329. doi:10.1002/eat.20132

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Campell-Sills, L. , & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorparating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542559). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cheetham, A. , Allen, N. B. , Yucel, M. , & Lubman, D. I. (2010). The role of affective dysregulation in drug addiction. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 621634. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dybek, I. , Bischof, G. , Grothues, J. , Reinhardt, S. , Meyer, C. , Hapke, U. , John, U. , Broocks, A. , Hohagen, F. , & Rumpf, H.-J. (2006). The reliability and validity of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) in a German general practice population sample. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(3), 473481. doi:10.15288/jsa.2006.67.473

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Estevez, A. , Jauregui, P. , Sanchez-Marcos, I. , Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 534544. doi:10.1556/2006.6.2017.086

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fox, H. C. , Axelrod, S. R. , Paliwal, P. , Sleeper, J. , & Sinha, R. (2007). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 89(2–3), 298301. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fox, H. C. , Hong, K. A. , & Sinha, R. (2008). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 33(2), 388394. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gould, W. (2000). Interpreting logistic regression in all its forms. Stata Technical Bulletin, 53, 1929.

  • Graser, J. , Bohn, C. , Kelava, A. , Schreiber, F. , Hofmann, S. G. , & Stangier, U. (2012). The Affective Style Questionnaire (ASQ): German adaptation and validity. Diagnostica, 58(2), 100111. doi:10.1026/0012-1924/a000056

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271299. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & Munoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental-health. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice, 2(2), 151164. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harrison, A. , Sullivan, S. , Tchanturia, K. , & Treasure, J. (2010). Emotional functioning in eating disorders: Attentional bias, emotion recognition and emotion regulation. Psychological Medicine, 40(11), 18871897. doi:10.1017/S0033291710000036

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, S. C. , Luoma, J. B. , Bond, F. W. , Masuda, A. , & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 125. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hofmann, S. G. , & Kashdan, T. B. (2010). The Affective Style Questionnaire: Development and psychometric properties. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(2), 255263. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9142-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kapsomenakis, A. , Simos, P. G. , Konstantakopoulos, G. , & Kasselimis, D. S. (2018). In search of executive impairment in pathological gambling: A neuropsychological study on non-treatment seeking gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(4), 13271340. doi:10.1007/s10899-018-9758-y

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kassel, J. D. , Veilleux, J. C. , Wardle, M. C. , Yates, M. C. , Greenstein, J. E. , Evatt, D. P. , & Roesch, L. L. (2007). Negative affect and addiction. In M. Al’Absi (Ed.), Stress and addiction: Biological and psychological mechanisms (pp. 171189). London, UK: Elsevier Science.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kastirke, N. , Rumpf, H. J. , John, U. , Bischof, A. , & Meyer, C. (2014). Demographic risk factors and gambling preference may not explain the high prevalence of gambling problems among the population with migration background – Results from a German nationwide survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31(3), 7417577. doi:10.1007/s10899-014-9459-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khantzian, E. J. (1985). The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders – Focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142(11), 12591264. doi:10.1176/ajp.142.11.1259

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kopera, M. , Trucco, E. M. , Jakubczyk, A. , Suszek, H. , Michalska, A. , Majewska, A. , Szejko, N. , Łoczewska, A. , Krasowska, A. , Klimkiewicz, A. , Brower, K. J. , Zucker, R. A. , & Klimkiewicz, A. (2018). Interpersonal and intrapersonal emotional processes in individuals treated for alcohol use disorder and non-addicted healthy individuals. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 813. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lesieur, H. R. , & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS) – A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 11841188. doi:10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levin, M. E. , Lillis, J. , Seeley, J. , Hayes, S. C. , Pistorello, J. , & Biglan, A. (2012). Exploring the relationship between experiential avoidance, alcohol use disorders, and alcohol-related problems among first-year college students. Journal of American College Health, 60(6), 443448. doi:10.1080/07448481.2012.673522

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levine, D. , Marziali, E. , & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185(4), 240246. doi:10.1097/00005053-199704000-00004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Linehan, M. M. (2018). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • McRae, K. , Ochsner, K. N. , Mauss, I. B. , Gabrieli, J. J. , & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(2), 143162. doi:10.1177/1368430207088035

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Navas, J. F. , Verdejo-García, A. , López-Gómez, M. , Maldonado, A. , & Perales, J. C. (2016). Gambling with rose-tinted glasses on: Use of emotion-regulation strategies correlates with dysfunctional cognitions in gambling disorder patients. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 271281. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.040

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , & Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 704708. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , Stice, E. , Wade, E. , & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 198207. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M. , Armentano, C. , Kuoch, T. , Norinth, T. , & Smith, L. (2003). Gambling participation and problems among South East Asian refugees to the United States. Psychiatric Services, 54(8), 11421148. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.54.8.1142

