Authors:
Stephanie Antons General Psychology: Cognition and Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany

Search for other papers by Stephanie Antons in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
and
Brand Matthias General Psychology: Cognition and Center for Behavioral Addiction Research (CeBAR), University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany
Erwin L. Hahn Institute for Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Essen, Germany

Search for other papers by Brand Matthias in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Diminished control over a specific behavior is a core characteristic in addictive behaviors such as problematic Internet-pornography (IP) use. First studies suggest that a hyperactivity of the impulsive system is one reason for impulsive behaviors in the context of problematic IP use. The tripartite-process theory of addiction explains neurocognitive mechanisms beyond common dual-process theories in addictive behaviors. However, the role of the reflective and interoceptive system is still unresolved.

Methods

The study comprised a stop-signal task (SST) including neutral and pornographic images during fMRI and questionnaires to investigate associations between symptoms of problematic IP use, craving, and neural activity of the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive system. We examined 28 heterosexual males with varying symptom severity of problematic IP use.

Results

Data indicates that individuals with more symptoms of problematic IP use showed better performance in the SST which was linked to decreased insula and inferior frontal gyrus activity during pornographic image processing. An increase in craving was associated with lower activity of the ventral striatum during pornographic image processing. The interoceptive system showed varying effects. Increased insula activity during inhibitory control and decreased activity during pornographic image processing were associated with higher inhibitory control performance.

Discussion and Conclusion

Effects of tolerance and motivational aspects may explain the better inhibitory control performance in individuals with higher symptom severity which was associated with differential activity of the interoceptive and reflective system. Diminished control over IP use presumably results from the interaction between the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive systems.

Abstract

Background and aims

Diminished control over a specific behavior is a core characteristic in addictive behaviors such as problematic Internet-pornography (IP) use. First studies suggest that a hyperactivity of the impulsive system is one reason for impulsive behaviors in the context of problematic IP use. The tripartite-process theory of addiction explains neurocognitive mechanisms beyond common dual-process theories in addictive behaviors. However, the role of the reflective and interoceptive system is still unresolved.

Methods

The study comprised a stop-signal task (SST) including neutral and pornographic images during fMRI and questionnaires to investigate associations between symptoms of problematic IP use, craving, and neural activity of the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive system. We examined 28 heterosexual males with varying symptom severity of problematic IP use.

Results

Data indicates that individuals with more symptoms of problematic IP use showed better performance in the SST which was linked to decreased insula and inferior frontal gyrus activity during pornographic image processing. An increase in craving was associated with lower activity of the ventral striatum during pornographic image processing. The interoceptive system showed varying effects. Increased insula activity during inhibitory control and decreased activity during pornographic image processing were associated with higher inhibitory control performance.

Discussion and Conclusion

Effects of tolerance and motivational aspects may explain the better inhibitory control performance in individuals with higher symptom severity which was associated with differential activity of the interoceptive and reflective system. Diminished control over IP use presumably results from the interaction between the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive systems.

Introduction

The unregulated or problematic use of Internet pornography (IP) is characterized by an intense urge to use IP, a diminished control over the use, and a continued use of IP despite experiencing negative consequences (Wéry & Billieux, 2017). Prevalence estimates from Australia and Europe vary between 0.1 and 5% for women and 2–13% for males, and indicate that especially males seem to be affected (Ballester-Arnal, Castro Calvo, Gil-Llario, & Gil-Julia, 2017; Rissel et al., 2017; Ross, Mansson, & Daneback, 2012). These estimates presumably show a high variance since there are no official diagnostic criteria for problematic IP use up to now. Furthermore, there is an ongoing debate if and under which category it should be classified (Gola & Potenza, 2018; Kraus & Sweeney, 2018; Walton & Bhullar, 2018). Problematic IP use often appears as one manifestation of compulsive sexual behavior (Reid et al., 2012) which was currently included in 11th revision of the International Classification of Diseases for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics in the section of impulse control disorders (code: 6C72; World Health Organization, 2019). Some authors, however, emphasize phenomenological and neurobiological parallels to disorders due to addictive behaviors (Kraus et al., 2018; Stark, Klucken, Potenza, Brand, & Strahler, 2018; Wéry & Billieux, 2017). Regardless of the discrimination between both classifications in the ICD-11, both - impulse control disorders and behavioral addictions - share not only common phenomenological but also neurobiological similarities with substance-use disorders such as the critical role of the imbalance between the impulsive (mainly ventral striatum, amygdala) and reflective brain system (mainly dorsolateral and inferior prefrontal cortex) (Bechara, 2005; Brett, Anton, Valabregue, & Poline, 2002; Zheng et al., 2019). Therefore, the current study aimed at investigating the role of the reflective, impulsive, and interoceptive system in the context of problematic IP use.

In the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model (Brand et al., 2019) it is argued that the imbalance between neural systems in early stages of addictive behaviors is particularly related to a hyperactive impulsive system. Mechanisms of affective and cognitive biases, incentive sensitization as well as cue-reactivity and craving are considered to be associated with this hyperactivity and reinforce each other within the addiction process (also see Bechara, 2005; Berridge & Robinson, 2016; Volkow & Baler, 2014). In later stages of addictive behaviors, the reflective system may continuously lose control over the impulsive system and the specific behaviors may become habitual despite experiencing negative consequences due to the addictive behaviors (Brand et al., 2019). The I-PACE model differentiates between general and stimuli-specific inhibitory control, depending upon the stages of addictive behaviors (Brand et al., 2019). Recent data suggests that while a general reduction in inhibitory control might be a vulnerability factor for the development of addictive behaviors (Meng, Deng, Wang, Guo, & Li, 2015), reductions of stimuli-specific inhibitory control may become additionally relevant in later stages of the disorder when affected individuals experience increasing reductions in control over their IP usage (Nie, Zhang, Chen, & Li, 2016; van Holst et al., 2012; Yao et al., 2015). On a neural level it has been demonstrated that cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions is linked to neural activity in striatal regions, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and cingulate cortex – regions associated with both the impulsive and the reflective system (Starcke, Antons, Trotzke, & Brand, 2018; cf; Wei, Zhang, Turel, Bechara, & He, 2017).

Recent data and theories suggest that diminished control over the specific behavior in behavioral addictions and substance-use disorders is likely resulting from an imbalance between the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive (insula) system (cf. Noël, Brevers, & Bechara, 2013; Turel & Bechara, 2016; Wei et al., 2017). However, the interoceptive system as a part of the tripartite-process theory of addiction has been less frequently examined in behavioral addictions (Noël et al., 2013). In substance-use disorders it has been identified as an intermediate component which potentiates the activity of the impulsive system and/or weakens the reflective system by translating bottom-up, interoceptive signals into subjective output (e.g., craving) (Noël et al., 2013). The interoceptive system has been associated with tendencies to relapse in substance-use disorders (Moeller & Paulus, 2018) and is also considered playing a balancing role between the impulsive and the reflective system (Droutman, Read, & Bechara, 2015). First studies on gaming disorder found increased connectivity between the interoceptive system and regions of the reflective and impulsive system (Chen et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016).

The current study aimed at investigating the potential role of these three neural systems during inhibitory control in the context of problematic IP use. Several studies have indicated that also in individuals with higher symptom severity of problematic IP use or compulsive sexual behaviors, the ventral striatum, as part of the impulsive system, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, as part of the reflective system, are hyperactive during cue-reactivity (Brand, Snagowski, Laier, & Maderwald, 2016; Gola et al., 2017; Seok & Sohn, 2015). These studies show that symptom severity is associated with increased processing of sexual stimuli within the impulsive and the reflective system (also see reviews on problematic pornography use and CSBD by Kowalewska et al., 2018; Stark et al., 2018). The hyperactivity of the reflective system may be interpreted as an increase in effort needed by individuals to maintain control over temptations, which are mainly driven by the impulsive system. And also behavioral data show that increased impulsivity in interaction with more impulsive actions are associated with a higher symptom severity of problematic IP use but only when confronted with pornographic images and not when faced with neutral images (Antons & Brand, 2018).

