Authors:
Kagan KircaburunInternational Gaming Research Unit, Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK
Educational Sciences Department, Düzce University, Düzce, Turkey

Search for other papers by Kagan Kircaburun in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8678-9078
,
Hüseyin ÜnübolInstitute of Social Sciences, Psychology Department, Üsküdar University, Istanbul, Turkey

Search for other papers by Hüseyin Ünübol in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Gökben H. SayarInstitute of Social Sciences, Clinical Psychology Department, Üsküdar University, Istanbul, Turkey

Search for other papers by Gökben H. Sayar in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Vasileios StavropoulosCollege of Health and Biomedicine, Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia

Search for other papers by Vasileios Stavropoulos in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Mark D. GriffithsInternational Gaming Research Unit, Psychology Department, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK

Search for other papers by Mark D. Griffiths in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
View More View Less
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

To date, a number of studies have investigated the prevalence and correlates of addictive food consumption. However, these studies have mostly relied on models that comprised a narrow range of variables in often small and heterogenous samples. The purpose of the present study was to comprehensively examine the measurement aspects, the prevalence, and the psychological correlates of addictive eating among a largescale national sample of Turkish adults.

Method

Participants (N = 24,380, 50% men, Mage = 31.79 years, age range = 18–81 years) completed a battery of tests including the Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire (FARQ), the Brief Symptom Inventory, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, and the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised.

Results

According to analyses conducted, the FARQ had a uni-dimensional factor structure. Based on Item Response Theory (IRT) calculated cut-off scores, 2.3% of the participants were at risk of addictive eating patterns, whilst criteria varied in their discriminating ability. The correlates of addictive food consumption were being male, being younger, having lower education, presenting with higher alcohol use, psychiatric symptoms, alexithymia, positive/negative affect, and anxious attachment.

Conclusion

These results suggest that a minority of Turkish community are at risk for addictive food consumption and that adverse psychological states promote this problematic behavior.

Abstract

Background and aims

To date, a number of studies have investigated the prevalence and correlates of addictive food consumption. However, these studies have mostly relied on models that comprised a narrow range of variables in often small and heterogenous samples. The purpose of the present study was to comprehensively examine the measurement aspects, the prevalence, and the psychological correlates of addictive eating among a largescale national sample of Turkish adults.

Method

Participants (N = 24,380, 50% men, Mage = 31.79 years, age range = 18–81 years) completed a battery of tests including the Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire (FARQ), the Brief Symptom Inventory, the Toronto Alexithymia Scale, the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, and the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised.

Results

According to analyses conducted, the FARQ had a uni-dimensional factor structure. Based on Item Response Theory (IRT) calculated cut-off scores, 2.3% of the participants were at risk of addictive eating patterns, whilst criteria varied in their discriminating ability. The correlates of addictive food consumption were being male, being younger, having lower education, presenting with higher alcohol use, psychiatric symptoms, alexithymia, positive/negative affect, and anxious attachment.

Conclusion

These results suggest that a minority of Turkish community are at risk for addictive food consumption and that adverse psychological states promote this problematic behavior.

Introduction

Some individuals develop addiction-like symptoms for eating food, including impaired control, tolerance, and withdrawal, which among a subset of individuals can become functionally impairing (Schulte, Potenza, & Gearhardt, 2017). There is an ongoing debate regarding the conceptualization of addictive food consumption. For example, it has been argued that some people develop an addiction to food per se like a substance addiction (Şengör & Gezer, 2019), whereas an alternative view is that the act of eating itself can be potentially addictive and is more akin to a behavioral addiction (Hebebrand et al., 2014; Schulte et al., 2017). However, addictive eating is susceptible to promoting eating disorders including bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder (Kakoschke, Aarts, & Verdejo-Garcia, 2018), and eating disorders (i.e. bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder) are known to facilitate obesity, non-suicidal self-injury, suicide attempts, and mortality (Keski-Rahkonen & Mustelin, 2016; Schmidt et al., 2016).

In the past decade, a growing number of studies have explored the psychosocial correlates and prevalence of addictive food consumption or “food addiction” (Kakoschke et al., 2018; Penzenstadler, Soares, Karila, & Khazaal, 2019). Most of these studies have assessed addictive eating using the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS), which was developed by applying seven symptoms of substance dependence criteria indicated in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) to food and eating behaviors (Gearhardt, Corbin, & Brownell, 2009). Crucially, the prevalence rates reported in the literature largely vary because prior studies have mostly relied on small and self-selected samples or heterogeneous groups (i.e., clinical samples with specific eating disorders), meaning that national largescale studies are warranted to better estimate the prevalence of addictive eating among the general population (Penzenstadler et al., 2019; Pursey, Stanwell, Gearhardt, Collins, & Burrows, 2014). Furthermore, the psychometric examination of the measures applied in past food addiction studies (i) was limited by utilizing Classical Test Theory (CTT) and (ii) did not employ Item Response Theory (IRT) techniques which enable the determination of measurement properties at the item level (Gearhardt, Corbin, & Brownell, 2016; Gomez, Stavropoulos, Beard, & Pontes, 2019; Granero et al., 2018; Meule, Müller, Gearhardt, & Blechert, 2017). In the following sections, results obtained by significant studies in this field (conducted in both community and/or clinical samples) are summarized. Most of these studies employed the YFAS, suggested heterogeneous prevalence rates, and did not provide psychometric properties at the item level, as can be done with IRT (Gearhardt et al., 2016; Granero et al., 2018; Meule et al., 2017).

Measurement of addictive food consumption

The 'components model of addiction' (Griffiths, 2005) has been used to assess different behavioral addictions including work addiction (Orosz, Dombi, Andreassen, Griffiths, & Demetrovics, 2016), study addiction (Atroszko, Andreassen, Griffiths, & Pallesen, 2015), exercise addiction (Szabo & Griffiths, 2004), shopping addiction (Andreassen et al., 2015), social media addiction (Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012), and sex addiction (Andreassen, Pallesen, Griffiths, Torsheim, & Sinha, 2018). However, this model has yet to be used to assess addictive food consumption. The present study developed and validated a new brief food addiction assessment tool, the Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire (FARQ), consisting of items constructed on the basis of core criteria that have been emphasized across several behavioral addictions and that uses established addiction frameworks to highlight the content validity (Griffiths, 2005). The components of addictive food consumption would be: (i) preoccupation with food and eating (salience), (ii) excessive eating to alter mood (mood modification), (iii) increasing amount of food consumption over time (tolerance), (iv) unpleasent emotional/physical symptoms when not eating (withdrawal), (v) interpersonal problems as a result of excessive food consumption, and (vi) returning to previous patterns after periods with abstinence (relapse).

The FARQ was developed because the present study was a large epidemiological study in which the widely used YFAS was considered overly long to assess addictive food consumption and because the all potentially addictive behaviors in the study were assessed using the same generic questions with specific instructions to respond to the questions in relation to a specific activity (in this instance, eating food). Additionally, the development and validation of a brief new tool for assessing addictive food consumption was expected to contribute to the literature by providing another option of assessment tool for addictive food consumption particularly for studies where researchers want to try and overcome questionnaire fatigue.

Item response theory evaluation

Although the Yale Food Addiction Scale has undergone thorough psychometric investigation across different age ranges and cultural populations (Brunault et al., 2017; Imperatori et al., 2016; Meule & Gearhardt, 2014; Pursey et al., 2014; Torres et al., 2017), IRT evaluation of a food addiction scale has (to the best of the authors' knowledge) never previously been attempted. Consequently, the present study assessed food addiction with the use of a scale taking into consideration IRT aspects. It should be noted that IRT is another form of psychometric validation that can be used to evaluate the psychometric properties of measures that is distinct in both theory and form from CTT (Embretson & Reise 2013). IRT provides a contemporary measurement option, which is model-driven, while assessing the associations between an item's scores and the latent trait/factor that the specific item is hypothesized to reflect (Embretson & Reise 2013). Furthermore, it is postulated that IRT outweighs CTT for estimating a scale's psychometric aspects (Embretson & Reise 2013) in the following ways: (i) whilst estimating the latent trait, CTT relies on the sum of the different items reflecting/assessing that latent trait (in this case food addiction), IRT enables its estimation at the item level (e.g., a food addiction presentation could be estimated/evaluated at the item level) and; (ii) whilst CTT estimates one reliability index (such as internal consistency) and one standard measurement error across all the range of scores addressed, IRT can estimate the reliability of each different item, taking concurrently into consideration the different levels of the trait assessed and accounting for the different item characteristics (e.g., difficulty; Embretson & Reise 2013).

The item parameters/estimations provided in the IRT context involve the item-difficulty (or threshold; β) and the item-discrimination (or slope; α). Difficulty (β) indicates the level of latent trait required for one to have a 0.5 probability of responding with a specific rate to an item (e.g., positive in a binary item or a specific point in a Likert scale item). Discrimination (α) describes the capacity of an item to discriminate individuals of various levels of the trait assessed (in this case food addiction) with reference to the item's difficulty/threshold (Embretson & Reise 2013). Depending on the number of these parameters (PLs) estimated IRT models are distinguished into 1PL and 2PL (3PL are models involving the additional parameter of pseudo-guessing, symbolized with C; Cai, Du Toit, & Thissen, 2011). More specifically, IRT models that assume items differ considering their difficulty (β) and discrimination (α) are called 2PL models. IRT models assuming no item differing discrimination power (α) are called 1 PL models (commonly know as Rasch analysis models; Embretson & Reise 2013). These models emphasize only the different item difficulty levels across all the items informing a scale (in this case the food addiction scale). Due to the dearth of past findings and to maximize the understanding of the psychometric properties of items assessing food addiction, a sequence of IRT models were examined in the present study and the results of the model with the optimum fit are reported in detail. Furthermore, to maximize the measurement information retrieved (and given that the items of the newly developed Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire were addressed via a ten-point Likert scale), a sequence of three different polytomous IRT model fit (i.e. graded model, generalized partial credit, and nominal model) was applied (i.e., polytomous models are employed for items with more than two potential responses; Cai et al., 2011).

Epidemiological studies examining addictive food consumption

Previous research examining the prevalence of addictive food consumption has tended to be carried out mostly utilizing relatively small non-representative samples. For instance, among 178 adult Amercians with obesity seeking treatment for weight loss, 6.7% met the proposed criteria for food addiction (Chao et al., 2017). In a Spanish study, the proposed criteria for food addiction were endorsed by 72.8% of clinical sample presenting with eating disorders compared to 2.4% in a sample of healthy controls (Granero et al., 2014). Among 717 American high school youth, 25.8% of the participants were classified as being characterized by addictive eating patterns (Sussman et al., 2014), and in a Turkish study including 100 overweight and/or obese adult women, 38% endorsed the proposed criteria for addictive food consumption (Özkan, Devrim, & Bilgiç, 2017). Among the few representative and/or large-scale studies, a representative German study with a community sample reported that 7.9% of the participants (N = 1,034) as being food addicts (Hauck, Weib, Schulte, Meule, & Ellrott, 2017). In a nationally representative study among the American community (N = 986), 15% of the participants were reported as being food addicts (Schulte & Gearhardt, 2018). In another study, the prevalence of addictive food consumption among a large Brazilian non-clinical sample (N = 7,639) was estimated at 4.32% (Nunes-Neto et al., 2018). Finally, another study of Dutch adolescents (N = 2,653) reported that 2.6% were addicted to food (Mies et al., 2017). On the whole, previous studies examining addictive food consumption have reported varying prevalence rates from one study to another, and is most likely due to the heterogeneity in the types of sample and population screened.

Factors associated with addictive food consumption

Existing studies have reported mixed results with regards to the socio-demographic correlates of addictive food consumption. For instance, a study conducted among adults showed food addiction symptoms to be more prevalent among younger individuals and/or individuals with higher annual income (Schulte & Gearhardt, 2018). In other studies, the prevalence of addictive food consumption was found to be significantly higher among females, emerging adults, and alcohol users in comparison to males, older adults, and non-alcohol users (Fouladi et al., 2015; Nunes-Neto et al., 2018; Schulte & Gearhardt, 2018), although some studies have failed to find any gender differences in addictive eating (Berenson, Laz, Pohlmeier, Rahman, & Cunningham, 2015). Therefore, the specific impact of various socio-demographic factors on addictive food consumption remains unclear (Nunes-Neto et al., 2018).

It is well established that addictive food consumption, compulsive overeating, and eating disorders (e.g., binge-eating disorder) share behavioral and clinical similarities as well as neural and psychological correlates, and they are highly overlapping constructs (Davis, 2017; Gearhardt, White, & Potenza, 2011). General psychiatric distress symptoms that are associated with eating disorder symptoms are strongly related to addictive food consumption (Burrows, Skinner, McKenna, & Rollo, 2017). Similarly, addictive food consumption and disordered eating share several psychological constructs including reward dysfunction, craving, emotion dysregulation, and impulsivity (Schulte, Grilo, & Gearhardt, 2016). A model testing study, using the theory of planned behavior (TPB) and the self-congruity theory, indicated that individual factors (e.g., emotional states, psychopathological problems) and socio-demographic factors (e.g., gender, age, education level) should be associated with food addiction (Farah & Shahzad, 2020).

Most of the studies in the compulsive eating behavior literature have focused on eating disorders (e.g., binge eating disorder) and their correlates. Fewer studies have examined the role of comorbid psychopathology on addictive food consumption. This is surprising given the specific cognitive, behavioral, and emotion-regulation-related deficits associated to psychopathological disorders favoring the development of emotional overeating (which is a strong predictor of addictive food consumption [Davis et al., 2013]). For instance, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, and hostility were found to positively correlate with addictive food consumption among a large community sample after adjusting for socio-demographic factors (Nunes-Neto et al., 2018). Some studies have focused on eating disorders and problematic eating behaviors. Another study found that compulsive eating symptoms were positively correlated with anxiety among Turkish adolescents (Ünal, Aydın, Gökler, & Ünsal, 2017). Positive correlations were identified between problematic eating behaviors and different psychopathological symptoms in Turkish participants including major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, and social phobia (Vardar & