Authors:
Pawel Sleczka German University of Health and Sport, Ismaning, Germany

Search for other papers by Pawel Sleczka in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Barbara Braun-Michl IFT Institut für Therapieforschung, Munich, Germany

Search for other papers by Barbara Braun-Michl in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Ludwig Kraus IFT Institut für Therapieforschung, Munich, Germany
Department of Public Health Sciences, Centre for Social Research on Alcohol and Drugs, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

Search for other papers by Ludwig Kraus in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Money plays a central role in gambling, and understanding the different attitudes of gamblers towards it might benefit both prevention and treatment of gambling-related problems. This study describes the development of a new German measure of attitudes to money and the differences in these attitudes between male non-gamblers, occasional, frequent and problem gamblers. Furthermore, it investigates the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between attitudes towards money and the severity of gambling disorder.

Methods

An online study was conducted among 2,584 men aged 18–25 years, recruited via the Munich citizen registry. Additionally, a sample of n = 105 Facebook users was included in part of the analyses. Frequent and problem gamblers were invited to a 12-month follow-up. Apart from gambling participation and related problems, the questionnaire included items from existing scales measuring attitudes to money.

Results

Three factors underlying a new 12-item German Scale of Money Attitudes (SMAG) were identified: success, budgeting and evil. Compared with other groups, participants reporting any gambling problems scored highest in success and lowest in budgeting. Budgeting was associated with gambling-related problems in both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses and strengthened the relationship between associating money with success and gambling disorder.

Discussion

For problem gamblers, money is important as a personal symbol of success. This attitude has an especially negative effect on gambling-related problems in individuals who handle money irresponsibly. Spending and winning money might play an important role in maintaining self-esteem among gamblers and thus hinder their attempts to quit.

Abstract

Background and aims

Money plays a central role in gambling, and understanding the different attitudes of gamblers towards it might benefit both prevention and treatment of gambling-related problems. This study describes the development of a new German measure of attitudes to money and the differences in these attitudes between male non-gamblers, occasional, frequent and problem gamblers. Furthermore, it investigates the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between attitudes towards money and the severity of gambling disorder.

Methods

An online study was conducted among 2,584 men aged 18–25 years, recruited via the Munich citizen registry. Additionally, a sample of n = 105 Facebook users was included in part of the analyses. Frequent and problem gamblers were invited to a 12-month follow-up. Apart from gambling participation and related problems, the questionnaire included items from existing scales measuring attitudes to money.

Results

Three factors underlying a new 12-item German Scale of Money Attitudes (SMAG) were identified: success, budgeting and evil. Compared with other groups, participants reporting any gambling problems scored highest in success and lowest in budgeting. Budgeting was associated with gambling-related problems in both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses and strengthened the relationship between associating money with success and gambling disorder.

Discussion

For problem gamblers, money is important as a personal symbol of success. This attitude has an especially negative effect on gambling-related problems in individuals who handle money irresponsibly. Spending and winning money might play an important role in maintaining self-esteem among gamblers and thus hinder their attempts to quit.

Background

The role of money goes beyond the sole means of payment. It also fulfils our psychological needs of power, security, love or freedom and often plays a central role in our lives (Goldberg & Lewis, 1978). In gambling, money has a distinctive function, as the goal of gambling is to redistribute money based on more-or-less random chances (Bühringer, Braun, Kräplin, Neumann, & Sleczka, 2013). Unrath (2007) concluded that, for gamblers, apart from the financial wins, money can have various other meanings. It can facilitate social relations, act as a sign of social status and power, give the feeling of control and superiority, stimulate and soothe. Among problem gamblers, the topic of money might also be a source of anxiety because of their frequent financial debts (Braun, Ludwig, Kraus, Kroher, & Bühringer, 2013). Furthermore, some of the diagnostic criteria for gambling disorder are also related to money, i.e. excessive spending on bets, financial debts or chasing; the latter is sometimes described as the driving force behind gambling-related problems (Petry, 2003).

Vaughan and Hogg (2005) define attitudes as a stable organisation of beliefs, feelings and behavioural tendencies towards an object, groups of people, events or symbols with social significance. Several dimensions underlying attitudes to money have previously been described in relation to sex, age, education, income, life satisfaction or work motivation (for an overview, see Mitchell & Mickel, 1999; Taneja, 2012). One of the dimensions of these attitudes is the belief that money should be saved and managed in a generally restrictive and thoughtful manner. It seems reasonable to assume that responsible budgeting is negatively associated with gambling and related problems. However, the two previous empirical studies conducted in this field failed to clearly verify this assumption. Blaszczynski and Nower (2010) identified no group differences in financial planning between 127 gamblers with different degrees of gambling-related problems. Lostutter and colleagues (2019) investigated a much larger sample of 2,534 students and concluded that responsible money management appears not to prevent high gambling involvement or related problems. However, both studies had some limitations, which could potentially account for the lack of significant associations. The former was based on a small sample size (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2010), and the latter sample included only students (Lostutter et al., 2019).

Another aspect of attitudes to money is the personal value of money and its cognitive associations with success, power and prestige. This dimension is defined as associating money with better social influence and status, with social comparisons of wealth and seeing being rich as a personal life goal (Tang, 1992; Yamauchi & Templer, 1982). People who see money as a sign of success are more prone to shop compulsively to raise their self-esteem (Hanley & Wilhelm, 1992; Khare, 2016). Similarly, gambling has also previously been described as a way to maintain self-esteem for some gamblers (i.e. poker and sport bets players; Fang & Mowen, 2009). Gambling is also frequently advertised as an easy way to earn money and, at the same time, a luxurious activity typical of the rich and famous (Monaghan, Derevensky, & Sklar, 2008). It can be hypothesised that there is a link between seeing money as a symbol of success and gambling. In line with this assumption, Blaszczynski and Nower (2010) found that problem gamblers see money as a sign of success more often than non-problem gamblers. Lostutter et al. (2019) reported a relationship between perceiving money as a symbol of success and gambling expenditures and gambling-related problems. Moreover, the different dimensions of money attitudes are inter-related. Therefore, apart from the direct influence of money attitudes on gambling, it is also possible that the personal importance of money as a sign of success might be even more problematic among people with poor budgeting: they might be especially prone to develop gambling-related problems.

At present, a German instrument measuring money attitudes is not available, and the English questionnaires generally lack sufficient validation. There is no consensus on the number and content of factors to be measured (Lay & Furnham, 2019; Tang, Furnham, & Davis, 2002), and the frequently used instruments such as the Money Belief and Behavior Scale (MBBS: Furnham, 1984) or Money Attitude Scale (MAS: Yamauchi & Templer, 1982) had different factor structures when tested in other cultures (Roberts & Sepulveda, 1999; Tatarko & Schmidt, 2012). Therefore, instead of adapting any of the available instruments, it seems more productive to use multiple existing scales to derive a new instrument.

