Authors:
Hyunsuk Jeong Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Hyunsuk Jeong in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Hyeon Woo Yim Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Hyeon Woo Yim in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-3646-8161
,
Seung-Yup Lee Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Seung-Yup Lee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Hae Kook Lee Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Hae Kook Lee in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Marc N. Potenza Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience and Child Study Center, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling, Wethersfield, CT, USA
Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, CT, USA

Search for other papers by Marc N. Potenza in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Sun-Jin Jo Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Sun-Jin Jo in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Hye Jung Son Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Hye Jung Son in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Gyeogmin Kim Department of Preventive Medicine, College of Medicine, Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, Korea

Search for other papers by Gyeogmin Kim in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Abstract

Objectives

We examined serial mediating roles of low self-control and aggression in explaining relationships between levels of inattention and hyperactivity problems (IHPs) and severity of Internet gaming disorder (IGD) features when exposed to online games among adolescents without Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) stratified by gender using three-wave longitudinal study.

Method

The sample comprised a total of 1,732 family dyads from a study that was conducted among seventh graders without diagnoses of ADHD at baseline. Levels of IHPs were assessed by the parent reported Korean version of the ADHD rating scale at baseline (wave1). Severity of IGD features was assessed by the Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen (IGUESS) at wave3. Both levels of self-control (wave1) and aggression (wave2) were assessed by self-report. The mediating role of low self-control and aggression in the relationships between level of IHPs and severity of IGD were evaluated using serial mediation analysis separately for each gender.

Results

Levels of IHPs were related directly to severity of IGD features in both genders. The indirect effects via low self-control were also significant in both genders, however, the indirect effects via aggression was significant only in women. The serial mediation effect via low self-control and aggression between levels of IHPs and IGD features was significant in both genders (men, coefficient:0.009, 95%CI 0.005–0.019; women, coefficient:0.010, 95%CI:0.005–0.026).

Conclusion

We revealed a possible mechanism underlying a serial mediation chain from low self-control to aggression explaining the effects of IHPs on severity of IGD features. However, this conclusion should be taken with a caution, because the effect sizes were very low.

Abstract

Objectives

We examined serial mediating roles of low self-control and aggression in explaining relationships between levels of inattention and hyperactivity problems (IHPs) and severity of Internet gaming disorder (IGD) features when exposed to online games among adolescents without Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) stratified by gender using three-wave longitudinal study.

Method

The sample comprised a total of 1,732 family dyads from a study that was conducted among seventh graders without diagnoses of ADHD at baseline. Levels of IHPs were assessed by the parent reported Korean version of the ADHD rating scale at baseline (wave1). Severity of IGD features was assessed by the Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen (IGUESS) at wave3. Both levels of self-control (wave1) and aggression (wave2) were assessed by self-report. The mediating role of low self-control and aggression in the relationships between level of IHPs and severity of IGD were evaluated using serial mediation analysis separately for each gender.

Results

Levels of IHPs were related directly to severity of IGD features in both genders. The indirect effects via low self-control were also significant in both genders, however, the indirect effects via aggression was significant only in women. The serial mediation effect via low self-control and aggression between levels of IHPs and IGD features was significant in both genders (men, coefficient:0.009, 95%CI 0.005–0.019; women, coefficient:0.010, 95%CI:0.005–0.026).

Conclusion

We revealed a possible mechanism underlying a serial mediation chain from low self-control to aggression explaining the effects of IHPs on severity of IGD features. However, this conclusion should be taken with a caution, because the effect sizes were very low.

Introduction

Although playing online games may provide beneficial and positive effects (Granic, Lobel, & Engels, 2014), excessive or addictive gaming is associated with aggression, maladaptive coping strategies, decreased academic achievement and performance. Individuals with gaming problems may sacrifice hobbies, sleep, and work (Ferguson, San Miguel, Garza, & Jerabeck, 2012; Kuss & Griffiths, 2012; Lam, 2014). Internet gaming disorder (IGD) was introduced by the American Psychiatric Association as a possible condition requiring further investigation (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and gaming disorder has been officially classified as a “disorder due to addictive behaviors” by the World Health Organization (2018).

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder of childhood that often persists into adulthood (Kidd, 2000). Individuals with ADHD show imbalances in neurobiological processes such as behavioral inhibition, self-regulation, and self-control, as well as altered reinforcement sensitivity (Hauser et al., 2014; Sebastian et al., 2014). The presence of ADHD symptoms increases the likelihood of exhibiting IGD symptoms (Gonzalez-Bueso et al., 2018; Pearcy, McEvoy, & Roberts, 2017; Yen, Chou, Liu, Yang, & Hu, 2014). Individuals with greater inattention and hyperactivity symptomatology have reported greater IGD symptom severity (Mathews, Morrell, & Molle, 2019). However, little is known about the mechanism underlying these relationships.

Low self-control may be defined by immediate and unplanned responses toward external stimuli (Potenza & de Wit, 2010). A longitudinal study suggested that low self-control represented a risk factor for becoming gaming problems among children and adolescents (Gentile et al., 2011). Lee et al. (2017) suggested that low self-control could be a marker of vulnerability to internet addiction because it is associated with the severity of internet addiction (Lee, Lee, & Choo, 2017). Impaired control has been posited as a main element in IGD and other addictive behaviors (Brand et al., 2019; Dong & Potenza, 2014). We hypothesized that low self-control contributes importantly to the occurrence and severity of gaming disorder in adolescents.

Aggression has been suggested as a vulnerability factor underlying problematic video-gaming, together with impulsivity and sensation-seeking (Lee et al., 2017). Aggressive behaviors have been linked to problematic video-gaming cross-sectionally (Zhai et al., 2019), and aggression may constitute a risk factor for IGD in adolescents (Gonzalez-Bueso et al., 2018; Paulus, Ohmann, von Gontard, & Popow, 2018), particularly as longitudinal data suggests that problematic video-gaming exerts a cross-lagged effect on physical aggression among adolescents (Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2011).

Low self-control has longitudinal associations with aggressive and risk-taking behaviors (Pratt, Turanovic, Fox, & Wright, 2014; Reisig & Pratt, 2011). According to the Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) General Theory of Crime, poor self-control is the source of aggressive and criminal behaviors (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Many studies have demonstrated robust associations between poor self-control and aggressive behavior or externalizing outcomes (Kühn et al., 2014; Rosen et al., 2014; Van Lange, Rinderu, & Bushman, 2017). Research across developmental periods has supported the notion that individuals who have self-regulation difficulties are more likely to show increased aggression and rule-breaking behaviors, including involvement in juvenile delinquency and later adult offending (Casey & Caudle, 2013; Glenn & Raine, 2014). Therefore, we examined serial mediating roles of low self-control and aggression in the relationships between level of IHPs and severity of IGD features using three wave longitudinal follow-up data.

Adolescence is a critical period of brain development and behavioral maturation, and adolescents are vulnerable to addictive behaviors (Casey & Caudle, 2013). Adolescents with greater disparities between frontal and limbic systems are more vulnerable to addictive disorders (Hammond, Mayes, & Potenza, 2014). Frontostriatal mismatch in neurodevelopment may contribute to increased impulsivity in children and adolescents (Lee et al., 2017). Higher levels of testosterone are released during the juvenile period than in other developmental stages, stimulating aggression and anger (Berenbaum, Beltz, & Corley, 2015; Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, 2010). Given the importance of emotional development, it is critical to understand whether it is susceptible to gender differences. We hypothesized that the levels of IHPs would directly and indirectly influence the severity of IGD features through mediators of low self-control and aggression among adolescents. In addition, we investigated gender difference in mediating role of low self-control and aggression in the relationship between the levels of IHPs and the severity of IGD features longitudinally.

Materials and methods

Study sample

The sample comprised a total of 1,732 family dyads (seventh grade adolescents and a parent or related caregiver each) derived from the iCURE study, which has been described in detail elsewhere (Jeong et al., 2017). A total of 2,319 third, fourth, and seventh grade students were enrolled in the iCURE study at baseline (wave 1). The follow-up assessments were completed at 12 months (wave 2) and 24 months (wave 3) after the baseline assessment. We included seventh grade students (n = 1,920) in the present study at baseline. Among them, we excluded those who did not complete follow-up assessment at wave 2 (n = 80) and wave 3 (n = 72), those who were diagnosed with ADHD by the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 4 (DISC-IV) (n = 30), and those who did not complete IHP assessments at wave 1 (n = 6). Individuals with ADHD were excluded given the focus of the present study on individuals without formal ADHD.

Measurements

Procedure

Adolescent data were collected at their schools during school hours at both baseline and follow-up assessments. All participants completed questionnaires using a self-administered web-based survey with a supervising research assistant available to answer questions. One of each adolescent's parents or primary caregivers completed self-administered questionnaires at the participant's home or in a private space in their child's school based on the participant's preferences at baseline.

Inattention and hyperactivity problems

IHPs were assessed according to the parent-report version of the Korean version of the ADHD rating scale (K-ARS) which was originally developed by Dupaul (1991). The K-ARS is an 18-item questionnaire based on DSM-IV ADHD. The K-ARS consists of two subscales, inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, each containing nine items. Parents were required to rate the frequency of each of the ADHD symptoms occurring over the past 12 months on a four-point Likert scale with 1 for never or rarely, 2 for sometimes, 3 for often, and 4 for very often. The sum of all 18 item scores results in a total score. The reliability and validity of the home version of the K-ARS was verified in a sample of Korean children and adolescents (So, Noh, Kim, Ko, & Koh, 2002). The internal consistency of the K-ARS for the present sample was 0.89. Higher scores indicated more severe ADHD symptoms.

Low self-control

Self-control was assessed using a self-reported self-control rating scale originally developed by Gottfredson and Hirschi (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). The scale contains 20 items that are divided into two self-control domains relating to immediate and delayed satisfaction-seeking tendencies. We used 10 items to assess immediate satisfaction-seeking tendency in this analysis, reflecting low self-control. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale: (4) strongly agree, (3) agree somewhat, (2) disagree somewhat, and (1) strongly disagree. Higher scores indicate lower self-control [24]. Cronbach's alpha was 0.86 in this study.

Level of aggression

The Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire (AQ) was used to evaluate level of aggression (Buss & Perry, 1992). This scale consists of 27 questions and 4 dimensions of aggression (physical aggression, verbal aggression, anger, and hostility). It is scored on a 5-point Likert scale, with scores ranging from 1 (never) to 5 points (very true). Total scores ranged from 27 to 135 points, with higher scores indicating higher levels of aggression. In this study, the internal consistency of this tool (Cronbach's α) was 0.89. The Korean version of the AQ was used in this study (Kwon & Seo, 2002).

Severity of IGD features

Severity of IGD features was assessed using the Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen (IGUESS). This instrument was created based on the nine DSM-5 IGD criteria, with each item rated on a 4-point scale: 0 = strongly disagree, 1 = somewhat disagree, 2 = somewhat agree, 3 = strongly agree. A higher score indicates greater severity of IGD features. This scale is reliable, with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.85 in the current study. Severity of IGD was considered to have a continuous severity dimension, in which higher scores on the IGUESS indicate greater severity (Jo et al., 2017).

Diagnosis of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder

ADHD was evaluated by each parent or guardian using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 4 (DISC-IV) at wave 1. The DISC-IV is a highly structured diagnostic interview used to assess psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents. The DISC-IV was administered by trained interviewers at the participants' homes or in a private space at the school, based on each participant's preference.

Socio-demographics

General characteristics, including age, sex, and family type, were obtained from adolescents' self-reports, and socioeconomic status (SES) was determined based on parents' self-report assessments at wave 1. Regarding family type, an intact family was defined as children living with both parents; non-intact families included children living with only a mother or father or with neither parent because of divorce, death, or separation of their parents. SES was surveyed in seven stages, from 1 = lowest to 7 = highest and 1–4 was categorized as low to moderate and 5–7 to high in the analysis. Academic achievement was surveyed as 1 = very good, 2 = good, 3 = neural, and 4 = bad and 1–2 was classified as high academic achievement and 3–4 as low academic achievement in the analysis.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive data were summarized with numbers and percentages for categorical variables or mean ± SD and ranges for continuous variables stratified by gender. Pearson correlation analysis was performed to detect linear relationships among study variables stratified by gender. Before conducting the analysis, severity measures of IGD features were log-transformed to approximate normality. T-test was conducted to detect gender difference among study variables.

The relationships between levels of IHPs (exposure variable at wave 1) and severity of IGD (outcome variable at wave 3) mediated by low self-control (mediation model 1 at wave 1) and aggression (mediator 2 at wave 2) were evaluated using serial mediation analysis by PROCESS macro, version 3.3, model 6 (Hays, 2013). Serial multiple mediation assumes that a causal chain may link the mediators, with a specified direction of flow. In serial multiple mediation analysis, we estimate one direct effect and three indirect effects of the exposure variable on the outcome variable through mediator 1 and mediator 2 (Fig. 1). Path c represents the direct effect of the exposure variable on the outcome variable. Path a1 represents the effect of the exposure variable on mediator 1, and path a2 represents the effect of the exposure variable on mediator 2. Path a3 is the effect of mediator 1 on mediator 2. Path b1 and b2 represent the effects of mediator 1 and mediator 2, respectively on outcomes. The total effect of ADHD symptoms on severity of IGD features was quantified as the sum of the direct and indirect effects. The indirect effect of ADHD symptoms on severity of IGD features mediated by low self-control was quantified as a1 × b1, and the indirect effect of ADHD symptoms on severity of IGD features through aggression was quantified as a2 × b2. The indirect effect of ADHD symptoms on severity of IGD features through low self-control and high aggression was quantified as a1 × a3 × b2.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Multiple serial mediation model. The relationships between IHPs (inattention and hyperactive problems) at wave 1 (exposure variable) and severity of IGD features at wave 3 (outcome variable) was investigated as being mediated by low self-control at wave 1 (mediator 1) and aggression at wave 2 (mediator 2). Path a1 represents the effect of exposure on mediator 1, and path a2 represents the effect of exposure on mediator 2. Path a3 is the effect of mediator 1 on mediator 2. Path b1 and b2 represent mediator 1 and mediator 2 on outcome, respectively. Path c represents the direct effect of exposure on outcome and Path c' is total effect

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 2; 10.1556/2006.2020.00039

To test the significance of mediation effects, 5,000 bootstrap resamples were performed and the 95% confidence interval was determined. Bias-corrected confidence intervals that did not include 0 were considered significant to determine indirect effects. Effect size in mediation analysis was assessed in terms of standardized indirect effect size, indicating that outcome variables are expected to increase according to the magnitude of standard deviations of effect size for every one SD increase in exposure variable indirectly through mediators. Effect size was interpreted as small (0.01), medium (0.09), or large (0.25) (Preacher & Kelley, 2011). Serial multiple mediation analysis was performed separately for each gender. Potential confounding factors such as, age, family type, SES, and baseline IGD score were included as covariates in all mediation models using SPSS AMOS, version 23.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Ethics

Written consent was acquired from both all participants and their parents or legal guardians following explanation of the nature of and principles of the research, including confidentiality and freedom of choice to participate. This study received a full review and approval from the Institutional Review Board of The Catholic University of Korea (MC19EAS10137). All procedures were performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2013.

Results

Demographic characteristics

Baseline demographic characteristics by sex are summarized in Table 1. Of 1,783 participants, 987 (57.0%) were male. No gender difference in age, family type and SES. Mean baseline IGD scores measured by the IGUESS were higher in men than in women (P < 0.001).

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of 1,732 iCURE study participants stratified by gender

VariablesMen (n = 987, 57%)Women (n = 745, 43%)P-value
n (%) or Mean ± SDn (%) or Mean ± SD
Age12.9 ± 0.212.8 ± 0.20.523
Family type0.934
Intact902 (91.4)680 (91.3)
Non-intact85 (8.6)65 (8.7)
Socio-economic status0.932
Moderate to high695 (70.4)526 (70.6)
Low292 (29.6)219 (29.4)
Academic achievement0.461
Good454 (46.0)356 (47.8)
Bad533 (54.0)389 (52.2)
Baseline IGUESS score4.3 ± 4.02.6 ± 3.6<0.001

IGUESS: Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen.

The correlations between study variables are shown in Table 2. Higher levels of IHPs (W1) were correlated with lower levels of self-control (W1) (men, r = 0.18, P < 0.01; women, r = 0.18, P < 0.01), higher levels of aggression (W2) (men, r = 0.10, P < 0.05; women, r = 0.20, P < 0.01), higher levels of IGD severity (W3) (men, r = 0.13, P < 0.01; women, r = 0.17, P < 0.01), lower levels of intact family (men, r = −0.15, P < 0.01; women, r = −0.11, P < 0.05), and higher levels of baseline IGD scores (men, r = 0.16, P < 0.01; women, r = 0.13, P < 0.01). Lower levels of self-control (W1) were correlated with higher levels of aggression (W2) (men, r = 0.33, P < 0.001; women, r = 0.49, P < 0.001), higher levels of IGD severity (W3) (men, r = 0.23, P < 0.001; women, r = 0.23, P < 0.001), and higher levels of baseline IGD scores (men, r = 0.25, P < 0.001; women, r = 0.26, P < 0.001). Higher levels of aggression (W2) were related to higher levels of IGD severity (W3) (men, r = 0.25, P < 0.001; women, r = 0.23, P < 0.001), and higher levels of baseline IGD scores (men, r = 0.16, P < 0.001; women, r = 0.25, P < 0.001). Men have higher level of IHP (W1), low self-control (W1), aggression (W2), and severity of IGD features (W3) than women.