The current study was the third part of a tripartite study which as well included an online survey and laboratory study (see Fig. 1 and Antons, Müller, et al., 2019; Antons, Trotzke, Wegmann, & Brand, 2019 for results of the online survey). The aim of the large scale study was to investigate the role of impulsivity, inhibitory control, and craving in males with problematic IP use. As a first study investigating these interactions we focused on a homogenous group of heterosexual males since sexual practices such as the amount of IP use and the preference of specific pornographic material differ between gender and types of sexual orientation (Bőthe, Bartok, et al., 2018). Within the online-survey it has been shown that craving and some facets of impulsivity (e.g., an impulsive cognitive style) may be aspects differentiating between recreational and problematic IP users (Antons, Müller, et al., 2019) although the effect of impulsivity seems to be lower as expected and its involvement might be enhanced in combination with further situational factors (Antons & Brand, 2018; Bőthe, Tóth-Király, et al., 2018).

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Screening procedure for fMRI study. The fMRI study was the last part of a large scale study which aimed to investigate the role of impulsivity, inhibitory control, and craving in the context of problematic IP use. First, heterosexual males participated in an online study and answered the s-IATporn and BIS-15 besides further questionnaires (for details see Antons, Müller, et al., 2019). After the online study, participants were invited to a laboratory study. The image rating was one part of this laboratory study and participants were briefed comprehensively with regard to the fMRI study. Aiming a high variety in symptom severity of problematic IP use, participants were invited to the fMRI study which consisted of questionnaires, a SST training, and the fMRI study. BIS-15 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; HBI = hypersexual behavior inventory; s-IATporn = short Internet addiction test modified for pornography; SST = stop-signal task

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 1; 10.1556/2006.2020.00010

Up to now, no study has investigated neural correlates of inhibitory control in the context of problematic IP use. We hypothesize that reduced inhibitory control is associated with a hyperactive impulsive system as well as altered activity within the reflective system and interoceptive system. Due to its centrality in the addiction process and previous results on problematic IP use and CSBD we focus on the ventral striatum as one structure of the impulsive system. Furthermore, we focus on the IFG pars opercularis as structure of the reflective system which has been repeatedly associated with inhibitory control performance (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, 2014; Bari & Robbins, 2013) and has been shown to be involved in cue-reactivity processes (Snagowski, Wegmann, Pekal, Laier, & Brand, 2015; Zheng et al., 2019). Due to mechanisms of incentive-sensitization and cue-reactivity (Berridge & Robinson, 2016; Brand et al., 2019), we hypothesize that the aforementioned effects are higher when pornographic cues are present and for individuals with higher symptom severity of problematic IP use.

Methods

Participants

Overall, 1,640 participants answered the online survey without missing data. A number of 1,401 participants indicated their interest in participating in the laboratory study; 516 participants were invited to the laboratory study, with the aim to measure a sample of 100 participants with a high variety in symptoms of problematic IP use. We used the short Internet Addiction Test modified for IP (s-IATporn) to evenly fill groups of unproblematic IP, problematic, and pathological IP users (see subsection measures for further details; Laier, Pawlikowski, Pekal, Schulte, & Brand, 2013). A sample of 108 heterosexual males participated in the laboratory study and were informed about the fMRT study and screened for contraindications and exclusion criteria (right-handedness, normal or corrected-to-normal vision including color vision). On average, the online survey was conducted 72 days before the fMRI study. All participants who were interested in participating in the fMRI study and eligible were invited to the fMRI study. Overall, 30 heterosexual male pornography users participated in the present fMRI study (IP use [min/week]: M = 490.00, SD = 963.47, Min-Max: 15–5,000, please note that IP use in minutes per week is a subjective estimation of IP use assessed during the online survey which was assessed with two questions: frequency per week and minutes per session. The subjective estimation might be biased which may lead to over- or underestimations of IP use. However, the mean is comparable to results by Laier et al. (2013). All participants provided written informed consent prior to the experiment and were informed about the content of the study and that they could terminate participation at any time without reprisal. One participant was identified as an outlier, due to unusual data in behavioral measures, and was therefore excluded from further analyses (see stop-signal task descriptions, for further details). Furthermore, one participant was excluded due to movement parameters during fMRI. Therefore, the final sample comprises 28 participants (Mage = 29.28, SDage = 8.81). More than half of the participants (57.1%) were in a relationship. Participants could choose between course credits or a monetary incentive of €40 in return for the 3-hour study as remuneration.

Measures

FMRI paradigm for the assessment of inhibitory control and craving

For the current study a modified Stop-Signal Task (SST, Antons & Brand, 2018) was used to measure inhibitory control. To compare SST performance during neutral and pornographic image processing, each participant randomly performed six SST blocks, three comprising pornographic and three comprising neutral images.

In the classic SST, participants have to respond (e.g., with their right index-finger) on go-signals which are followed by a stop-signal in some trials. In these stop-trials, participants are asked to inhibit the already initiated response on the go-signal. In the modified SST version which was used in the current study each trial is initiated by the presentation of an image (neutral or pornographic), which is shown until the end of the trial. The images have no direct relevance for carrying out the task but serve as cues to induce craving in the pornographic condition. Furthermore, informative cues (differently colored dashed frames) are included which inform the participant whether the current trial is a certain or uncertain go-trial. The certain-go cue (green dashed frame) indicates that in this trial no stop-signal is going to appear. Accordingly, participants can be sure that they have to respond to the following go-signal. An uncertain go-trial is introduced by a red dashed frame and indicates that it is uncertain whether a stop-signal is going to appear. After the cue presentation and a variable inter-stimulus interval (ISI), the go-signal (green, solid frame) is presented. The ISI is included to reduce predictability of the go-signal. Participants have to respond as fast as possible following the go-signal. In one third of all uncertain go-trials, the go-signal is followed by a stop-signal (red, solid frame) after a variable stop-signal-delay (SSD). See Fig. 2 for timing details. Participants are advised that prompt responses on go-signals are as important as staying alert for the stop-signal.

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

Stop-signal task as used during fMRI. Each image (neutral or pornographic) lasted for the whole trial. Here, an example for the pornographic condition is presented. Pornographic images were not censored. In the neutral condition images showed people during jogging or hiking. In certain go trials, participants could be sure that no stop-signal will occur. In uncertain go trials, the go-signal was followed by a stop-signal in one third of the cases (represented by a dashed arrow). ISI = inter-stimulus interval; ITI = inter-trial-interval; SSD = stop signal delay

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 1; 10.1556/2006.2020.00010

Each SST block consists of 85 trials with 34 certain go-trials, 52 uncertain trials of which 34 are go trials, and 17 are stop trials. Participants were trained before the scan with 45 trials (same proportion of certain go trials and uncertain go and stop trials). The training trials provide feedback for slow or wrong responses, whereas during the experimental blocks no feedback was given. Trials with go-reaction times (go-RT) faster than 100 ms or deviations of more than two standard deviations from the participants' mean go-RT were considered outliers and were therefore excluded from further analyses.

The SST version used in this study measures inhibitory control using three different variables: the certain go-RT as a measure for general response speed in certain situations, the uncertain go-RT as a measure for impulsive action tendencies on go-signals in uncertain situations, which also includes preparatory processes for a possible stop-process, and the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) as a measure for inhibitory response ability. The SSRT was computed using the integration method (Verbruggen, Chambers, & Logan, 2013). This method requires a nearly equal distribution of false and correct stop-trials and is more robust against the violation of this requirement compared to the mean method. An equal distribution of false and correct stop-trials was attained by using a staircase-tracking procedure for the stop-signal-delay (Verbruggen, Logan, & Stevens, 2008). Since this staircase procedure adapts to the participants' performances from stop-trial to stop-trial, we used the SSD of the last correct stop-trial of the training trials as an initial SSD for the fMRI paradigm. One participant with more than 70% missed responses in certain go-trials was excluded from the analyses. The task was programmed using the Presentation software package (Neurobehavioral Systems Inc., Albany, CA, USA).

Furthermore, after each block during the fMRI SST paradigm participants were asked to indicate their current urge to use IP (“Please indicate your current urge to use IP right now”). The question had to be answered on a four-point Likert-scale ranging from 1 (no urge at all) to 4 (very high urge). Answers, which were given after the blocks, were summed up for the three neutral and three pornographic SST blocks as measures for current craving.

Image rating

During the laboratory study which took place before the fMRI study (see Fig. 1), neutral and pornographic images were rated with regard to arousal (from 1 = not at all arousing to 5 = very arousing), valence (from 1 = very negative to 5 = very positive), and urge to use IP (from 1 = not at all to 5 = very strong). Viewing times were also assessed.

The pornographic images were used in earlier studies and showed scenes of male and female sexual vaginal and oral intercourse (Antons & Brand, 2018; Laier, Pawlikowski, & Brand, 2014; Schiebener, Laier, & Brand, 2015). Neutral images showed males and females jogging or hiking as examples for activities with non-sexual connotation which were also already used in an earlier study (Snagowski et al., 2015).

Symptom severity of problematic IP use

We assessed the symptom severity of problematic IP use with the German short Internet Addiction Test modified for IP (s-IATporn, in accordance with Laier et al., 2013). The questionnaire consists of twelve items divided into two sub-scales: craving/social problems and loss of control/time management. Example items are: “How often do you feel depressed, moody, or nervous when you are offline, which goes away once you are back on Internet-pornography?” and “How often do you try to cut down the amount of time you spend on Internet-pornography and fail?” All items were answered on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often) resulting in a sum score between twelve and 60. For general Internet-use disorder a cut-off score higher than 30 indicates problematic and higher than 37 pathological behavior (Pawlikowski, Altstötter-Gleich, & Brand, 2013). This cut-off has been used in previous studies as an indicator for problematic IP use (Antons & Brand, 2018; Laier et al., 2013). The s-IATporn was assessed during all three parts of the large scale study and had an excellent internal consistency Cronbach's alpha = 0.927 in the current study.

Hypersexual behavior inventory

The hypersexual behavior inventory (HBI; Reid, Garos, & Carpenter, 2011) in German language (Klein, Rettenberger, Boom, & Briken, 2014) assesses problems due to hypersexual behavior, which is, in comparison to the s-IATporn, not only addressing IP use but includes all sexual activities that stimulate or arouse sexual desire or orgasms. Example items are: “Even though I promised myself I would not repeat a sexual behavior, I find myself returning to it over and over again”; “I use sex to forget about the worries of daily life,” and “I sacrifice things I really want in life in order to be sexual.” Nineteen items had to be answered on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). Three subscales (control, coping, and consequences) were summed up to one HBI score ranging between 19 and 95 with excellent internal consistency in the current sample Cronbach's alpha = 0.949.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

Trait impulsivity was assessed with the short version of the German Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-15; Meule, Vögele, & Kübler, 2011) during the online survey. Within 15 items answered on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost always/always) the scale measures the frequency of several common impulsive or non-impulsive behaviors/traits. Example items are: “I am restless at the theater or lectures,” “I do things without thinking,” and “I plan for job security.” A higher sum score (possible range from 15 to 60) indicates increased trait impulsivity. Three subscales attentional, non-planning, and motor impulsivity were summed up to one BIS-15 score (in the current sample good internal consistency with Cronbach's alpha = 0.873).

fMRI data acquisition

See supplementary material.

Statistical analysis

Behavioral data

In accordance with West, Finch, and Curran (1995) data was normally distributed which allows parametric testing. Data from the image rating was analyzed with t-tests for dependent samples. Cohen's d for repeated measures was calculated using the formula proposed by Dunlap, Cortina, Vaslow, and Burke (1996). Pearson correlations were computed for questionnaire data and SST performance in both conditions.

FMRI data

Imaging data was analyzed using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, London). The following procedure was employed: realignment and unwarp, co-registration, normalization, and smoothing. The first three images of the first fMRI block were removed to allow magnetization equilibration. Images were smoothed with 5 mm full-width half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian distribution.

In the first level analysis, the onsets in both SST conditions were set on the certain go-cue, uncertain go-cue, certain go-signal, uncertain go-signal, stop-signal, and ITI (see Fig. 2). Thus, seven regressors were entered for each of the six blocks in the general linear model. To account for residual head motion effects, the motion parameters from the realignment procedure were also included into the statistical model. Contrast images were created for the neutral and pornographic conditions comprising all three blocks, respectively. Our primary interest referred to processes of inhibitory control. In accordance with the horse-race model (Verbruggen & Logan, 2009) and previous studies using the SST during fMRI (e.g. Chikazoe et al., 2009; Majid, Cai, Corey-Bloom, & Aron, 2013; Swann et al., 2012), activity at the onset stop with activity during certain go-processing (stop vs. certain go) was contrasted to isolate neural activity specific for inhibitory control. During the whole trial, a neutral or pornographic image was shown. Therefore, this contrast does not only isolate the inhibitory control process from the go-process but also from the image processing. To obtain neural activity for pornographic image processing, activity of all onsets in the pornographic condition with activity of all onsets in the neutral condition (besides ITI, pornographic vs. neutral) was contrasted. To obtain group statistics, contrast images of each participant were entered into a second-level one-sample or paired-sample t-test. For whole brain analyses, significance was accepted for clusters exceeding a statistical threshold of p < 0.05, family-wise error corrected.

Furthermore, a region of interest (ROI) approach was used to examine estimated values for the ventral striatum, IFG, and insula activity and to perform Pearson correlations on these data and behavioral as well as questionnaire data. The binary masks for the IFG pars opercularis and insula were obtained from Automatic Anatomical Labeling atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). The ventral striatum mask was obtained from a large-scale meta-analysis for the term “ventral striatum” in the Neurosynth data base (Yarkoni, Poldrack, Nichols, Van Essen, & Wager, 2011). To isolate ventral striatum activity, we used a threshold of 15. Parameter estimates for the ROIs were extracted using MarsBaR (Brett et al., 2002). All activation loci reported in this work as MNI coordinates were verified using the Anatomy software (Version 2.1; Eickhoff et al., 2007).

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were fully instructed and gave informed consent prior to the investigation. The study was approved by the local ethics committee.

Results

Descriptive statistics and behavioral results

Descriptive statistics for questionnaire data are presented in Table 1. At the time of the fMRI study n = 10 participants showed an unproblematic IP use, n = 9 showed problematic and n = 9 pathological symptoms of problematic IP use using the s-IATporn. Neutral and pornographic images were rated significantly different regarding arousal and urge to use IP (see supplementary materials). No significant differences were found regarding valence ratings and viewing times of the three ratings. There were no differences in SST measures between conditions (Table 2).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of questionnaire data

Min Max M SD
s-IATporn
 Loss of control/time management 6 27 14.71 5.99
 Craving/social problems 6 24 11.50 4.30
 Sum 14 51 26.21 9.42
HBI sum 20 81 44.93 14.49
Craving after neutral SST blocks 3 12 4.50 2.29
Craving after pornographic SST blocks 3 11 5.82 2.29
Delta craving –2 4 1.32 1.44
BIS-15 sum 20 46 32.36 7.52

Note. BIS-15 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; HBI = hypersexual behavior inventory; s-IATporn = short Internet addiction test modified for pornography.

Table 2.

Descriptive values and test statistics of comparisons between neutral and pornographic inhibitory control performance during fMRI scanning

Neutral Pornographic
M SD M SD t(27) p Cohen's d 95% CI
Certain go RT 331.22 41.43 330.80 50.37 0.10 0.918 −0.02 [−0.55, 0.50]
Uncertain go RT 501.16 111.10 495.11 106.80 1.23 0.213 −0.23 [−0.76, 0.30]
SSRT 242.71 39.00 234.74 43.72 1.32 0.198 −0.27 [−0.79, 0.26]
Correct stop (%) 51.58 4.11 49.10 6.88 1.49 0.556 −0.41 [−0.94, 0.12]
SSD 255.47 124.41 258.58 133.14 −0.60 0.556 0.12 [−0.40, 0.65]

Note. CI = confidence interval; SSD = stop-signal delay; SSRT = stop-signal reaction time.

The SSRT in both conditions correlated negatively with s-IATporn with a high effect size and in the pornographic condition positively with craving also with a high effect size. Higher s-IATporn scores and craving scores in the pornographic condition were associated with faster SSRTs. The HBI did not correlate significantly with SSRT (however, direction of effect is consistent with the correlation between SSRT and s-IATporn). No significant correlations were found between the SSRT in both conditions and BIS-15. There was a significant positive correlation between craving and uncertain go RT in the neutral condition with a high effect size (Table 3). The s-IATporn significantly correlated with craving after the pornographic (r = 0.664, p < 0.001) and neutral (r = 0.655, p < 0.001) SST conditions, but not with the delta craving (difference between craving ratings before and after the pornographic and neutral condition, r = −0.054, p = 0.783)

Table 3.

Correlations between stop-signal task measures and questionnaire data

s-IATporn sum HBI sum Craving BIS-15
Neutral
 Certain go RT 0.048 0.007 0.156 0.029
 Uncertain go RT 0.022 0.159 0.380* −0.136
 SSRT −0.496** −0.337 −0.118 −0.047
Pornographic
 Certain go RT 0.190 0.185 0.239 −0.004
 Uncertain go RT −0.013 0.143 0.281 −0.186
 SSRT −0.520** −0.372 −0.550** 0.056

Note. BIS-15 = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; HBI = hypersexual behavior inventory; s-IATporn = short Internet addiction test modified for Internet-Pornography; SSRT = stop-signal reaction time.

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.

Neural results

Consistent with behavioral data, also the neural data showed no or marginal differences between conditions in inhibitory control processing and pornographic image processing (see supplementary material)

During pornographic image processing, activity in the left IFG pars opercularis was moderately and negatively associated with symptom severity of problematic IP use (s-IATporn). Right insula activity correlated positively with SSRT (high effect size) and negatively with the craving/social problems subscale of the s-IATporn (moderate effects size). Furthermore, delta craving (pornographic – neutral) correlated negatively with activity of the ventral striatum during pornographic image processing (Table 4 and Fig. 3).

Table 4.

Correlations between measures of inhibitory control and regions of interest during pornographic image processing

Ventral striatum IFG pars opercularis Insula
L R L R L R
Pornographic condition:
Uncertain go-RT −0.245 −0.280 −0.058 −0.013 −0.131 −0.367
 SSRT −0.032 0.145 0.313 0.161 0.261 0.502**
 Craving −0.253 −0.192 −0.097 0.124 −0.255 −0.057
 Delta craving −0.408* −0.339 0.111 −0.127 −0.199 −0.177
s-IATporn
 Loss of control/time Management −0.078 −0.041 −0.392* −0.014 −0.201 −0.077
 Craving/social problems −0.044 0.086 −0.447* −0.128 −0.364 −0.392*
 Sum −0.070 0.013 −0.453* −0.067 −0.294 −0.228

Note. IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; s-IATporn = short Internet addiction test modified for Internet-Pornography; SSRT = stop-signal reaction time.

*p < 0.05; **p < 0.001.

Figure 3.
Figure 3.

Correlations of neural effect sizes during pornographic image processing on measures of symptom severity of problematic Internet-pornography use, craving and inhibitory control. Neural effect sizes (contrast beta) was taken from the contrast of pornographic images – neutral images. A shows the correlation between left inferior frontal gyrus pars opercularis and symptom severity of problematic Internet-pornography use (s-IATporn sum), B between right insula and symptom severity of problematic Internet-pornography use (s-IATporn craving and social problems subscale), C between left ventral striatum effect size and the craving delta (pornographic – neutral), and D between the right insula and inhibitory control ability (stop-signal reaction time, SSRT)

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 1; 10.1556/2006.2020.00010

Overall, during inhibitory control processing neural activity of the ventral striatum, the IFG pars opercularis, and the insula were not associated with behavioral measures of inhibitory control and craving or symptom severity of problematic IP use. There was only one moderate significant negative correlation between right insula and SSRT in the pornographic condition, indicating that better inhibitory control is associated with higher right insula activity (Table 5 and Fig. 4).

Table 5.

Correlations between measures of inhibitory control and regions of interest during inhibitory control processing

Ventral striatum IFG pars opercularis Insula
L R L R L R
Neutral condition
 Uncertain go-RT 0.029 0.108 −0.094 −0.150 −0.054 0.130
 SSRT −0.029 −0.100 −0.314 −0.159 −0.297 −0.350
 Craving 0.017 −0.001 −0.081 0.048 −0.024 0.099
s-IATporn
 Loss of control/time management −0.271 −0.258 −0.040 0.060 −0.086 0.018
 Craving/social problems −0.308 −0.166 0.002 0.010 0.062 0.086
 Sum −0.313 −0.239 −0.024 0.043 −0.026 0.051
Pornographic condition
 Uncertain go-RT 0.030 0.103 −0.011 0.001 −0.053 0.201
 SSRT 0.216 −0.028 −0.142 −0.120 −0.093 −0.455*
 Craving −0.065 0.271 −0.015 −0.103 −0.128 0.210
 Delta craving −0.034 −0.259 0.057 −0.234 −0.031 0.002
s-IATporn
 Loss of control/time management −0.109 0.177 −0.207 −0.137 −0.244 −0.050
 Craving/social problems −0.094 0.180 −0.141 −0.123 −0.206 −0.022
 Sum −0.112 0.195 −0.196 −0.143 −0.249 −0.042

Note. IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; s-IATporn = short Internet addiction test modified for Internet-Pornography; SSRT = stop-signal reaction time.

*p < 0.05.

Fig. 4
Fig. 4

Correlation of neural effect size in right insula during inhibitory control processing on stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). Neural effect size (contrast beta) was taken from the contrast of stop-certain go for the neutral and pornographic condition

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 1; 10.1556/2006.2020.00010

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the neural correlates of inhibitory control when confronted with neutral and pornographic stimuli and its association with craving and symptoms of problematic IP use. The main result of the current study is that higher symptom severity of problematic IP use and craving were associated with better inhibitory control performance (lower SSRT), especially when pornographic images were present. Compared with previous studies (Antons & Brand, 2018; Nie et al., 2016; van Holst et al., 2012; Yao et al., 2015), this result was somehow unexpected, but neural data show a consistent pattern with this behavioral finding. Symptom severity of problematic IP use was associated with decreased activity of the reflective (IFG pars opercularis) and interoceptive (insula) system during pornographic image processing. An increase in craving (delta craving) was associated with lower activity of the impulsive system (ventral striatum) during pornographic image processing which also contrasts results from previous studies (Brand, Snagowski, Laier, & Maderwald, 2016; Gola et al., 2017; Kühn & Gallinat, 2011). Furthermore, better inhibitory control performance was associated with increased activity of the interoceptive system (insula) during inhibitory control processing. Combining this effect with the decreased activity during pornographic image processing the results may suggest the differential and balancing role of the interoceptive system in addictive behaviors.

The associations between symptom severity of problematic IP use and activity of the reflective and interoceptive system are consistent with the altered function of both systems in the course of the development of addiction, as it is proposed in the tripartite-process model for substance-use disorders and gaming disorder (Noël et al., 2013; Wei et al., 2017). However, we found activity of the left IFG pars opercularis to be negatively and inhibitory control ability to be positively associated with symptom severity of problematic IP use. This indicates that individuals with higher symptom severity performed better in the task which might be mediated by reduced control by the reflective system. Also in the context of alcohol-use disorder, it has been shown that individuals with heavy alcohol use show reduced activity in prefrontal regions compared to social drinkers (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010). The activation of the reflective system may prevent social drinkers and individuals with regulated IP use to lose control over their behavior. Although the left IFG has been identified as critical node in inhibitory control (Swick, Ashley, & Turken, 2008), it also has been shown to be involved in speech, empathy, and working memory which might be relevant in pornographic image processing (Liakakis, Nickel, & Seitz, 2011). In the addiction context it has been shown that individuals with more symptoms show less emotional empathy (Ferrari, Smeraldi, Bottero, & Politi, 2014). Also problematic IP use has been associated with socio-emotional problems (Efrati & Gola, 2018; Tonioni et al., 2018). Accordingly, it might be possible that the processing of pornographic images was less intense due to the reduced processing of socio-emotional signs in the pornographic images which may be the reason why less resources of the left IFG were needed.

Furthermore, we found a negative instead of a positive association between the increase in craving (as a subjective measure of cue-reactivity) and activity of the impulsive system (ventral striatum) and a non-significant association between the increase in craving and symptom severity of problematic IP use. This result may be interpreted in the context of tolerance and shifts from urge-driven to habitual behavior in individuals with higher symptom severity (Brand et al., 2019; Everitt & Robbins, 2013). It is assumed that this shift is mediated by a decrease in processing via the ventral striatum and an increase of processing via the dorsal striatum. Consistent with this also Vollstädt-Klein et al. (2010) found craving to be negatively associated with ventral striatum activity in the context of alcohol-use disorder.

Furthermore, it has to be considered that the inhibitory control performance can be affected by situational factors such as behavior-specific cues but also by motivational incentives (Jones, Christiansen, Nederkoorn, Houben, & Field, 2013; Leotti & Wager, 2010). One further explanation could therefore be that individuals with higher symptom severity were more motivated to perform well in the inhibitory control task or to suppress feelings of craving. The insula is involved in balancing between the impulsive and reflective system, which can be realized by re-focusing attention towards salient stimuli (Cai, Ryali, Chen, Li, & Menon, 2014; Droutman, Bechara, & Read, 2015). It is possible that individuals with higher symptom severity of problematic IP use voluntarily shifted their attention from pornographic images (negative association between inhibitory control and insula activity during pornographic image processing) to the more salient stop-signals in this situation (positive association between inhibitory control and insula activity during inhibitory control processing) which increased inhibitory control performance. In contrast, participants with increased insula activity during pornographic image processing performed worse in the inhibitory control task. This is consistent with a study by Li et al. (2008) showing that strategic adjustments of behaviors are associated with increased insula activity (e.g., during inhibitory control). Interestingly, increased insula activity during inhibitory control has been identified as a predictor for the ability to maintain tobacco abstinence (Gilman et al., 2018). Also, Moeller and Paulus (2018) identified the insula as a marker of clinical outcomes. In the current study, participants were invited based on the s-IATporn assessed during an online survey on average 72 days before the fMRI study. In the meantime, participants participated in a laboratory study. During all three assessments participants answered the s-IATporn. We compared s-IATporn scores between the three assessments, which revealed that the s-IATporn score decreased in in 22 participants in comparison to the fMRI study (means and standard deviations are presented in the supplementary material). Since this decrease in symptom severity is present between the fMRI study and both the online survey and laboratory study, we do not assume that it can be explained by effects of social desirability. It is possible that a high number of participants in the current sample reflected on their IP use and became aware of the negative consequences resulting from their IP use. This might have triggered a need to control their IP-use behavior and craving and to ignore or avoid pornographic images which is consistent with studies showing that higher symptoms are associated with a more negative attitude towards IP and both approach and avoidance tendencies in terms of a U-shape relationship (Antons, Müller, et al., 2019; Snagowski & Brand, 2015).

The study has several limitations: Consistent with previous studies (e.g., Antons & Brand, 2018; Brand, Snagowski, Laier, & Maderwald, 2016; Gola et al., 2017; Laier et al., 2013), we found a high correlation between subjective craving and symptom severity of problematic IP use in both conditions. However, the increase in craving as measure for cue-reactivity was not associated with symptom severity of problematic IP use, this may relate to tolerance (cf. Wéry & Billieux, 2017) given that the pornographic images used in this study were not individualized in terms of subjective preferences. Therefore, the standardized pornographic material used may not be strong enough for inducing cue-reactivity in individuals with high symptom severity associated with low effects on the impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive systems as well as inhibitory control ability. Furthermore, since altered neural processing in addictive behaviors is assumed to develop with increasing symptom severity (Brand, Young, Laier, Wölfling, & Potenza, 2016), we decided to investigate individuals with varying degrees of symptom severity of problematic IP use. Future studies should (1) compare individuals with regulated IP use and those with problematic IP use and (2) investigate shifts from urge-driven to habitual behavior and its neural processing (e.g., ventral vs. dorsal striatum) in longitudinal studies. In addition, the sample size of N = 30 was chosen because of experience in a previous and similar study by Brand, Snagowski, et al. (2016). In this study a picture paradigm including preferred and non-preferred pornographic materials was used which is similar to the current SST paradigm with regard to processes of cue-reactivity, but which did not include an inhibitory control task. Therefore, an fMRI power analysis for effects on inhibitory control were impossible. Nevertheless, the current fMRI data should be used for power analysis in future replication studies. Our current study should be seen as a first approach inspiring future investigations regarding the associations between psychological and neural mechanisms of craving, problematic IP use, motivation to change behavior, and inhibitory control.

Conclusion

Taken together, the insula as the key structure representing the interoceptive system plays a pivotal role in inhibitory control when pornographic images are present. Data suggest that individuals with higher symptom severity of problematic IP use performed better in the task due to decreased insula activity during image processing and increased activity during inhibitory control processing. This pattern of activity might be based on effects of tolerance, that is, less hyperactivity of the impulsive system causes less controlling resources of the interoceptive and reflective system. Hence, a shift from impulsive to compulsive behaviors as a consequence of developing problematic IP use or a motivational (avoidance-related) aspect might be relevant, so that all resources were focused on the task and away from pornographic images. The study contributes to a better understanding of diminished control over IP use which is presumably not only a result of an imbalance between dual systems but of the interaction between impulsive, reflective, and interoceptive systems.

Funding sources

No financial support was received for this study.

Authors' contribution

MB and SA designed the study. Data collection was carried out by SA. SA conducted the statistical analysis, interpreted the results, and wrote the first and final draft of the manuscript. MB supervised the interpretation of the data and writing of the manuscript. All authors had full access to all data, contributed to and have approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Stefan Maderwald and Lena Schäfer for their valuable contributions to Data collection and technical support.

Supplementary Material

The online version of this article offers supplementary material (https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00010).

References

  • Antons, S., & Brand, M. (2018). Trait and state impulsivity in males with tendency towards Internet-pornography-use disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 171177. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.029.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antons, S., Müller, S. M., Wegmann, E., Trotzke, P., Schulte, M. M., & Brand, M. (2019). Facets of impulsivity and related aspects differentiate among recreational and unregulated use of Internet pornography. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 111. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.22.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antons, S., Trotzke, P., Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2019). Interaction of craving and functional coping styles in heterosexual males with varying degrees of unregulated Internet-pornography use. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 237243. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.051.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences ,18, 177185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ballester-Arnal, R., Castro Calvo, J., Gil-Llario, M. D., & Gil-Julia, B. (2017). Cybersex addiction: A study on Spanish college students. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 43, 567585. doi: 10.1080/0092623x.2016.1208700.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bari, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 4479. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 14581463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2016). Liking, wanting and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. The American Psychologist, 71, 670679. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bőthe, B., Bartok, R., Tóth-Király, I., Reid, R. C., Griffiths, M. D., Demetrovics, Z., & Orosz, G. (2018). Hypersexuality, gender, and sexual orientation: A large-scale psychometric survey study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 22652276. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1201-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bőthe, B., Tóth-Király, I., Orosz, G., Potenza, M. N., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2018). Revisiting the role of impulsivity and compulsivity in problematic sexual behaviors. Journal of Sex Research, 56, 166179. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2018.1480744.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Snagowski, J., Laier, C., & Maderwald, S. (2016). Ventral striatum activity when watching preferred pornographic pictures is correlated with symptoms of Internet pornography addiction. NeuroImage, 129, 224232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.033.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond Internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 252266. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brett, M., Anton, J. L., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J. B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. Sendai, Japan: Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brewer, J. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2008). The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: Relationships to drug addictions. Biochemical Pharmacology, 75, 6375. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cai, W., Ryali, S., Chen, T., Li, C. S., & Menon, V. (2014). Dissociable roles of right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula in inhibitory control: Evidence from intrinsic and task-related functional parcellation, connectivity, and response profile analyses across multiple datasets. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1465214667. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3048-14.2014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chen, C. Y., Yen, J. Y., Wang, P. W., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2016). Altered functional connectivity of the insula and nucleus accumbens in internet gaming disorder: A resting state fMRI study. European Addiction Research, 22, 192200. doi: 10.1159/000440716.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chikazoe, J., Jimura, K., Hirose, S., Yamashita, K., Miyashita, Y., & Konishi, S. (2009). Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a Stop-Signal Task. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1587015877. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3645-09.2009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Droutman, V., Bechara, A., & Read, S. J. (2015). Roles of the different sub-regions of the insular cortex in various phases of the decision-making process. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, (309–309). doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00309.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Droutman, V., Read, S. J., & Bechara, A. (2015). Revisiting the role of the insula in addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 414420. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dunlap, W. P., Cortina, J. M., Vaslow, J. B., & Burke, M. J. (1996). Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological Methods, 1, 170177. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Efrati, Y., & Gola, M. (2018). Understanding and predicting profiles of compulsive sexual behavior among adolescents. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 10041014. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.100.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Paus, T., Caspers, S., Grosbras, M.-H., Evans, A. C., Zilles, K. (2007). Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage, 36, 511521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.060.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 19461954. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferrari, V., Smeraldi, E., Bottero, G., & Politi, E. (2014). Addiction and empathy: A preliminary analysis. Neurological Sciences, 35, 855859. doi: 10.1007/s10072-013-1611-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gilman, J. M., Radoman, M., Schuster, R. M., Pachas, G., Azzouz, N., Fava, M. (2018). Anterior insula activation during inhibition to smoking cues is associated with ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 7, 4046. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.01.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gola, M., & Potenza, M. N. (2018). Promoting educational, classification, treatment, and policy initiatives: Commentary on: Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11 (Kraus et al., 2018). Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 208210. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.51.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gola, M., Wordecha, M., Sescousse, G., Lew-Starowicz, M., Kossowski, B., Wypych, M. (2017). Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 20212031. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.78.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Attentional bias and disinhibition toward gaming cues are related to problem gaming in male adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 541546. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.07.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones, A., Christiansen, P., Nederkoorn, C., Houben, K., & Field, M. (2013). Fluctuating disinhibition: Implications for the understanding and treatment of alcohol and other substance use disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klein, V., Rettenberger, M., Boom, K.-D., & Briken, P. (2014). A Validation Study of the German Version of the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI) [Eine Validierungsstudie der deutschen Version des Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI)]. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 64, 136140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kowalewska, E., Grubbs, J. B., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Draps, M., & Kraus, S. W. (2018). Neurocognitive mechanisms in compulsive sexual behavior disorder. Current Sexual Health Reports. doi: 10.1007/s11930-018-0176-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. W., Krueger, R. B., Briken, P., First, M. B., Stein, D. J., Kaplan, M. S. (2018). Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11. World Psychiatry, 17, 109110. doi: 10.1002/wps.20499.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. W., & Sweeney, P. J. (2018). Hitting the target: Considerations for differential diagnosis when treating individuals for problematic use of pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48, doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1301-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kühn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis on cue-induced male sexual arousal. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8, 22692275. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02322.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., & Brand, M. (2014). Sexual picture processing interferes with decision-making under ambiguity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 473482. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0119-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Pekal, J., Schulte, F. P., & Brand, M. (2013). Cybersex addiction: Experienced sexual arousal when watching pornography and not real-life sexual contacts makes the difference. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2, 100107. doi: 10.1556/jba.2.2013.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leotti, L. A., & Wager, T. D. (2010). Motivational influences on response inhibition measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 430447. doi: 10.1037/a0016802.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S., Huang, C., Yan, P., Paliwal, P., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2008). Neural correlates of post-error slowing during a stop signal task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 10211029. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20071.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liakakis, G., Nickel, J., & Seitz, R. J. (2011). Diversity of the inferior frontal gyrus—a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 225, 341347. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.06.022.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Majid, D. S., Cai, W., Corey-Bloom, J., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Proactive selective response suppression is implemented via the basal ganglia. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1325913269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5651-12.2013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meng, Y., Deng, W., Wang, H., Guo, W., & Li, T. (2015). The prefrontal dysfunction in individuals with internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Addiction Biology, 20, 799808. doi: 10.1111/adb.12154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Vögele, C., & Kübler, A. (2011). Psychometrische Evaluation der deutschen Barratt Impulsiveness Scale – Kurzversion (BIS-15). Diagnostica, 57, 126133. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000042.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moeller, S. J., & Paulus, M. P. (2018). Toward biomarkers of the addicted human brain: Using neuroimaging to predict relapse and sustained abstinence in substance use disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 80, 143154. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.03.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nie, J., Zhang, W., Chen, J., & Li, W. (2016). Impaired inhibition and working memory in response to internet-related words among adolescents with internet addiction: A comparison with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 236, 2834. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Noël, X., Brevers, D., & Bechara, A. (2013). A neurocognitive approach to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 632638. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.018.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pawlikowski, M., Altstötter-Gleich, C., & Brand, M. (2013). Validation and psychometric properties of a short version of Young's Internet Addiction Test. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 12121223. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C., Carpenter, B. N., Hook, J. N., Garos, S., Manning, J. C., Gilliland, R. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 field trial for hypersexual disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 28682877. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02936.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C., Garos, S., & Carpenter, B. N. (2011). Reliability, validity, and psychometric development of the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory in an outpatient sample of men. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 18, 3051. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2011.555709.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rissel, C., Richters, J., de Visser, R. O., McKee, A., Yeung, A., & Caruana, T. (2017). A profile of pornography users in Australia: Findings from the second Australian study of health and relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 54, 227240. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1191597.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ross, M. W., Mansson, S. A., & Daneback, K. (2012). Prevalence, severity, and correlates of problematic sexual Internet use in Swedish men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 459466. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9762-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schiebener, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Getting stuck with pornography? Overuse or neglect of cybersex cues in a multitasking situation is related to symptoms of cybersex addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4, 1421. doi: 10.1556/jba.4.2015.1.5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seok, J.-W., & Sohn, J.-H. (2015). Neural substrates of sexual desire in individuals with problematic hypersexual behavior. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 321. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00321.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snagowski, J., & Brand, M. (2015). Symptoms of cybersex addiction can be linked to both approaching and avoiding pornographic stimuli: Results from an analog sample of regular cybersex users. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 653. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00653.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snagowski, J., Wegmann, E., Pekal, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Implicit associations in cybersex addiction: Adaption of an Implicit Association Test with pornographic pictures. Addictive Behaviors, 49, 712. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., & Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 227238. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.39.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stark, R., Klucken, T., Potenza, M. N., Brand, M., & Strahler, J. (2018). A current understanding of the behavioral neuroscience of compulsive sexual behavior disorder and problematic pornography use. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 5, 218231. doi: 10.1007/s40473-018-0162-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swann, N. C., Cai, W., Conner, C. R., Pieters, T. A., Claffey, M. P., George, J. S. (2012). Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. NeuroImage, 59, 28602870. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.049.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tonioni, F., Mazza, M., Autullo, G., Pellicano, G. R., Aceto, P., Catalano, V. (2018). Socio-emotional ability, temperament and coping strategies associated with different use of Internet in Internet addiction. European Review for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences, 22, 34613466. doi: 10.26355/eurrev_201806_15171.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Turel, O., & Bechara, A. (2016). A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: Behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 601.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15, 273289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Chambers, C. D., & Logan, G. D. (2013). Fictitious inhibitory differences: How skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychological Science, 24, 352362. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457390.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 647661. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., & Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 479483. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.2.479.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., & Baler, R. D. (2014).Addiction science: Uncovering neurobiological complexity. Neuropharmacology, 76, Part B, 235249. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction, 105, 17411749. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Walton, M. T., & Bhullar, N. (2018). Compulsive sexual behavior as an impulse control disorder: Awaiting field studies data. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 13271331. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1200-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wei, L., Zhang, S., Turel, O., Bechara, A., & He, Q. (2017). A tripartite neurocognitive model of Internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00285.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wéry, A., & Billieux, J. (2017). Problematic cybersex: Conceptualization, assessment, and treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 238246. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. (pp. 5675). Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization . (2019, April). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th Revision). Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yao, Y. W., Wang, L. J., Yip, S. W., Chen, P. R., Li, S., Xu, J. (2015). Impaired decision-making under risk is associated with gaming-specific inhibition deficits among college students with Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Research, 229, 302309. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Wager, T. D. (2011). Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature Methods, 8, 665. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, J. T., Yao, Y. W., Li, C. S. R., Zang, Y. F., Shen, Z. J., Liu, L. (2016). Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the insula in young adults with Internet gaming disorder. Addiction Biology, 21, 743751. doi: 10.1111/adb.12247.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zheng, H., Hu, Y., Wang, Z., Wang, M., Du, X., & Dong, G. (2019). Meta-analyses of the functional neural alterations in subjects with Internet gaming disorder: Similarities and differences across different paradigms. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 109656. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109656.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Supplementary Materials

  • Antons, S., & Brand, M. (2018). Trait and state impulsivity in males with tendency towards Internet-pornography-use disorder. Addictive Behaviors, 79, 171177. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.12.029.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antons, S., Müller, S. M., Wegmann, E., Trotzke, P., Schulte, M. M., & Brand, M. (2019). Facets of impulsivity and related aspects differentiate among recreational and unregulated use of Internet pornography. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 111. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.22.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antons, S., Trotzke, P., Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2019). Interaction of craving and functional coping styles in heterosexual males with varying degrees of unregulated Internet-pornography use. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 237243. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.051.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aron, A. R., Robbins, T. W., & Poldrack, R. A. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences ,18, 177185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ballester-Arnal, R., Castro Calvo, J., Gil-Llario, M. D., & Gil-Julia, B. (2017). Cybersex addiction: A study on Spanish college students. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 43, 567585. doi: 10.1080/0092623x.2016.1208700.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bari, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology, 108, 4479. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 14581463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2016). Liking, wanting and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. The American Psychologist, 71, 670679. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bőthe, B., Bartok, R., Tóth-Király, I., Reid, R. C., Griffiths, M. D., Demetrovics, Z., & Orosz, G. (2018). Hypersexuality, gender, and sexual orientation: A large-scale psychometric survey study. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 22652276. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1201-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bőthe, B., Tóth-Király, I., Orosz, G., Potenza, M. N., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2018). Revisiting the role of impulsivity and compulsivity in problematic sexual behaviors. Journal of Sex Research, 56, 166179. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2018.1480744.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Snagowski, J., Laier, C., & Maderwald, S. (2016). Ventral striatum activity when watching preferred pornographic pictures is correlated with symptoms of Internet pornography addiction. NeuroImage, 129, 224232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.033.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond Internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Young, K. S., Laier, C., Wölfling, K., & Potenza, M. N. (2016). Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71, 252266. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brett, M., Anton, J. L., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J. B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. Sendai, Japan: Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brewer, J. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2008). The neurobiology and genetics of impulse control disorders: Relationships to drug addictions. Biochemical Pharmacology, 75, 6375. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cai, W., Ryali, S., Chen, T., Li, C. S., & Menon, V. (2014). Dissociable roles of right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula in inhibitory control: Evidence from intrinsic and task-related functional parcellation, connectivity, and response profile analyses across multiple datasets. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1465214667. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3048-14.2014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chen, C. Y., Yen, J. Y., Wang, P. W., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2016). Altered functional connectivity of the insula and nucleus accumbens in internet gaming disorder: A resting state fMRI study. European Addiction Research, 22, 192200. doi: 10.1159/000440716.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chikazoe, J., Jimura, K., Hirose, S., Yamashita, K., Miyashita, Y., & Konishi, S. (2009). Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a Stop-Signal Task. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1587015877. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3645-09.2009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Droutman, V., Bechara, A., & Read, S. J. (2015). Roles of the different sub-regions of the insular cortex in various phases of the decision-making process. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, (309–309). doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00309.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Droutman, V., Read, S. J., & Bechara, A. (2015). Revisiting the role of the insula in addiction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 414420. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dunlap, W. P., Cortina, J. M., Vaslow, J. B., & Burke, M. J. (1996). Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychological Methods, 1, 170177. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.170.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Efrati, Y., & Gola, M. (2018). Understanding and predicting profiles of compulsive sexual behavior among adolescents. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 10041014. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.100.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eickhoff, S. B., Paus, T., Caspers, S., Grosbras, M.-H., Evans, A. C., Zilles, K. (2007). Assignment of functional activations to probabilistic cytoarchitectonic areas revisited. NeuroImage, 36, 511521. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.060.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2013). From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 19461954. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferrari, V., Smeraldi, E., Bottero, G., & Politi, E. (2014). Addiction and empathy: A preliminary analysis. Neurological Sciences, 35, 855859. doi: 10.1007/s10072-013-1611-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gilman, J. M., Radoman, M., Schuster, R. M., Pachas, G., Azzouz, N., Fava, M. (2018). Anterior insula activation during inhibition to smoking cues is associated with ability to maintain tobacco abstinence. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 7, 4046. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.01.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gola, M., & Potenza, M. N. (2018). Promoting educational, classification, treatment, and policy initiatives: Commentary on: Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11 (Kraus et al., 2018). Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 208210. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.51.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gola, M., Wordecha, M., Sescousse, G., Lew-Starowicz, M., Kossowski, B., Wypych, M. (2017). Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 20212031. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.78.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van Holst, R. J., Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., Veltman, D. J., & Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). Attentional bias and disinhibition toward gaming cues are related to problem gaming in male adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50, 541546. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.07.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones, A., Christiansen, P., Nederkoorn, C., Houben, K., & Field, M. (2013). Fluctuating disinhibition: Implications for the understanding and treatment of alcohol and other substance use disorders. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 140. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klein, V., Rettenberger, M., Boom, K.-D., & Briken, P. (2014). A Validation Study of the German Version of the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI) [Eine Validierungsstudie der deutschen Version des Hypersexual Behavior Inventory (HBI)]. Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 64, 136140.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kowalewska, E., Grubbs, J. B., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Draps, M., & Kraus, S. W. (2018). Neurocognitive mechanisms in compulsive sexual behavior disorder. Current Sexual Health Reports. doi: 10.1007/s11930-018-0176-z.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. W., Krueger, R. B., Briken, P., First, M. B., Stein, D. J., Kaplan, M. S. (2018). Compulsive sexual behaviour disorder in the ICD-11. World Psychiatry, 17, 109110. doi: 10.1002/wps.20499.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kraus, S. W., & Sweeney, P. J. (2018). Hitting the target: Considerations for differential diagnosis when treating individuals for problematic use of pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 48, doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1301-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kühn, S., & Gallinat, J. (2011). A quantitative meta-analysis on cue-induced male sexual arousal. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 8, 22692275. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2011.02322.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., & Brand, M. (2014). Sexual picture processing interferes with decision-making under ambiguity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43, 473482. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0119-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Pekal, J., Schulte, F. P., & Brand, M. (2013). Cybersex addiction: Experienced sexual arousal when watching pornography and not real-life sexual contacts makes the difference. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2, 100107. doi: 10.1556/jba.2.2013.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leotti, L. A., & Wager, T. D. (2010). Motivational influences on response inhibition measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 430447. doi: 10.1037/a0016802.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Li, C. S., Huang, C., Yan, P., Paliwal, P., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2008). Neural correlates of post-error slowing during a stop signal task: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 10211029. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20071.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liakakis, G., Nickel, J., & Seitz, R. J. (2011). Diversity of the inferior frontal gyrus—a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 225, 341347. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.06.022.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Majid, D. S., Cai, W., Corey-Bloom, J., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Proactive selective response suppression is implemented via the basal ganglia. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 1325913269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5651-12.2013.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meng, Y., Deng, W., Wang, H., Guo, W., & Li, T. (2015). The prefrontal dysfunction in individuals with internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Addiction Biology, 20, 799808. doi: 10.1111/adb.12154.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Vögele, C., & Kübler, A. (2011). Psychometrische Evaluation der deutschen Barratt Impulsiveness Scale – Kurzversion (BIS-15). Diagnostica, 57, 126133. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924/a000042.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moeller, S. J., & Paulus, M. P. (2018). Toward biomarkers of the addicted human brain: Using neuroimaging to predict relapse and sustained abstinence in substance use disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 80, 143154. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.03.003.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nie, J., Zhang, W., Chen, J., & Li, W. (2016). Impaired inhibition and working memory in response to internet-related words among adolescents with internet addiction: A comparison with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 236, 2834. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Noël, X., Brevers, D., & Bechara, A. (2013). A neurocognitive approach to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 632638. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.018.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pawlikowski, M., Altstötter-Gleich, C., & Brand, M. (2013). Validation and psychometric properties of a short version of Young's Internet Addiction Test. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 12121223. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C., Carpenter, B. N., Hook, J. N., Garos, S., Manning, J. C., Gilliland, R. (2012). Report of findings in a DSM-5 field trial for hypersexual disorder. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 9, 28682877. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02936.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reid, R. C., Garos, S., & Carpenter, B. N. (2011). Reliability, validity, and psychometric development of the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory in an outpatient sample of men. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 18, 3051. doi: 10.1080/10720162.2011.555709.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rissel, C., Richters, J., de Visser, R. O., McKee, A., Yeung, A., & Caruana, T. (2017). A profile of pornography users in Australia: Findings from the second Australian study of health and relationships. Journal of Sex Research, 54, 227240. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1191597.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ross, M. W., Mansson, S. A., & Daneback, K. (2012). Prevalence, severity, and correlates of problematic sexual Internet use in Swedish men and women. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 459466. doi: 10.1007/s10508-011-9762-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schiebener, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Getting stuck with pornography? Overuse or neglect of cybersex cues in a multitasking situation is related to symptoms of cybersex addiction. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4, 1421. doi: 10.1556/jba.4.2015.1.5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seok, J.-W., & Sohn, J.-H. (2015). Neural substrates of sexual desire in individuals with problematic hypersexual behavior. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 321. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00321.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snagowski, J., & Brand, M. (2015). Symptoms of cybersex addiction can be linked to both approaching and avoiding pornographic stimuli: Results from an analog sample of regular cybersex users. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 653. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00653.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snagowski, J., Wegmann, E., Pekal, J., Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2015). Implicit associations in cybersex addiction: Adaption of an Implicit Association Test with pornographic pictures. Addictive Behaviors, 49, 712. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.009.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Starcke, K., Antons, S., Trotzke, P., & Brand, M. (2018). Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7, 227238. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.39.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stark, R., Klucken, T., Potenza, M. N., Brand, M., & Strahler, J. (2018). A current understanding of the behavioral neuroscience of compulsive sexual behavior disorder and problematic pornography use. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 5, 218231. doi: 10.1007/s40473-018-0162-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swann, N. C., Cai, W., Conner, C. R., Pieters, T. A., Claffey, M. P., George, J. S. (2012). Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity. NeuroImage, 59, 28602870. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.049.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swick, D., Ashley, V., & Turken, A. U. (2008). Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-102.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tonioni, F., Mazza, M., Autullo, G., Pellicano, G. R., Aceto, P., Catalano, V. (2018). Socio-emotional ability, temperament and coping strategies associated with different use of Internet in Internet addiction. European Review for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences, 22, 34613466. doi: 10.26355/eurrev_201806_15171.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Turel, O., & Bechara, A. (2016). A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: Behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 601.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage, 15, 273289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Chambers, C. D., & Logan, G. D. (2013). Fictitious inhibitory differences: How skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychological Science, 24, 352362. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457390.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Models of response inhibition in the stop-signal and stop-change paradigms. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 647661. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.014.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., & Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 479483. doi: 10.3758/brm.40.2.479.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Volkow, N. D., & Baler, R. D. (2014).Addiction science: Uncovering neurobiological complexity. Neuropharmacology, 76, Part B, 235249. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction, 105, 17411749. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Walton, M. T., & Bhullar, N. (2018). Compulsive sexual behavior as an impulse control disorder: Awaiting field studies data. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47, 13271331. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1200-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wei, L., Zhang, S., Turel, O., Bechara, A., & He, Q. (2017). A tripartite neurocognitive model of Internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00285.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wéry, A., & Billieux, J. (2017). Problematic cybersex: Conceptualization, assessment, and treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 238246. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.007.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications. (pp. 5675). Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization . (2019, April). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (11th Revision). Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yao, Y. W., Wang, L. J., Yip, S. W., Chen, P. R., Li, S., Xu, J. (2015). Impaired decision-making under risk is associated with gaming-specific inhibition deficits among college students with Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry Research, 229, 302309. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yarkoni, T., Poldrack, R. A., Nichols, T. E., Van Essen, D. C., & Wager, T. D. (2011). Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nature Methods, 8, 665. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhang, J. T., Yao, Y. W., Li, C. S. R., Zang, Y. F., Shen, Z. J., Liu, L. (2016). Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the insula in young adults with Internet gaming disorder. Addiction Biology, 21, 743751. doi: 10.1111/adb.12247.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zheng, H., Hu, Y., Wang, Z., Wang, M., Du, X., & Dong, G. (2019). Meta-analyses of the functional neural alterations in subjects with Internet gaming disorder: Similarities and differences across different paradigms. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 109656. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109656.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Aug 2024 0 156 94
Sep 2024 0 131 56
Oct 2024 0 273 106
Nov 2024 0 220 134
Dec 2024 0 170 75
Jan 2025 0 70 41
Feb 2025 0 0 0