Study aims

The current study aims to (1) develop and evaluate a German scale of money attitudes. By addressing the limitations of the two previous studies, it (2) investigates money attitudes among a large sample of male non-gamblers, occasional, frequent gamblers and participants reporting any gambling problems. Moreover, (3) it analyses the cross-sectional and longitudinal relations between money attitudes and the severity of gambling disorder and (4) tests the hypothesis of a moderating effect of budgeting on the relation between viewing money as a sign of success and the severity of gambling disorder.

Methods

Study design and procedure

The current study was part of the Munich Leisure-time Study (MLS), a longitudinal online study of factors associated with gambling-related problems among young men. The study design was based on a large-scale, cross-sectional baseline survey and a 12-month follow-up with a smaller sample of frequent gamblers and participants reporting gambling problems. For further details on the study design, see Sleczka, Braun, Grüne, Bühringer, and Kraus (2016, 2018).

Participants

In 2014, 25,000 men aged 18–25 years, randomly chosen from the population registry of Munich, Bavaria (Einwohnermeldeamtsregister), were invited to join the study. The registry includes the addresses of all city inhabitants, who are obligated by law to notify the city council within two weeks after changing their permanent addresses. Restricting the recruitment only to young men was determined by the primary goals of the MLS. A total number of n = 2,588 (response rate = 10.3%) participants fulfilled the baseline MLS questionnaire. From these, n = 4 questionnaires were excluded because of unreliable answers. In the baseline study, the participants were 22.3 (SD = 2.29) years old, 58.5% were students, and 43.9% were in a relationship.

Part of the analyses was conducted on longitudinal data from the 12-month follow-up. In addition to participants recruited from the baseline study, a sample of n = 102 Facebook users, targeted based on their gambling interests, was recruited in order to increase the sample size. All participants who gambled frequently (at least once a week) and/or reported any signs of problem gambling (≥ 1 symptom according to DSM-5 or ≥ 1 point in the Lie-Bet questionnaire: American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Johnson, Hamer, & Nora, 1998) were invited to join the follow-up study. The follow-up sample consisted of n = 122 participants: n =73 from the cross-sectional study and n = 49 Facebook users. Participants in the longitudinal part of the study received shopping coupons (€15) as incentives.

Measures

Development of the German measure of money attitudes

The first aim of the study was to create a new short measure of money attitudes based on previous instruments. The search for instruments provided several options, which have nonetheless not been fully evaluated and were rarely applied in research in clinical populations: MBBS (Furnham, 1984), MAS (Yamauchi & Templer, 1982), the long and short versions of the Money Ethic Scale (MES: Tang, 1992, 1995) and the Money Importance Scale (MIS: Mitchell & Mickel, 1999).

All scales were translated into German by two translators with psychology backgrounds and back-translated by an assistant unaware of the original items. Differences in translation were discussed and revised; some idioms and formulations were replaced by their German equivalents and, in a few cases, items were excluded when no German equivalent could be found. Based on the translated scales, a base set of n = 38 items was created (see Appendix 1). This set included the complete long version of the MES, which was the most widely validated instrument at that time. It also included items related to constructs not measured by the MES and one new item ‘money has to be earned through hard work’. To ensure divergent validity (difference from financial consequences of gambling), no items related to gambling were included.

The baseline study included the full set of items and the follow-up only its shortened final version (described under Results). Participants responded by moving the mouse cursor on a visual analogue scale with the extremes labelled 1 = ‘not at all’ and 10 = ‘completely’.

Gambling participation and gambling-related problems

Gambling participation was defined as participating in any kind of 19 different types of gambling available in Germany. These included sport bets, slot machines, online gambling, roulette, lotteries, bingo, card games, gambling with family etc. The possible answers were 1 = never, 2 = more than 12 months ago, 3 = within the last 12 months but less than weekly and 4 = more than once a week in the last 12 months.

The 12-month prevalence of symptoms of gambling disorder were measured using the questionnaire from Stinchfield (2003), adapted to DSM-5 by excluding two items related to the DSM-IV criterion of illegal activities. All but the withdrawal symptom (one item) were assessed with two items on a dichotomous (yes/no) answer scale. The German adaptation of the questionnaire was previously applied in several epidemiological studies (Bühringer, Kraus, Sonntag, Pfeiffer-Gerschel, & Steiner, 2007; Sassen et al., 2011) showing a high internal consistency of α = 0.91.

Statistical analyses

An Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) with orthogonal rotation (varimax) was conducted on the base set of items. Based on the EFA results, a factor structure was chosen. The set of items was then shortened through exclusion of items with low factor loading, with a redundant content compared with other items in the assigned factor or with an unclear relation to the assumed factor. The final factor structure was evaluated in a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using the maximum likelihood method. The factor loadings and residual variances were unrestrained, and the standard errors were estimated based on the observed information matrix (OIM). The baseline sample was randomly split in half for the EFA and the CFA. Consequently, the analyses were conducted on independent samples, each containing n = 1,292 participants.

ANOVAs were conducted to compare the values of the identified scales in groups with (a) no gambling (never gambled or not in the last 12 months), (b) past year occasional gambling (less than weekly), (c) frequent gambling (more than once a week) and (d) any gambling problems (≥ 1 symptom according to DSM-5, regardless of gambling frequency). Regression analyses were conducted to investigate cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between money attitudes and severity of gambling disorder. The outcome variables in the models were the log-transformed number of fulfilled symptoms at baseline (Model 1) and follow-up (Model 2). The logarithm of the number of symptoms was taken to adjust the data to the assumption of normally distributed standard errors. In both models, the baseline scores in money attitudes were included as predictors. Non-gamblers were excluded from the analyses as they could not fulfil any criteria. This resulted in a sample of n = 1,247 participants in Model 1; Model 2 was conducted with data from n = 122 participants in the follow-up study. The moderation effects were investigated using the PROCESS extension (Hayes, 2017) in SPSS. The outcome variable in the regression model was the log-transformed number of fulfilled criteria at baseline, the perception of money as a symbol of success was the main predictor, and budgeting was included as a moderator. Heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors were applied. The analyses were conducted on the data from n = 1,247 occasional baseline gamblers. The moderation analysis based on longitudinal data was not possible because of the small size of the follow-up sample. The CFA was conducted in STATA 12 SE (StataCorp LP, 2011). All other analyses were conducted in SPSS (IBM Corp., 2017).

Ethics

The study was approved by the Ethical Board of the German Psychological Society (LK-102013). All participants were informed about the study and provided informed consent.

Results

Psychometric analysis

The EFA indicated a good sample fit with an overall KMO = 0.88 (e.g. Field, 2009), the sphericity test χ2 (703) = 12,787.10, p < 0.001, R-matrix determinant of > 0.00001 and 11 (1.0%) non-redundant residuals (< 0.05) between the observed and reproduced correlations. Nine factors had an eigenvalue of >1 (Kaiser criterion). However, based on the scree plot analysis (Appendix 2) and strong similarities to the previously identified factor solution in MES, a three-factor solution was chosen. The excluded factors were either strongly under-determined with no or only one item exceeding factor loading of 0.3, included heterogeneous items, or were very similar to the obtained factors. The three factors explained 34.7% of the variance of all answers and reflected the perception of money as a symbol of success, budgeting and seeing money as evil. The factor loadings are presented in Appendix 1. In a second step, all items with low factor loadings, with redundant content within the assumed factor or with an unclear relation to the assumed factor were eliminated, resulting in a questionnaire with 12 items. An additional EFA on the 12 items showed the same three factors explaining 59.7% of the variance of all answers. All factor loadings were higher than 0.45 (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Factor loadings and item characteristics for the final scale (n = 2,588).

Item characteristics1Factor loadings2
No.Scale/itemMiss.MSDDiff.rit−1rit−2rit−3α123
Success (M = 4.97, SD = 1.81)72
12 Money can give you the opportunity to be what you want to be.744.942.55.44.49.55−.05.00
10 Money is a symbol of success.585.512.49.50.62.69.02−.05
11 Money is the most important thing (goal) in my life.1463.532.35.28.63.72−.02.02
2 I value money very high.1055.872.30.54.61.70.04−.20
8 I believe the more money you have, the happier you are.784.072.50.34.66.81.74−.06.01
Budget (M = 7.32, SD = 1.70)91
6 I use my money very carefully.246.742.25.64.66.05.78.06
9 I budget my money very well.656.892.26.65.71.05.85−.01
1 I know almost to the penny how much money I have.338.082.12.79.40.09.45−.07
4 I show some worrisome behaviour when it comes to money.31267.512.41.72.56−.16.62−.12
5 If I have money left over at the end of the month (week) I often feel uncomfortable until it is all spent.1588.112.13.79.54.80−.16.60−.10
Evil (M = 4.37, SD = 2.19)72
3 Money is the root of all evil.744.932.72.44.464−.05−.04.62
7 Money is shameful.3583.712.39.30.464.63−.07−.12.73

Note: Miss. – missing values, M – mean, SD – standard deviation, Diff. – item difficulty, rit – item-total correlation, α – Cronbach's alpha; 1 pairwise deletion when missing values; 2 listwise deletion when missing values; 3 slightly different wording in German; 4 correlation between the two items.

The results of the CFA are illustrated in Figure 1. The fit statistics were χ2 (51) = 259.24, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.94, SRMR = 0.05, RMSEA = 0.06, RMSEA 90% CI = 0.06–0.07. Although most of the fit statistics indicated a good model fit, the CFI was lower than 0.95, suggesting a suboptimal data fit1.

Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Factor structure of the German measure of money attitudes; *significant at p < 0.05.

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00042

Owing to similarities between the new measure and the MES (Tang, 1995), the scales were labelled as success, budgeting and evil. The item and scale characteristics as well as the factor loadings of the scale are shown in Table 1. Cronbach's alphas were α = 0.81 for success, α = 0.80 for budgeting and α = 0.63 for evil. The usefulness of Cronbach's alpha in assessing the reliability of very short scales is limited because of its dependence on the number of items (Cortina, 1993). Therefore, the inter-correlation coefficient between items was used. The obtained correlation value of r =0.46 can be interpreted as acceptable (Clark & Watson, 1995).

The scales correlated with each other at a low level. Success correlated with budgeting at r = −0.04, p < 0.05, and with evil at r = −0.10, p < 0.001. The correlation between evil and budgeting was r = −0.12, p < 0.001. In order to estimate the stability of the three factors, the scale scores at baseline were correlated with the scores at follow-up. The stability rates were rtt = 0.64, p < 0.001 for success, rtt = 0.72, p < 0.001 for budgeting and rtt = 0.65, p < 0.001 for evil.

Money attitudes and gambling

In total n = 1,337 participants were classified as non-gamblers, n = 1,033 as occasional gamblers, n = 84 as frequent gamblers (without gambling problems) and n = 130 gamblers fulfilled at least one DSM-5 criterion for gambling disorder (among the latter n = 24 did not gamble frequently). The latter group included n = 30 pathologic gamblers (4+ DSM-5 criteria; n = 2 did not gamble frequently). The ANOVA revealed statistically significant differences between the four compared groups in two scales: success (F (3, 2,579) = 15.67, p < 0.001) and budgeting (F (3, 2,579) = 10.40, p < 0.001). The scores on evil did not differ between the groups (F (3, 2,538) = 0.71, p > 0.05). The results of the post hoc tests are illustrated in Figure 2.

Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Money attitudes (with 95% confidence intervals) among the compared groups; *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001.

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00042

The results of the regression models are displayed in Table 2. As hypothesised, success was positively, but budgeting negatively associated with the severity of gambling disorder in cross-sectional analyses. Evil (no specific hypothesis) was also a statistically significant predictor of gambling-related problems. Only budgeting predicted the severity of gambling disorder after 12 months.

Table 2.

Regression analysis with money attitudes as predictors of the severity of gambling disorder.

Severity of gambling disorder1 at baseline (n = 1,247)Severity of gambling disorder1 at 12-month follow-up (n = 122)
BSE BBBSE Bβ
Constant0.0810.0281.6900.436
Success0.009**0.003.101**0.0130.042.027
Budget−0.013**0.003−.133**−0.152**0.037−.363**
Evil0.005*0.002.060*0.0160.034.041

Note: R2 = 0.04 in Model 1 and R2 = 0.14 in Model 2. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001; 1log-transformed number of the fulfilled DSM-5 criteria for the gambling disorder.

Table 3 contains the results of the moderation analysis with budgeting moderating the effects of success on the severity of gambling disorder. The significant interaction coefficient and the analysis of the conditional effects (illustrated in Figure 3) support the hypothesis that poor budgeting strengthens the impact of success on the severity of gambling disorder. At higher values of budgeting, the effect of success on gambling-related problems was not statistically significant. Owing to the inclusion of the interaction effect, the effects of predictors and β-values (not reported) cannot be interpreted in the usual manner (Hayes, 2017).

Table 3.

Moderation analysis with budget scale as the moderator of the relation between the success scale and the severity of gambling disorder.

Severity of gambling disorder1 (n = 1,247)
BSE B
Constant−0.0590.028
Success0.0100.017
Budget−0.080*0.003
Interaction: Success × Budget0.005*0.002
Conditional effects for different values of the moderator (budget)
Severity of gambling disorder1 (n = 1,247)
Conditional effectsBSE B
Success (Budget = 5.622)−0.015*0.003
Success (Budget = 7.320)−0.009*0.002
Success (Budget = 9.018)−0.0030.003

Note: R2 = 0.04. *p < 0.001; 1log-transformed number of the fulfilled. DSM-5 criteria for gambling disorder at baseline; β values are not interpreted within moderation analysis.