Table 2.

Correlations between study variables and measurement reliabilities stratified by gender

Variables1234567Mean ± SDaCronbach's alpha
Men
1. IHPs (W1)15.6 ± 5.3∗∗∗0.90
2. Low self-control (W1)0.18∗∗117.3 ± 4.9∗∗∗0.82
3. Aggression (W2)0.100.33∗∗∗156.9 ± 14.60.89
4. Severity of IGD features (W3)0.13∗∗0.23∗∗∗0.25∗∗∗14.1 ± 4.0∗∗∗0.84
5. Socioeconomic status (W1)−0.04−0.02−0.01−0.061NANA
6. Family type (W1)−0.15∗∗−0.01−0.01−0.050.091NANA
7. Baseline IGD score (W1)0.16∗∗0.25∗∗∗0.16∗∗0.32∗∗∗0.03−0.0214.3± 4.00.89
8. Low academic achievement (W1)0.32∗∗∗0.12∗∗0.020.10−0.18∗∗−0.040.14∗∗1NANA
Women
1. IHPs (W1)14.2 ± 4.8∗∗∗0.88
2. Low self-control (W1)0.18∗∗116.2 ± 3.9∗∗∗0.76
3. Aggression (W2)0.20∗∗∗0.49∗∗∗154.8 ± 13.00.88
4. Severity of IGD features (W3)0.17∗∗0.23∗∗∗0.23∗∗∗11.7 ± 3.0∗∗∗0.87
5. Socioeconomic status (W1)−0.100.02−0.030.041NANA
6. Family type (W1)−0.11−0.07−0.09−0.040.11NANA
7. Baseline IGD score (W1)0.13∗∗0.26∗∗∗0.25∗∗∗0.26∗∗∗−0.03−0.0712.6 ± 3.60.89
8. Low academic achievement (W1)0.29∗∗∗0.12∗∗0.080.10−0.14∗∗−0.060.14∗∗1NANA

IHP: inattention and hyperactivity problems; IGD: Internet gaming disorder; NA: not available; W1: wave 1; W2: Wave 2; W3: Wave 3.

Family type was coded 0 = non-intact, 1 = intact; Socioeconomic status was coded 1 = moderate to high, 2 = low; Low academic achievement was coded 1 = no, 2 = yes.

P < 0.05; ∗∗P < 0.01; ∗∗∗P < 0.001.

aGroup differences between men and women were calculated by t-test.

The serial multiple mediation analysis is performed separately for each gender. Potential confounding factors such as, age, family type, SES, baseline IGD score were included in the model. Levels of IHPs (W1) directly affected severity of IGD features (W3) only in women, but the association was small (c, women, coefficient: 0.09, P < 0.05). The indirect effects via low self-control (W1) (a1 × b1, men, coefficient: 0.014, 95% CI: 0.003–0.036; women, coefficient: 0.016, 95% CI: 0.006–0.043) were also significant in the relationship between IHP (W1) and severity of IGD features (W3) in both genders, however, the indirect effects via aggression (W2) (a2 × b2, women, coefficient: 0.015, 95% CI: 0.006–0.035) was significant only in women. The serial mediation effect between the levels of IHPs (W1) and severity of IGD features (W3) through low self-control (W1) via aggression (W2) was statistically significant but small in both genders (a1 × a2 × b2, men, coefficient: 0.009, 95% CI: 0.005–0.019; women, coefficient: 0.010, 95% CI: 0.005–0.026). The overall model explained 17% (in both men and women) of the total variance of severity of IGD features (W3) (men, F (4,982) = 51.51, P < 0.001; women, F (4,740) = 39.46, P < 0.001) (Fig. 2) (Table 3).

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

The associations between levels of IHPs (inattention hyperactive problems) (wave 1) and severity of IGD features (wave 3) were investigated within each pathway in the serial mediation model. Each arrow with a solid line represents the significant path and dotted lines are insignificant between variables. The path estimation from the mediation analysis is shown with each arrow is the standardized coefficient after adjusting for age, family type, socio-economic status, academic achievement, and baseline IGD feature. *P<0.05 **P<0.01 ***P<0.001; F (4,982) = 51.51, P < 0.001; F (4,740) = 39.46, P < 0.001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 2; 10.1556/2006.2020.00039

Table 3.

Indirect and serial mediation paths in the relationships between levels of IHPs and severity of IGD features

PathsβSE95% CI*
Men (n = 987)
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Low self-control (W1) → Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0140.0080.003 to 0.036
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Aggression (W2)→ Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0070.007−0.007 to 0.023
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Low self-control (W1) → Aggression (W2) → Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0090.0040.005 to 0.019
Women (n = 745)
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Low self-control (W1) → Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0160.0100.006 to 0.043
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Aggression (W2)→ Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0150.0080.006 to 0.035
Levels of IHPs (W1) → Low self-control (W1) → Aggression (W2) → Severity of IGD features (W3)0.0100.0050.005 to 0.026

IHPs: inattention and hyperactivity problems; IGD: Internet gaming disorder.

*95% CI is presented as bias-corrected and accelerated 5,000 bootstrapping.