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reinert, D. F. , & Allen, J. P. (2002). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): A review of recent research. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 26(2), 272279. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02534.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rottenberg, J. , Gross, J. J. , & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 627639. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumpf, H.-J. , Meyer, C. , Hapke, U. , & John, U. (2001). Screening for mental health: Validity of the MHI-5 using DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders as gold standard. Psychiatry Research, 105(3), 243253. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00329-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2002). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS). Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 119. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00158-1

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2003). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of a measure of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 180182. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.180

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Werner, K. , & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A conceptual framework. In A. M. Kring & D. M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 1337). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, A. D. , Grisham, J. R. , Erskine, A. , & Cassedy, E. (2012). Deficits in emotion regulation associated with pathological gambling. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(2), 223238. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02022.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wong, C. F. , Silva, K. , Kecojevic, A. , Schrager, S. M. , Bloom, J. J. , Iverson, E. , & Lankenau, S. E. (2013). Coping and emotion regulation profiles as predictors of nonmedical prescription drug and illicit drug use among high-risk young adults. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 132(1–2), 165171. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.01.024

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aldao, A. , Jazaieri, H. , Goldin, P. R. , & Gross, J. J. (2014). Adaptive and maladaptive emotion regulation strategies: Interactive effects during CBT for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(4), 382389. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.03.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aldao, A. , Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association [APA]. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baker, T. B. , Piper, M. E. , McCarthy, D. E. , Majeskie, M. R. , & Fiore, M. C. (2004). Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review, 111(1), 3351. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berking, M. , Margraf, M. , Ebert, D. , Wupperman, P. , Hofmann, S. G. , & Junghanns, K. (2011). Deficits in emotion-regulation skills predict alcohol use during and after cognitive-behavioral therapy for alcohol dependence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 307318. doi:10.1037/a0023421

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berking, M. , & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: Recent findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25(2), 128134. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503669

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berwick, D. M. , Murphy, J. M. , Goldman, P. A. , Ware, J. E. , Barsky, A. J. , & Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance of a 5-item Mental-Health Screening-Test. Medical Care, 29(2), 169176. doi:10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bush, K. , Kivlahan, D. R. , McDonell, M. B. , Fihn, S. D. , & Bradley, K. A. (1998). The AUDIT Alcohol Consumption Questions (AUDIT-C). An effective brief screening test for problem drinking. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158(16), 17891795. doi:10.1001/archinte.158.16.1789

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bydlowski, S. , Corcos, M. , Jeammet, P. , Paterniti, S. , Berthoz, S. , Laurier, C. , Chambry, J. , & Consoli, S. M. (2005). Emotion-processing deficits in eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37(4), 321329. doi:10.1002/eat.20132

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Campell-Sills, L. , & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorparating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542559). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cheetham, A. , Allen, N. B. , Yucel, M. , & Lubman, D. I. (2010). The role of affective dysregulation in drug addiction. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(6), 621634. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dybek, I. , Bischof, G. , Grothues, J. , Reinhardt, S. , Meyer, C. , Hapke, U. , John, U. , Broocks, A. , Hohagen, F. , & Rumpf, H.-J. (2006). The reliability and validity of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) in a German general practice population sample. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(3), 473481. doi:10.15288/jsa.2006.67.473

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Estevez, A. , Jauregui, P. , Sanchez-Marcos, I. , Lopez-Gonzalez, H. , & Griffiths, M. D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 534544. doi:10.1556/2006.6.2017.086

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fox, H. C. , Axelrod, S. R. , Paliwal, P. , Sleeper, J. , & Sinha, R. (2007). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control during cocaine abstinence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 89(2–3), 298301. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.026

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fox, H. C. , Hong, K. A. , & Sinha, R. (2008). Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 33(2), 388394. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gould, W. (2000). Interpreting logistic regression in all its forms. Stata Technical Bulletin, 53, 1929.

  • Graser, J. , Bohn, C. , Kelava, A. , Schreiber, F. , Hofmann, S. G. , & Stangier, U. (2012). The Affective Style Questionnaire (ASQ): German adaptation and validity. Diagnostica, 58(2), 100111. doi:10.1026/0012-1924/a000056

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271299. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348362. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & Munoz, R. F. (1995). Emotion regulation and mental-health. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice, 2(2), 151164. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gross, J. J. , & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harrison, A. , Sullivan, S. , Tchanturia, K. , & Treasure, J. (2010). Emotional functioning in eating disorders: Attentional bias, emotion recognition and emotion regulation. Psychological Medicine, 40(11), 18871897. doi:10.1017/S0033291710000036