Figure 3.
Figure 3.

The moderating effect of budgeting on the relation between success and the severity of gambling disorder.

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00042

Discussion

Based on the present analyses, a new German instrument is proposed, measuring three dimensions of money attitudes: viewing money as a sign of success, of evil and responsible budgeting. The instrument was applied to investigate differences in money attitudes in non-gamblers, occasional, frequent and gamblers reporting any problems (regardless of gambling frequency). The study also estimated the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between the three scales and the severity of gambling disorder and analysed the moderating effect of budgeting on the association between perceptions of money as a sign of success and gambling disorder.

The identified factor structure is very similar to that of the Money Ethic Scale (Tang, 1992), which is the reason why the original scale names were applied. The similarities are remarkable given that the initial set of items included items from several scales and only five items in the final version are items from the MES. Despite small differences in item wording as a result of language and cultural differences, our results support the validity of the short MES. The scale evil reflects the negative evaluation of money as a possible reason for immoral behaviour. The other two scales measure the personal importance of money and its cognitive inclinations to success and the behavioural tendencies in spending money, ranging from impulsive expenditures to responsible budgeting and savings. These two constructs fit the broader definition of attitudes (Vaughan & Hogg, 2005) and not the conservative definition, which restricts attitudes to evaluations of attitudes' object (Albarracin & Shavitt, 2018). The internal consistency of the three scales can be evaluated as only just acceptable (evil) to good, and the correlations between baseline and 12-month follow-up values speak for good retest reliability and high stability of money attitudes (Stemmler, Hagemann, Amelang, & Spinath, 2016). Altogether, the presented German Scale of Money Attitudes (SMAG) can be recommended for further research, but requires further research on samples representative of women and other age groups before being applied for individual diagnosis.

The ANOVAs revealed that participants reporting any gambling problems had higher scores on success than non-gamblers, occasional and frequent gamblers. The scale resembles the personal value of money (‘Money is the most important goal in my life’) and its association with social prestige and success (‘Money is a symbol of success’) and was also related to the severity of gambling-related problems in the cross-sectional analyses. Although significant, the associations were generally low, which might be attributed to the rather general assessment of money attitudes, not specific to gambling. These results are in line with reports from Blaszczynski and Nower (2010) and Lostutter et al. (2019), where seeing money as a symbol of success raised the chances of more intensive gambling involvement and more related problems. Gambling is advertised as a way to get rich quickly and effortlessly; gamblers in advertisements are rich and successful and are often portrayed by celebrities (Monaghan, Derevensky, & Sklar, 2008). It is possible that individuals viewing money as a promise of success, power, freedom and social prestige might see gambling as a way to fulfil their ambitions. Apart from dreams of becoming rich, gambling can also play an important role in raising and maintaining self-esteem. In fact, Fang and Mowen (2009) showed that some gamblers use gambling to boost their self-esteem, and Blaszczynski and Nower (2010) proposed that the symbolical value of gambling wins as personal success might be more important than their monetary value. Similarly, gambling expenditures can also be viewed as ‘proof’ of disposable income and wealth. Thus, in analogy with shopping expenditures among compulsive buyers (Hanley & Wilhelm, 1992; Khare, 2016; Roberts & Jones, 2001), gambling expenditures could be a means of demonstrating status and power.

In our study, participants reporting any gambling problems differed in their attitude towards money budgeting from the three other compared groups, which gambled less or gambled without experiencing problems. A responsible attitude towards handling money was also a negative predictor of the severity of gambling disorder in both cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. The two previous studies in this field hypothesised these relations, but were unable to find them, which might have been a result of small sample size (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2010) or a sample of students, for whom budgeting might play a less significant role (Lostutter et al., 2019). The current study addressed these limitations by investigating a large randomly selected sample of young men. The investigated associations could presumably have been stronger had the study sample also included older participants. The results obtained suggest that responsible budgeting might not differ between non-gamblers, recreational and frequent gamblers, but rather between individuals with and without gambling-related problems. Irresponsible money management can be a result of impulsiveness, which has often been described as a risk factor for gambling-related problems (Lorains, Cowlishaw, & Thomas, 2011; MacLaren, Fugelsang, Harrigan, & Dixon, 2011). The potential mediating role of budgeting in the relationship between impulsiveness and gambling-related problems should therefore be addressed in further studies. However, it cannot be ruled out that expressing difficulties with personal budgeting might actually reflect problem insights into the gambling disorder. Finally, the conducted moderation analysis supported the hypothesis that irresponsible budgeting strengthens the effect of viewing money as a sign of success and gambling-related problems. Individuals who see money as a sign of personal success and, at the same time, have difficulties managing their budget have an even higher risk of gambling-related problems.

The current study has some limitations that need to be considered when generalising the results. First, our results are generalizable to young men only. However, in their analysis, Lostutter and colleagues (2019) demonstrated that the major effects of money attitudes are independent of sex. Second, a large part of the analysis is based on cross-sectional design, which does not allow any causal conclusions, and the presented explanations thus reflect hypotheses rather than causal statements. We assumed that specific money attitudes can lead to gambling-related problems, but it is also possible that these attitudes change in the course of the development of such problems. Third, although individuals were randomly selected in the registry sample, both samples (registry and Facebook) can be presumed to be selective because of the low response rate. Fourth, the retest reliability rates might be inflated due to the homogeneous sample consisting of frequent or problem gamblers. Fifth, the groups in the ANOVA differ qualitatively and the last group of participants reporting any problems includes both frequent and non-frequent gamblers (e.g. irregular high-stake gamblers) and, at the same time, sub-clinical problems (1–3 DSM-5 criteria) and pathological (4+ DSM-5 criteria) gamblers. As such, this group is rather heterogeneous, which partially limits the possible interpretation of the identified differences. Finally, in the cross-sectional analysis, there was a statistically significant positive association between the perception of money as evil and gambling disorder. However, this relation is complex, and the current literature does not allow clear conclusions about it. This association should therefore be considered with caution.

Conclusion

The current study highlights the importance of attitudes towards money in the understanding of gambling-related problems. In general, problem gamblers see money as an important factor in life, symbolic of personal success, which might be a further motivator to gamble. Moreover, irresponsible money management seems to be a risk factor for gambling-related problems and moderates the relation between the personal value of money and the severity of gambling disorder: seeing money as a personal symbol of success has an especially negative effect on gambling-related problems for individuals with irresponsible budgeting.

Spending and winning money might play an important role in the self-concept of gamblers, analogous to extensive shopping in compulsive buyers (Khare, 2016). Maintaining high self-esteem is a natural need of every individual (Petersen, Stahlberg, & Dauenheimer, 2000); hence, ceasing gambling without any other alternative way to boost self-esteem might facilitate relapse. These results provide empirical support for the experience-based treatment recommendations of Gupta and Derevensky (2000). Discussing effective money management and addressing the role that money, gambling expenditures and gambling wins play in the self-esteem of the gambler can benefit the therapy of gambling disorder.