Discussion

We investigated whether and how low self-control and high aggression may serially mediate relationships between levels of IHPs and severity of IGD features among adolescents without ADHD using three-wave longitudinal data. Our findings suggest that higher levels of IHPs are directly associated with severity of IGD features among adolescents without ADHD in both genders. Similarly, IHPs have been shown to increase the likelihood of exhibiting IGD symptoms in other age groups (Ferguson, 2011; Lemmens et al., 2011; Vadlin, Aslund, Hellstrom, & Nilsson, 2016). IHPs may be related to alterations within the brain's reward system associated with overestimations of the short-term value of gambling- or gaming-related rewards (Sethi, Voon, Critchley, Cercignani, & Harrison, 2018), and these may operate behaviorally through low self-control. Since online games offer many different forms of in-game reward-related reinforcement (Lorenz, Gleich, Gallinat, & Kuhn, 2015), adolescents with higher levels of IHPs might be more susceptible to the influence of reward-related stimuli, thus increasing the risk of IGD.

Our findings showed that higher levels of IHPs link prospectively to low self-control, which in turn indirectly impacts the severity of IGD features among adolescents without ADHD in both genders. This result was consistent with previous findings that self-control mediates the relationship between IHPs levels and IGD risk (Yen et al., 2017).

According to the dual systems (DS) model of the adolescent brain, risky, impulsive, sensation-seeking behavior in adolescence is the product of an interaction between two distinct brain circuits: the cognitive control system and the reward system (Casey & Caudle, 2013). Adolescence is a time of diminished capacity for self-control compared to adulthood, in part because of imbalance between these two systems. The cognitive control brain circuit is still developing and does not reach maturity until the early twenties (Cohen & Casey, 2014). This suggests that adolescents with IHPs may struggle to maintain self-control during gaming activity, which may increase the risk of IGD.

Our findings indicate that higher levels of IHPs link prospectively to high aggression, which in turn indirectly impacts the severity of IGD features in women; however, the indirect effect was not statistically significant in men. When it comes to the gender difference in mediating role of aggression, it can be explained by gender difference in neurodevelopment and parent's view's on behavioral problem. Adolescence is a period of continued brain growth and change and largely finished maturing by puberty (Mills et al., 2016). When hormones surge in puberty, the amygdala becomes more active, leading to emotional behavior. The prefrontal cortex regulates the amygdala to ensure socially correct behavior. But during adolescence, it still has maturing to do. Longitudinal studies have shown gender differences in brain development, with females reaching peak values of brain volumes earlier than males (Koolschijn & Crone, 2013). Since our participants were all seventh graders, the majority of women might have already reached puberty, but the men would not have reached puberty yet. Alternative explanation can be possible. Since IHP was evaluated by parents, it is possible that differing perceptions of behaviors and impairment in sons or daughters. Parents, in their own ratings, may under-rate IHP symptoms and impairment in girls compared to boys, suggesting sex-specific biases in perceptions of behavior (Mowlem, Agnew-Blais, Taylor, & Asherson, 2019).

Our findings indicate that higher levels of IHPs serially and prospectively link to low self-control and high aggression, which in turn impacts the severity of IGD features in adolescents without ADHD in both genders. Even though no studies have identified the mechanism underlying the serial mediation effects of low self-control and high aggression in the association between higher levels of IHPs and IGD symptom severity, empirical studies have repeatedly found significantly lower self-control and higher aggression among adolescents with IGD (Choo et al., 2010; Mehroof & Griffiths, 2010; Nuyens et al., 2016).

Youth with low self-control are more likely to commit aggressive crimes when they become adults (Mehroof & Griffiths, 2010). Associations between poor self-control and aggressive behavior were detected in the context of Gottfredson and Hirschi's General Theory of Crime (1990), which holds that poor self-control distinguishes aggressive criminal offenders from non-offenders (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Since adolescents with low self-control are more likely to show increased aggression and rule-breaking behavior, when exposed to online games and related rewards they may become sensitized and exhibit more features of IGD.

Our results support the typology of IGD proposed by Lee et al. (2017), in which low self-control and high aggression are mediating factors in the relationship between IHPs and risk of IGD. In this context, gamers may become motivated to impulsively express aggression through excessive gaming (Lee et al., 2017). A previous study found that people with IHPs experience reduced activity in cortical regions associated with attention, impulse control, and stimulus-integration abilities (Sakai, Uchiyama, Shin, Hayashi, & Sadato, 2013). IHPs may contribute importantly to IGD via low levels of self-control and high levels of aggressive behavior. Neurodevelopmental mismatches in the frontostriatal system or dysfunctional neurotransmission may be the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these observations (Norman et al., 2018).

The current study has several strengths. First, we used three-wave longitudinal data to examine the serial mediation model. This made it possible to assume temporal precedence between variables (Preacher & Kelley, 2011). Second, we analyzed a homogeneous sample, in which all participants were seventh graders at baseline. The variables included in this model, such as IHPs, self-control, and aggression, are likely to be affected by hormones or brain development. Since the participants were within a limited age range, the effects of these biological factors may have been reduced. Finally, data to determine levels of IHPs were collected from parental reports, which may have reduced the influence of common method biases on our results because we collected information from multiple sources.

Despite these strengths, it is difficult to generalize our results to other populations because we did not include a broader sample of adolescents. It is necessary to include larger and more diverse samples to generalize study findings to adolescents more broadly. In addition, it is not possible to precisely determine the causal direction of serial mediation from low self-control to aggression explaining the effects of IHPs on severity of IGD features in the current study. The analyses cast the variables in a temporal order, based on the available literature and using a longitudinal dataset. However, without including all the variables measured at each time point in the model, the full causal pathways linking level of IHPs and severity of IGD features cannot be established. In addition, the effect sizes were very small.

Conclusion

In the current study, we provide the results that might explain the mechanism of how IHPs may impact severity of IGD features in adolescents without ADHD using three-wave longitudinal data. We revealed a possible mechanism underlying a serial mediation chain from low self-control to aggression explaining the effects of IHPs on severity of IGD features. Our findings highlight the importance of self-control and aggression in the relationship between IHPs and severity of IGD features among adolescents without ADHD. Thus, clinicians need to focus on enhancing self-control and reducing aggression when performing interventions to prevent IGD in individuals with high levels of IHPs. However, this conclusion should be taken with a caution, because the effect sizes were very low.

Funding sources

This study was supported by a grant from the Korean Mental Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Republic of Korea (HL19C0012). MNP has received support from the National Center for Responsible Gaming, Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling, and Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services.

Authors' contributions

HJ conducted the analyses and led the writing of the manuscript. HWY guided and supervised the writing of the manuscript. HJ and HWY developed and proposed the basic idea of the study. SL, HL, and MNP reviewed scientific content and edited the manuscript. HWY, HJ, SJ, HS, and GK contributed to conducting the study. All authors contributed editorial comments on the manuscript.

Conflict of interests

All authors declare that they have no competing interests except for Dr Potenza. Dr Potenza has consulted for and advised Game Day Data, the Addiction Policy Forum, RiverMend Health, Lakelight Therapeutics/Opiant and Jazz Pharmaceuticals. He has received research support from the Mohegan Sun Casino and the National Center for Responsible Gaming (now the International Center for Responsible Gaming). He has participated in surveys, mailings, and telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse control disorders, and other health topics, and has consulted for law offices and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control and addictive disorders.

Data Availability

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders –text revision (5th ed.). DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berenbaum, S. A., Beltz, A. M., & Corley, R. (2015). The importance of puberty for adolescent development: Conceptualization and measurement. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 48, 5392.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 926933.

  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Muller, A., Wolfling, K., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452459.

  • Casey, B., & Caudle, K. (2013). The teenage brain: Self control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 8287.

  • Choo, H., Gentile, D. A., Sim, T., Li, D., Khoo, A., & Liau, A. K. (2010). Pathological video-gaming among Singaporean youth. Annals Academy of Medicine Singapore, 39(11), 822829.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, A. O., & Casey, B. J. (2014). Rewiring juvenile justice: The intersection of developmental neuroscience and legal policy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 6365.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dong, G., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). A cognitive-behavioral model of internet gaming disorder: Theoretical underpinnings and clinical implications. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 58, 711.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 245253.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C. (2011). Video games and youth violence: A prospective analysis in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(4), 377391.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C., San Miguel, C., Garza, A., & Jerabeck, J. (2012). A longitudinal test of video game violence influences on dating and aggression: A 3-year longitudinal study of adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(2), 141146.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., et al. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319e329.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2014). Neurocriminology: Implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(1), 5463.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gonzalez-Bueso, V., Santamaria, J. J., Fernandez, D., Merino, L., Montero, E., & Ribas, J. (2018). Association between internet gaming disorder or pathological video-game use and comorbid psychopathology: A comprehensive review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(4), 668.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi,T. (1990). A generaltheory ofcrime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

  • Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. The American Psychologist, 69(1), 6678.

  • Hammond, C. J., Mayes, L. C., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Neurobiology of adolescent substance use and addictive behaviors: Treatment implications. Adolescent Medicine: State of the Art Reviews, 25(1), 1532.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Ball, J., Mathys, C., Brandeis, D., Walitza, S., et al. (2014). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in impaired decision making in juvenile attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(10), 11651173.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hays, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press .

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeong, H., Yim, H. W., Jo, S. J., Lee, S. Y., Kim, E., Son, H. J., et al. (2017). Study protocol of the internet user Cohort for Unbiased Recognition of gaming disorder in Early adolescence (iCURE), Korea, 2015-2019. BMJ Open, 7(10), e018350.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jo, S. J., Yim, H. W., Lee, H. K., Lee, H. C., Choi, J. S., & Baek, K. Y. (2017). The Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen proved to be a valid tool for adolescents aged 10-19 years. Acta Paediatrica, 107, 511516.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kidd, P. M. (2000). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children: Rationale for its integrative management. Alternative Medicine Review, 5(5), 402428.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koolschijn, P. C., & Crone, E. A. (2013). Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 106118.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kühn, S., Lorenz, R., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G. J., Büchel, C., Conrod, P. J., et al. (2014). Positive association of video game playing with left frontal cortical thickness in adolescents. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e91506.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Internet and gaming addiction: A systematic literature review of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sciences, 2(3), 347374.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lam, L. T. (2014). Internet gaming addiction, problematic use of the internet, and sleep problems: A systematic review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16(4), 444.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, S. Y., Lee, H. K., & Choo, H. (2017). Typology of Internet gaming disorder and its clinical implications. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 479491.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). The effects of pathological gaming on aggressive behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 3847.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorenz, R. C., Gleich, T., Gallinat, J., & Kuhn, S. (2015). Video game training and the reward system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 40.

  • Mathews, C. L., Morrell, H. E. R., & Molle, J. E. (2019). Video game addiction, ADHD symptomatology, and video game reinforcement. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 45(1), 6776.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mehroof, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: The role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13(3), 313316.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mills, K. L., Goddings, A. L., Herting, M. M., Meuwese, R., Blakemore, S. J., Crone, E. A., et al. (2016). Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: Convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage, 141, 273281.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mowlem, F., Agnew-Blais, J., Taylor, E., & Asherson, P. (2019). Do different factors influence whether girls versus boys meet ADHD diagnostic criteria? Sex differences among children with high ADHD symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 272, 765773.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C. O., Christakou, A., Murphy, C. M., Chantiluke, K., Giampietro, V., et al. (2018). Frontostriatal dysfunction during decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(8), 694703.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nuyens, F., Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Billieux, J. (2016). Impulsivity in multiplayer online battle arena gamers: Preliminary results on experimental and self-report measures. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 351356.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2018). Classification of diseases (ICD). 2018. http://www.who.int/classifications/icd/en/.

  • Paulus, F. W., Ohmann, S., von Gontard, A., & Popow, C. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 60(7), 645659.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pearcy, B. T., McEvoy, P. M., & Roberts, L. D. (2017). Internet gaming disorder explains unique variance in psychological distress and disability after controlling for comorbid depression, OCD, ADHD, and anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 20(2), 126132.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Potenza, M. N., & de Wit, H. (2010). Control yourself: Alcohol and impulsivity. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(8), 13031305.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pratt, T. C., Turanovic, J. J., Fox, K. A., & Wright, K. A. (2014). Self-control and victimization: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 52, 87116.

  • Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93115.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reisig, M. D., & Pratt, T. C. (2011). Low self-control and imprudent behavior revisited. Deviant Behavior, 32, 589625.

  • Rosen, P. J., Vaughn, A. J., Epstein, J. N., Hoza, B., Arnold, L. E., Hechtman, L., et al. (2014). Social self control, externalizing behavior, and peer liking among children with ADHD-CT: A mediation model. Social Development 23(2), 288305.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sakai, H., Uchiyama, Y., Shin, D., Hayashi, M. J., & Sadato, N. (2013). Neural activity changes associated with impulsive responding in the sustained attention to response task. PLoS One, 8(6), e67391.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sebastian, A., Jung, P., Krause-Utz, A., Lieb, K., Schmahl, C., & Tuscher, O. (2014). Frontal dysfunctions of impulse control - a systematic review in borderline personality disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 698.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seo, S., & Kwon, S. (2002). Validation study of the Korean version of the aggression questionnaire. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 487501.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sethi, A., Voon, V., Critchley, H. D., Cercignani, M., & Harrison, N. A. (2018). A neurocomputational account of reward and novelty processing and effects of psychostimulants in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain, 141(5), 15451557.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • So, Y. K., Noh, J. S., Kim, Y. S., Ko, S. G., & Koh, Y. J. (2002). The reliability and validity of Korean parent and teacher ADHD rat-ing scale. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 41, 283289.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vadlin, S., Aslund, C., Hellstrom, C., & Nilsson, K. W. (2016). Associations between problematic gaming and psychiatric symptoms among adolescents in two samples. Addictive Behaviors, 61, 815.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Lange, P. A. M., Rinderu, M. I., & Bushman, B. J. (2017). Aggression and violence around the world: A model of CLimate, Aggression, and Self-control in Humans (CLASH). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e75.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, C., Chou, W. J., Liu, T. L., Yang, P., & Hu, H. F. (2014). The association of Internet addiction symptoms with anxiety, depression and self-esteem among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(7), 16011608.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, J. Y., Liu, T. L., Wang, P. W., Chen, C. S., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2017). Association between Internet gaming disorder and adult attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and their correlates: Impulsivity and hostility. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 308313.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhai, Z. W., Hoff, R. A., Howell, J. C., Wampler, J., Krishnan-Sarin, S., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). Differences in associations between problematic video-gaming, video-gaming duration, and weapon-related and physically violent behaviors in adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 121, 4755.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders –text revision (5th ed.). DC, USA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berenbaum, S. A., Beltz, A. M., & Corley, R. (2015). The importance of puberty for adolescent development: Conceptualization and measurement. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 48, 5392.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 926933.

  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Muller, A., Wolfling, K., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452459.

  • Casey, B., & Caudle, K. (2013). The teenage brain: Self control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 8287.

  • Choo, H., Gentile, D. A., Sim, T., Li, D., Khoo, A., & Liau, A. K. (2010). Pathological video-gaming among Singaporean youth. Annals Academy of Medicine Singapore, 39(11), 822829.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, A. O., & Casey, B. J. (2014). Rewiring juvenile justice: The intersection of developmental neuroscience and legal policy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 6365.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dong, G., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). A cognitive-behavioral model of internet gaming disorder: Theoretical underpinnings and clinical implications. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 58, 711.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DuPaul, G. J. (1991). Parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms: Psychometric properties in a community-based sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 245253.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C. (2011). Video games and youth violence: A prospective analysis in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(4), 377391.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C., San Miguel, C., Garza, A., & Jerabeck, J. (2012). A longitudinal test of video game violence influences on dating and aggression: A 3-year longitudinal study of adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 46(2), 141146.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. A., Choo, H., Liau, A., Sim, T., Li, D., Fung, D., et al. (2011). Pathological video game use among youths: A two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics, 127(2), e319e329.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2014). Neurocriminology: Implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(1), 5463.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gonzalez-Bueso, V., Santamaria, J. J., Fernandez, D., Merino, L., Montero, E., & Ribas, J. (2018). Association between internet gaming disorder or pathological video-game use and comorbid psychopathology: A comprehensive review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(4), 668.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi,T. (1990). A generaltheory ofcrime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

  • Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. The American Psychologist, 69(1), 6678.

  • Hammond, C. J., Mayes, L. C., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). Neurobiology of adolescent substance use and addictive behaviors: Treatment implications. Adolescent Medicine: State of the Art Reviews, 25(1), 1532.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Ball, J., Mathys, C., Brandeis, D., Walitza, S., et al. (2014). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in impaired decision making in juvenile attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(10), 11651173.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hays, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press .

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jeong, H., Yim, H. W., Jo, S. J., Lee, S. Y., Kim, E., Son, H. J., et al. (2017). Study protocol of the internet user Cohort for Unbiased Recognition of gaming disorder in Early adolescence (iCURE), Korea, 2015-2019. BMJ Open, 7(10), e018350.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jo, S. J., Yim, H. W., Lee, H. K., Lee, H. C., Choi, J. S., & Baek, K. Y. (2017). The Internet Game Use-Elicited Symptom Screen proved to be a valid tool for adolescents aged 10-19 years. Acta Paediatrica, 107, 511516.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kidd, P. M. (2000). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children: Rationale for its integrative management. Alternative Medicine Review, 5(5), 402428.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koolschijn, P. C., & Crone, E. A. (2013). Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 106118.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kühn, S., Lorenz, R., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G. J., Büchel, C., Conrod, P. J., et al. (2014). Positive association of video game playing with left frontal cortical thickness in adolescents. PLoS ONE, 9(3), e91506.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2012). Internet and gaming addiction: A systematic literature review of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sciences, 2(3), 347374.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lam, L. T. (2014). Internet gaming addiction, problematic use of the internet, and sleep problems: A systematic review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16(4), 444.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, S. Y., Lee, H. K., & Choo, H. (2017). Typology of Internet gaming disorder and its clinical implications. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71(7), 479491.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). The effects of pathological gaming on aggressive behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 3847.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorenz, R. C., Gleich, T., Gallinat, J., & Kuhn, S. (2015). Video game training and the reward system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 40.

  • Mathews, C. L., Morrell, H. E. R., & Molle, J. E. (2019). Video game addiction, ADHD symptomatology, and video game reinforcement. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 45(1), 6776.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mehroof, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2010). Online gaming addiction: The role of sensation seeking, self-control, neuroticism, aggression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13(3), 313316.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mills, K. L., Goddings, A. L., Herting, M. M., Meuwese, R., Blakemore, S. J., Crone, E. A., et al. (2016). Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: Convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage, 141, 273281.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mowlem, F., Agnew-Blais, J., Taylor, E., & Asherson, P. (2019). Do different factors influence whether girls versus boys meet ADHD diagnostic criteria? Sex differences among children with high ADHD symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 272, 765773.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norman, L. J., Carlisi, C. O., Christakou, A., Murphy, C. M., Chantiluke, K., Giampietro, V., et al. (2018). Frontostriatal dysfunction during decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(8), 694703.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nuyens, F., Deleuze, J., Maurage, P., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., & Billieux, J. (2016). Impulsivity in multiplayer online battle arena gamers: Preliminary results on experimental and self-report measures. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(2), 351356.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2018). Classification of diseases (ICD). 2018. http://www.who.int/classifications/icd/en/.

  • Paulus, F. W., Ohmann, S., von Gontard, A., & Popow, C. (2018). Internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 60(7), 645659.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pearcy, B. T., McEvoy, P. M., & Roberts, L. D. (2017). Internet gaming disorder explains unique variance in psychological distress and disability after controlling for comorbid depression, OCD, ADHD, and anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 20(2), 126132.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Potenza, M. N., & de Wit, H. (2010). Control yourself: Alcohol and impulsivity. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(8), 13031305.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pratt, T. C., Turanovic, J. J., Fox, K. A., & Wright, K. A. (2014). Self-control and victimization: A meta-analysis. Criminology, 52, 87116.

  • Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93115.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reisig, M. D., & Pratt, T. C. (2011). Low self-control and imprudent behavior revisited. Deviant Behavior, 32, 589625.

  • Rosen, P. J., Vaughn, A. J., Epstein, J. N., Hoza, B., Arnold, L. E., Hechtman, L., et al. (2014). Social self control, externalizing behavior, and peer liking among children with ADHD-CT: A mediation model. Social Development 23(2), 288305.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sakai, H., Uchiyama, Y., Shin, D., Hayashi, M. J., & Sadato, N. (2013). Neural activity changes associated with impulsive responding in the sustained attention to response task. PLoS One, 8(6), e67391.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sebastian, A., Jung, P., Krause-Utz, A., Lieb, K., Schmahl, C., & Tuscher, O. (2014). Frontal dysfunctions of impulse control - a systematic review in borderline personality disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 698.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seo, S., & Kwon, S. (2002). Validation study of the Korean version of the aggression questionnaire. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21, 487501.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sethi, A., Voon, V., Critchley, H. D., Cercignani, M., & Harrison, N. A. (2018). A neurocomputational account of reward and novelty processing and effects of psychostimulants in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain, 141(5), 15451557.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • So, Y. K., Noh, J. S., Kim, Y. S., Ko, S. G., & Koh, Y. J. (2002). The reliability and validity of Korean parent and teacher ADHD rat-ing scale. Journal of Korean Neuropsychiatric Association, 41, 283289.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vadlin, S., Aslund, C., Hellstrom, C., & Nilsson, K. W. (2016). Associations between problematic gaming and psychiatric symptoms among adolescents in two samples. Addictive Behaviors, 61, 815.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Lange, P. A. M., Rinderu, M. I., & Bushman, B. J. (2017). Aggression and violence around the world: A model of CLimate, Aggression, and Self-control in Humans (CLASH). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e75.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, C., Chou, W. J., Liu, T. L., Yang, P., & Hu, H. F. (2014). The association of Internet addiction symptoms with anxiety, depression and self-esteem among adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(7), 16011608.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yen, J. Y., Liu, T. L., Wang, P. W., Chen, C. S., Yen, C. F., & Ko, C. H. (2017). Association between Internet gaming disorder and adult attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder and their correlates: Impulsivity and hostility. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 308313.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhai, Z. W., Hoff, R. A., Howell, J. C., Wampler, J., Krishnan-Sarin, S., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). Differences in associations between problematic video-gaming, video-gaming duration, and weapon-related and physically violent behaviors in adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 121, 4755.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2024 0 419 80
Nov 2024 0 182 75
Dec 2024 0 178 70
Jan 2025 0 207 63
Feb 2025 0 219 71
Mar 2025 0 81 39
Apr 2025 0 0 0