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, S. C. , Luoma, J. B. , Bond, F. W. , Masuda, A. , & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(1), 125. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hofmann, S. G. , & Kashdan, T. B. (2010). The Affective Style Questionnaire: Development and psychometric properties. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 32(2), 255263. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9142-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kapsomenakis, A. , Simos, P. G. , Konstantakopoulos, G. , & Kasselimis, D. S. (2018). In search of executive impairment in pathological gambling: A neuropsychological study on non-treatment seeking gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 34(4), 13271340. doi:10.1007/s10899-018-9758-y

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kassel, J. D. , Veilleux, J. C. , Wardle, M. C. , Yates, M. C. , Greenstein, J. E. , Evatt, D. P. , & Roesch, L. L. (2007). Negative affect and addiction. In M. Al’Absi (Ed.), Stress and addiction: Biological and psychological mechanisms (pp. 171189). London, UK: Elsevier Science.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kastirke, N. , Rumpf, H. J. , John, U. , Bischof, A. , & Meyer, C. (2014). Demographic risk factors and gambling preference may not explain the high prevalence of gambling problems among the population with migration background – Results from a German nationwide survey. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31(3), 7417577. doi:10.1007/s10899-014-9459-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khantzian, E. J. (1985). The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders – Focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 142(11), 12591264. doi:10.1176/ajp.142.11.1259

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kopera, M. , Trucco, E. M. , Jakubczyk, A. , Suszek, H. , Michalska, A. , Majewska, A. , Szejko, N. , Łoczewska, A. , Krasowska, A. , Klimkiewicz, A. , Brower, K. J. , Zucker, R. A. , & Klimkiewicz, A. (2018). Interpersonal and intrapersonal emotional processes in individuals treated for alcohol use disorder and non-addicted healthy individuals. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 813. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lesieur, H. R. , & Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS) – A new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144(9), 11841188. doi:10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levin, M. E. , Lillis, J. , Seeley, J. , Hayes, S. C. , Pistorello, J. , & Biglan, A. (2012). Exploring the relationship between experiential avoidance, alcohol use disorders, and alcohol-related problems among first-year college students. Journal of American College Health, 60(6), 443448. doi:10.1080/07448481.2012.673522

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levine, D. , Marziali, E. , & Hood, J. (1997). Emotion processing in borderline personality disorders. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 185(4), 240246. doi:10.1097/00005053-199704000-00004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Linehan, M. M. (2018). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • McRae, K. , Ochsner, K. N. , Mauss, I. B. , Gabrieli, J. J. , & Gross, J. J. (2008). Gender differences in emotion regulation: An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal. Group processes & Intergroup Relations, 11(2), 143162. doi:10.1177/1368430207088035

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Navas, J. F. , Verdejo-García, A. , López-Gómez, M. , Maldonado, A. , & Perales, J. C. (2016). Gambling with rose-tinted glasses on: Use of emotion-regulation strategies correlates with dysfunctional cognitions in gambling disorder patients. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 271281. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.040

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , & Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(6), 704708. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. , Stice, E. , Wade, E. , & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 198207. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M. , Armentano, C. , Kuoch, T. , Norinth, T. , & Smith, L. (2003). Gambling participation and problems among South East Asian refugees to the United States. Psychiatric Services, 54(8), 11421148. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.54.8.1142

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reinert, D. F. , & Allen, J. P. (2002). The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): A review of recent research. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 26(2), 272279. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02534.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rottenberg, J. , Gross, J. J. , & Gotlib, I. H. (2005). Emotion context insensitivity in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(4), 627639. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.627

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumpf, H.-J. , Meyer, C. , Hapke, U. , & John, U. (2001). Screening for mental health: Validity of the MHI-5 using DSM-IV Axis I psychiatric disorders as gold standard. Psychiatry Research, 105(3), 243253. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(01)00329-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2002). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS). Addictive Behaviors, 27(1), 119. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00158-1

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2003). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of a measure of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 180182. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.180

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Werner, K. , & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A conceptual framework. In A. M. Kring & D. M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 1337). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, A. D. , Grisham, J. R. , Erskine, A. , & Cassedy, E. (2012). Deficits in emotion regulation associated with pathological gambling. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51(2), 223238. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02022.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wong, C. F. , Silva, K. , Kecojevic, A. , Schrager, S. M. , Bloom, J. J. , Iverson, E. , & Lankenau, S. E. (2013). Coping and emotion regulation profiles as predictors of nonmedical prescription drug and illicit drug use among high-risk young adults. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 132(1–2), 165171. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.01.024

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jul 2024 0 25 15
Aug 2024 0 48 12
Sep 2024 0 60 6
Oct 2024 0 283 9
Nov 2024 0 115 10
Dec 2024 0 72 14
Jan 2025 0 17 5