Funding sources

The study was conducted at the Bavarian Coordination Centre for Gambling Issues, which is funded by the State of Bavaria based on an unrestricted grant. Funding for LK was also provided by the Swedish program grant “Responding to and Reducing Gambling Problems – Studies in Help-seeking, Measurement, Comorbidity and Policy Impacts” financed by the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare (Forte), grant number 2016-07091.

Authors' contribution

PS, BBM and LK designed the study. PS and BBM developed the research question and interpreted the results. BBM conceptualised and developed the first version of the questionnaire. PS coordinated, prepared and conducted the study, ran the statistical analyses and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors had full access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. All authors contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The State of Bavaria provides gambling services (lotteries, sports betting and casino games) within the State gambling monopoly via the State Lottery Administration. Authors have no other personal COI.

References

  • Albarracin, D., & Shavitt, S. (2018). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(4), 299327. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011911.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Pub.

  • Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2010). Instrumental tool or drug: Relationship between attitudes to money and problem gambling. Addiction Research & Theory, 18(6), 681691. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066351003786752.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Braun, B., Ludwig, M., Kraus, L., Kroher, M., & Bühringer, G. (2013). Ambulante Suchthilfe für pathologische Glücksspieler in Bayern: Passung zwischen Behandlungsbedarf und -angebot. Suchttherapie, 14(01), 3745. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1323802.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühringer, G., Braun, B., Kräplin, A., Neumann, M., & Sleczka, P. (2013). Gambling-two sides of the same coin: Recreational activity and public health problem. ALICE RAP Policy Paper Series-Policy Brief, vol. 2. http://www.alicerap.eu/resources/documents/cat_view/1-alice-rap-project-documents/19-policy-paper-series.html?start=5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühringer, G., Kraus, L., Sonntag, D., Pfeiffer-Gerschel, T., & Steiner, S. (2007). Pathologisches Glücksspiel in Deutschland: Spiel-und Bevölkerungsrisiken. Sucht, 53(5), 296307. https://doi.org/10.1024/2007.05.06.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 98. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fang, X., & Mowen, J. C. (2009). Examining the trait and functional motive antecedents of four gambling activities: Slot machines, skilled card games, sports betting, and promotional games. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(2), 121131. https://doi.org/10.1108/07363760910940483.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS, 3rd Edition. Sage Publications.

  • Furnham, A. (1984). Many sides of the coin: The psychology of money usage. Personality and Individual Differences, 5(5), 501509. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(84)90025-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldberg, H., & Lewis, R. T. (1978). Money madness: The psychology of saving, spending, loving, and hating money. Wellness Institute.

  • Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. L. (2000). Adolescents with Gambling Problems: From Research to Treatment. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2), 315342. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009493200768.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanley, A., & Wilhelm, M. S. (1992). Compulsive buying: An exploration into self-esteem and money attitudes. Journal of Economic Psychology, 13(1), 518. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(92)90049-D.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach, 2nd ed., Guilford Publications.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (25.0.) [Computer software]. IBM Corp.

  • Johnson, E. E., Hamer, R. M., & Nora, R. M. (1998). The Lie/Bet Questionnaire for screening pathological gamblers: A follow-up study. Psychological Reports, 83(3), 12191224. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1998.83.3f.1219.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khare, A. (2016). Money attitudes, credit card and compulsive buying behaviour: A study on Indian consumers. International Journal of Business Competition and Growth, 5(1), 49. https://doi.org/10.1504/IJBCG.2016.079935.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lay, A., & Furnham, A. (2019). A new money attitudes questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35(6), 813822. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000474.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorains, F. K., Cowlishaw, S., & Thomas, S. A. (2011). Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: Systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. Addiction, 106(3), 490498. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03300.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lostutter, T. W., Enkema, M., Schwebel, F., Cronce, J. M., Garberson, L. A., Ou, B., et al. (2019). Doing it for the money: The relationship between gambling and money attitudes among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(1), 143153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9789-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacLaren, V. V., Fugelsang, J. A., Harrigan, K. A., & Dixon, M. J. (2011). The personality of pathological gamblers: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 10571067. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mitchell, T. R., & Mickel, A. E. (1999). The meaning of money: An individual-difference perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 568578. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1999.2202138.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Monaghan, S., Derevensky, J., & Sklar, A. (2008). Impact of gambling advertisements and marketing on children and adolescents: Policy recommendations to minimise harm. Journal of Gambling Issues, 1(22), 252. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2008.22.7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petersen, L. E., Stahlberg, D., & Dauenheimer, D. (2000). Selbstkonsistenz und Selbstwerterhöhung: Der Integrative Selbstschemaansatz. In W. Greve (Ed.), Psychologie des Selbst (pp. 239254). Psychologie Verlags Union.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, J. (2003). Glücksspielsucht: Entstehung, Diagnostik und Behandlung. Hogrefe Verlag.

  • Roberts, J. A., & Jones, E. (2001). Money attitudes, credit card use, and compulsive buying among American college students. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 35(2), 213240.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roberts, J. A., & Sepulveda, C. J. (1999). Demographics and money attitudes: A test of Yamauchi and Templers (1982) money attitude scale in Mexico. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1), 1935. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00241-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sassen, M., Kraus, L., Bühringer, G., Pabst, A., Piontek, D., & Taqi, Z. (2011). Gambling among adults in Germany: Prevalence, disorder and risk factors. Sucht, 57(4), 249257. https://doi.org/10.1024/0939-5911.a000115.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sleczka, P., Braun, B., Grüne, B., Bühringer, G., & Kraus, L. (2016). Proactive coping and gambling disorder among young men. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(4), 639648. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.080.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sleczka, P., Braun, B., Grüne, B., Bühringer, G., & Kraus, L. (2018). Family functioning and gambling problems in young adulthood: The role of the concordance of values. Addiction Research & Theory, 26(6), 447456. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1393531.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StataCorp LP. (2011). Stata user's guide. Release 12. Stata Press Publication.

  • Stemmler, G., Hagemann, D., Amelang, M., & Spinath, F. (2016). Differentielle Psychologie und Persönlichkeitsforschung. Kohlhammer Verlag.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2003). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of a measure of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 180182. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.180.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Taneja, R. M. (2012). Money attitude—An abridgement. International Refereed Research Journal, 3(3), 15.

  • Tang, T. L.-P. (1992). The meaning of money revisited. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(2), 197202. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030130209.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tang, T. L.-P. (1995). The development of a short money ethic scale: Attitudes toward money and pay satisfaction revisited. Personality and Individual Differences, 19(6), 809816. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(95)00133-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tang, T. L.-P., Furnham, A., & Davis, G. M.-T. W. (2002). The meaning of money: The money ethic endorsement and work-related attitudes. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 547563. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940210444021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tatarko, A., & Schmidt, P. (2012). Social capital and attitudes towards money (No. WP BRP/07/SOC; Higher School of Economics Research Paper). National Research University Higher School of Economics. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2011044.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Unrath, S. (2007). Die Bedeutung des Geldes bei der Entstehung und Behandlung von Glücksspielsucht. [Thesis]. Evangelische Fachhochschule Reutlingen-Ludwigsburg.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vaughan, G., & Hogg, M. A. (2005). Introduction to social psychology, 4th ed.. Prentice-Hall.

  • Yamauchi, K. T., & Templer, D. J. (1982). The development of a money attitude scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46(5), 522528. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4605_14.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Appendix 1

The base set of items with their German translations, factor loadings and psychometric characteristics.

No.No. SMAGItems in GermanOriginalScale/ Item no.Item characteristics (n = 2,588)2Factor loadings (n = 1,294)3
M1Miss.SDDiff.123
1Geld zeigt, was man erreicht hat.Money represents one's achievement.MES 5*5.38732.61.49.68.07−.01
22Ich schätze Geld sehr.I value money very high.MES 46*5.911462.29.55.71.12−.06
3Geld heißt Macht.Money means power.MES 306.76542.46.64.42-.05.00
44Meinen Umgang mit Geld finde ich teilweise beunruhigend.I show some worrisome behavior when it comes to money. aMAS 153.52652.41.28-.18.55−.15
5Ich beschäftige mich gern und viel mit Finanzthemen (Börsenkurse beobachten, Finanzmagazine lesen, etc.).Constructed based on two items: Money will help you express your competence and abilities (MES 25); I have explicit plans for how I can make more money (MIS 10).MES 25*3.67492.79.30.31.09.02
6Geld kann dir Luxus kaufen.Money can buy you luxuries.MES 278.09521.90.79.40.04−.17
77Geld ist unanständig.Money is shameful. aMES 323.711582.39.30-.11-.18.50
8Ausgegebenes Geld ist verlorenes/verschwendetes Geld.Money spent is money lost (wasted).MES 213.14711.99.24.14.10.49
9Lieber risikiere ich einen großen Verlust, um die Chance auf einen großen Gewinn zu haben.I would prefer to win big or lose big than to be conservative.aMIS 212.63621.80.18.24-.28.06
10Ich mag es nicht, mir Geld zu leihen.I am comfortable borrowing substantial sums of money for investment purposes.bMIS 228.16522.35.80.09.22.07
1111Geld ist das wichtigste Ziel in meinem Leben.Money is the most important thing (goal) in my life.MES 93.51582.33.28.66.00.12
12Geld gibt Unabängigkeit und Freiheit.Money gives you autonomy und freedom.MES 11*7.20462.06.69.54.10−.16
13Geld führt dazu, dass man respektiert wird.Money makes people respect you in the community.MES 20*4.89862.39.43.50-.10.03
1412Geld gibt dir die Möglichkeit zu sein, wer du sein willst.Money can give you the opportunity to be what you want to be.MES 29*4.91722.53.43.59-.06.02
15Geld führt häufig zu Konflikten.I often argue with my partner (spouse, lover, etc.) about money.cMBBS 497.32712.13.70-.09-.02.26
16Ein gesparter Pfennig ist zweimal verdient.A penny saved is a penny earned.MES 375.342292.35.48.12.27.45
171Ich weiß jederzeit ziemlich gut, wie viel Geld ich besitze.I know almost to the penny how much money I have in my purse, wallet or pocket at all times. cMBBS 88.02242.16.78.11.53−.05
18Ich kaufe oft Sachen, die ich nicht brauche oder will, nur weil sie reduziert sindI often buy things that I don't need or want because they are in a sale or reduced in a sale or reduced in price.MBBS 12.89282.14.21.19-.33.22
19Ich zahle Rechnungen sofort, um Zinsen oder Strafen zu vermeiden.I pay my bills immediately in order to avoid interest or penalties.MES 438.15552.29.79.01.47-.04
208Je mehr Geld man hat, desto glücklicher ist man.I believe the more money you have, the happier you are.MIS 14.021052.47.34.68-.06.08
21Ich fantasiere oft über Geld und was ich damit tun könnte.I often fantasize about money and what I could do with it.MBBS 576.17622.68.57.50-.07.14
225Wenn ich Geld übrig habe, muss ich es ausgeben.If I have money left over at the end of the month (week) I often feel uncomfortable until it is all spent.MBBS 152.91332.12.21-.18.60−.09
23Ich weiß meistens sehr genau, wie viel Geld ich dabei habe.I know almost to the penny how much money I have in my purse, wallet or pocket at all times.cMBBS 87.87282.09.76.15.42.00
24Ich streite mich häufig über Geld.I often argue with my partner (spouse, lover, etc.) about money.cMBBS 492.20301.61.13.26-.25.34
25Ich würde praktisch alles für Geld tun.I would do practically anything legal for money if it were enough.aMBBS 432.32181.86.15.47-.20.18
26Für Geld muss man hart arbeiten.new e7.33622.14.70.20.25.14
27Geld gibt Sicherheit.Money in the bank is a sign of security.aMES 77.96391.72.77.49.20−.12
28Geld ist ein wichtiger Faktor im Leben.Money is an important factor in the lives of all of us.aMES 1*6.97862.02.66.66.12−.07
296Ich gehe mit meinem Geld sehr vorsichtig um.I use my money very carefully.MES 47*6.69782.26.63.00.74.21
30Mit Geld kann man alles kaufen.Money can buy everything.MES 34.09302.89.34.49-.08.06
31Auch wenn ich genug Geld habe, fällt es mir schwer, es für Dinge auszugeben, die ich brauche.Even when I have sufficient money I often feel guilty about spending money on necessities like clothes etc.aMBBS 44.11672.51.35.09.16.42
32Es ist wichtig, Geld zu sparen.I think that it is very important to save some money.MES 457.54511.87.73.21.49.26
33Ich denke viel mehr über Geld nach als die meisten anderen.Compared to most other people that I know, I believe that I think about money much more than they do.MBBS 554.271392.48.36.49.15.30
3410Geld ist das Symbol für Erfolg.Money is a symbol of success.MES 8*5.51742.47.50.74.06−.01
35Geld kann dir viele Freunde schaffen.Money can bring you many friends.MES 123.12442.37.24.30-.11.13
36Ich gebe Geld aus, um mich besser zu fühlen.I spend money to make myself feel better.MAS 193.53532.39.28.30-.42.06
373Geld ist die Wurzel allen Übels.Money is the root of all evil. fMES-S 12 f4.931262.72.44-.10-.11.44
389Ich teile mir mein Geld sehr gut ein.I budget my money very well.MES 48*6.85912.27.65.01.76.06

Note: No. SMAG – Item number in the final version of the scale; MES – Money Ethic Scale (*short version); MAS – Money Attitude Scale; MBBS – Money Belief and Behavior Scale; MIS – Money Importance Scale; Miss. – missing values, M – mean, SD – standard deviation, Diff. – item difficulty; Items included in the final scale are written in bold; a phrasing adjusted; b positive phrasing changed to negative; c original item divided into two items in the German version (item no.: 15 and 24, 17 and 23); e new item; f the original MES-15 item ‘money is evil’ could not be translated and was replaced by the MES-Short item no. 12; 1all items had a range between 1 and 10; 2pairwise deletion when missing values; 3listwise deletion when missing values, the German factor names are: Geld als Erfolgssymbol, Haushalten mit Geld and Übel/Böse.

Scree plot of the Eigenvalues in the exploratory factor analysis. The three-factor solution was chosen based on the scree plot and a theoretical consideration: The excluded factors (4–9) were either strongly under-determined with no or only one item excluding factor loading of 0.3 (factors 5, 7, 8, 9), included heterogeneous items (factor 4) or were very similar to the obtained factors (factors 6 similar to the Budget-factor)

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00042

Appendix 2

1

Adding two additional covariance relations between item 4 and items 11 and 12, which was suggested in modification indices and is coherent with the content of the items, improved the model (χ2 (49) = 198.65, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.96, SRMR = 0.05, RMSEA = 0.05, RMSEA 90% CI =0.05–0.06).

  • Albarracin, D., & Shavitt, S. (2018). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 69(4), 299327. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011911.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Pub.

  • Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2010). Instrumental tool or drug: Relationship between attitudes to money and problem gambling. Addiction Research & Theory, 18(6), 681691. https://doi.org/10.3109/16066351003786752.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Braun, B., Ludwig, M., Kraus, L., Kroher, M., & Bühringer, G. (2013). Ambulante Suchthilfe für pathologische Glücksspieler in Bayern: Passung zwischen Behandlungsbedarf und -angebot. Suchttherapie, 14(01), 3745. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0032-1323802.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühringer, G., Braun, B., Kräplin, A., Neumann, M., & Sleczka, P. (2013). Gambling-two sides of the same coin: Recreational activity and public health problem. ALICE RAP Policy Paper Series-Policy Brief, vol. 2. http://www.alicerap.eu/resources/documents/cat_view/1-alice-rap-project-documents/19-policy-paper-series.html?start=5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bühringer, G., Kraus, L., Sonntag, D., Pfeiffer-Gerschel, T., & Steiner, S. (2007). Pathologisches Glücksspiel in Deutschland: Spiel-und Bevölkerungsrisiken. Sucht, 53(5), 296307. https://doi.org/10.1024/2007.05.06.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 98. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fang, X., & Mowen, J. C. (2009). Examining the trait and functional motive antecedents of four gambling activities: Slot machines, skilled card games, sports betting, and promotional games. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(2), 121131. https://doi.org/10.1108/07363760910940483.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS, 3rd Edition. Sage Publications.

  • Furnham, A. (1984). Many sides of the coin: The psychology of money usage. Personality and Individual Differences, 5(5), 501509. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(84)90025-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldberg, H., & Lewis, R. T. (1978). Money madness: The psychology of saving, spending, loving, and hating money. Wellness Institute.

  • Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. L. (2000). Adolescents with Gambling Problems: From Research to Treatment. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2), 315342. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009493200768.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanley, A., & Wilhelm, M. S. (1992). Compulsive buying: An exploration into self-esteem and money attitudes. Journal of Economic Psychology, 13(1), 518. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-4870(92)90049-D.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach, 2nd ed., Guilford Publications.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • IBM Corp. (2017). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (25.0.) [Computer software]. IBM Corp.

  • Johnson, E. E., Hamer, R. M., & Nora, R. M. (1998). The Lie/Bet Questionnaire for screening pathological gamblers: A follow-up study. Psychological Reports, 83(3), 12191224. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1998.83.3f.1219.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Khare, A. (2016). Money attitudes, credit card and compulsive buying behaviour: A study on Indian consumers. International Journal of Business Competition and Growth, 5(1), 49. https://doi.org/10.1504/IJBCG.2016.079935.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lay, A., & Furnham, A. (2019). A new money attitudes questionnaire. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35(6), 813822. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000474.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorains, F. K., Cowlishaw, S., & Thomas, S. A. (2011). Prevalence of comorbid disorders in problem and pathological gambling: Systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. Addiction, 106(3), 490498. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03300.x.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lostutter, T. W., Enkema, M., Schwebel, F., Cronce, J. M., Garberson, L. A., Ou, B., et al. (2019). Doing it for the money: The relationship between gambling and money attitudes among college students. Journal of Gambling Studies, 35(1), 143153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9789-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacLaren, V. V., Fugelsang, J. A., Harrigan, K. A., & Dixon, M. J. (2011). The personality of pathological gamblers: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(6), 10571067. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.02.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mitchell, T. R., & Mickel, A. E. (1999). The meaning of money: An individual-difference perspective. Academy of Management Review, 24(3), 568578. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1999.2202138.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Monaghan, S., Derevensky, J., & Sklar, A. (2008). Impact of gambling advertisements and marketing on children and adolescents: Policy recommendations to minimise harm. Journal of Gambling Issues, 1(22), 252. https://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2008.22.7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petersen, L. E., Stahlberg, D., & Dauenheimer, D. (2000). Selbstkonsistenz und Selbstwerterhöhung: Der Integrative Selbstschemaansatz. In W. Greve (Ed.), Psychologie des Selbst (pp. 239254). Psychologie Verlags Union.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, J. (2003). Glücksspielsucht: Entstehung, Diagnostik und Behandlung. Hogrefe Verlag.

  • Roberts, J. A., & Jones, E. (2001). Money attitudes, credit card use, and compulsive buying among American college students. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 35(2), 213240.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roberts, J. A., & Sepulveda, C. J. (1999). Demographics and money attitudes: A test of Yamauchi and Templers (1982) money attitude scale in Mexico. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1), 1935. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00241-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sassen, M., Kraus, L., Bühringer, G., Pabst, A., Piontek, D., & Taqi, Z. (2011). Gambling among adults in Germany: Prevalence, disorder and risk factors. Sucht, 57(4), 249257. https://doi.org/10.1024/0939-5911.a000115.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sleczka, P., Braun, B., Grüne, B., Bühringer, G., & Kraus, L. (2016). Proactive coping and gambling disorder among young men. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5(4), 639648. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.5.2016.080.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sleczka, P., Braun, B., Grüne, B., Bühringer, G., & Kraus, L. (2018). Family functioning and gambling problems in young adulthood: The role of the concordance of values. Addiction Research & Theory, 26(6), 447456. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1393531.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • StataCorp LP. (2011). Stata user's guide. Release 12. Stata Press Publication.

  • Stemmler, G., Hagemann, D., Amelang, M., & Spinath, F. (2016). Differentielle Psychologie und Persönlichkeitsforschung. Kohlhammer Verlag.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stinchfield, R. (2003). Reliability, validity, and classification accuracy of a measure of DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 180182. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.180.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Taneja, R. M. (2012). Money attitude—An abridgement. International Refereed Research Journal, 3(3), 15.

  • Tang, T. L.-P. (1992). The meaning of money revisited. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(2), 197202. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030130209.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tang, T. L.-P. (1995). The development of a short money ethic scale: Attitudes toward money and pay satisfaction revisited. Personality and Individual Differences, 19(6), 809816. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(95)00133-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tang, T. L.-P., Furnham, A., & Davis, G. M.-T. W. (2002). The meaning of money: The money ethic endorsement and work-related attitudes. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 547563. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940210444021.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tatarko, A., & Schmidt, P. (2012). Social capital and attitudes towards money (No. WP BRP/07/SOC; Higher School of Economics Research Paper). National Research University Higher School of Economics. https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2011044.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Unrath, S. (2007). Die Bedeutung des Geldes bei der Entstehung und Behandlung von Glücksspielsucht. [Thesis]. Evangelische Fachhochschule Reutlingen-Ludwigsburg.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vaughan, G., & Hogg, M. A. (2005). Introduction to social psychology, 4th ed.. Prentice-Hall.

  • Yamauchi, K. T., & Templer, D. J. (1982). The development of a money attitude scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 46(5), 522528. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4605_14.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand
The author instruction is available in PDF.
Please, download the file from HERE

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2022  
Web of Science  
Total Cites
WoS
5713
Journal Impact Factor 7.8
Rank by Impact Factor

Psychiatry (SCIE) 18/155
Psychiatry (SSCI) 13/144

Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
7.2
5 Year
Impact Factor
8.9
Journal Citation Indicator 1.42
Rank by Journal Citation Indicator

Psychiatry 35/264

Scimago  
Scimago
H-index
69
Scimago
Journal Rank
1.918
Scimago Quartile Score Clinical Psychology Q1
Medicine (miscellaneous) Q1
Psychiatry and Mental Health Q1
Scopus  
Scopus
Cite Score
11.1
Scopus
Cite Score Rank
Clinical Psychology 10/292 (96th PCTL)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 30/531 (94th PCTL)
Medicine (miscellaneous) 25/309 (92th PCTL)
Scopus
SNIP
1.966

 

 
2021  
Web of Science  
Total Cites
WoS
5223
Journal Impact Factor 7,772
Rank by Impact Factor Psychiatry SCIE 26/155
Psychiatry SSCI 19/142
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
7,130
5 Year
Impact Factor
9,026
Journal Citation Indicator 1,39
Rank by Journal Citation Indicator

Psychiatry 34/257

Scimago  
Scimago
H-index
56
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,951
Scimago Quartile Score Clinical Psychology (Q1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health (Q1)
Scopus  
Scopus
Cite Score
11,5
Scopus
CIte Score Rank
Clinical Psychology 5/292 (D1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 20/529 (D1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) 17/276 (D1)
Scopus
SNIP
2,184

2020  
Total Cites 4024
WoS
Journal
Impact Factor
6,756
Rank by Psychiatry (SSCI) 12/143 (Q1)
Impact Factor Psychiatry 19/156 (Q1)
Impact Factor 6,052
without
Journal Self Cites
5 Year 8,735
Impact Factor
Journal  1,48
Citation Indicator  
Rank by Journal  Psychiatry 24/250 (Q1)
Citation Indicator   
Citable 86
Items
Total 74
Articles
Total 12
Reviews
Scimago 47
H-index
Scimago 2,265
Journal Rank
Scimago Clinical Psychology Q1
Quartile Score Psychiatry and Mental Health Q1
  Medicine (miscellaneous) Q1
Scopus 3593/367=9,8
Scite Score  
Scopus Clinical Psychology 7/283 (Q1)
Scite Score Rank Psychiatry and Mental Health 22/502 (Q1)
Scopus 2,026
SNIP  
Days from  38
submission  
to 1st decision  
Days from  37
acceptance  
to publication  
Acceptance 31%
Rate  

2019  
Total Cites
WoS
2 184
Impact Factor 5,143
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
4,346
5 Year
Impact Factor
5,758
Immediacy
Index
0,587
Citable
Items
75
Total
Articles
67
Total
Reviews
8
Cited
Half-Life
3,3
Citing
Half-Life
6,8
Eigenfactor
Score
0,00597
Article Influence
Score
1,447
% Articles
in
Citable Items
89,33
Normalized
Eigenfactor
0,7294
Average
IF
Percentile
87,923
Scimago
H-index
37
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,767
Scopus
Scite Score
2540/376=6,8
Scopus
Scite Score Rank
Cllinical Psychology 16/275 (Q1)
Medicine (miscellenous) 31/219 (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 47/506 (Q1)
Scopus
SNIP
1,441
Acceptance
Rate
32%

 

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article for articles submitted after 30 April 2023 (850 EUR for articles submitted prior to this date)
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Max W. ABBOTT (Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand)
  • Elias N. ABOUJAOUDE (Stanford University School of Medicine, USA)
  • Hojjat ADELI (Ohio State University, USA)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (University of Dundee, United Kingdom)
  • Alex BLASZCZYNSKI (University of Sidney, Australia)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Kenneth BLUM (University of Florida, USA)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Sam-Wook CHOI (Eulji University, Republic of Korea)
  • Damiaan DENYS (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Heather HAUSENBLAS (Jacksonville University, USA)
  • Tobias HAYER (University of Bremen, Germany)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Jaeseung JEONG (Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Emmanuel KUNTSCHE (La Trobe University, Australia)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Michel LEJOXEUX (Paris University, France)
  • Anikó MARÁZ (Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Astrid MÜLLER  (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Frederick GERARD MOELLER (University of Texas, USA)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Nancy PETRY (University of Connecticut, USA)
  • Bettina PIKÓ (University of Szeged, Hungary)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Rory C. REID (University of California Los Angeles, USA)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ferenc TÚRY (Semmelweis University, Hungary)
  • Alfred UHL (Austrian Federal Health Institute, Austria)
  • Róbert URBÁN  (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Johan VANDERLINDEN (University Psychiatric Center K.U.Leuven, Belgium)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN  (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Kimberly YOUNG (Center for Internet Addiction, USA)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jun 2023 0 13 16
Jul 2023 0 20 20
Aug 2023 0 50 38
Sep 2023 0 49 46
Oct 2023 0 44 39
Nov 2023 0 53 43
Dec 2023 0 24 10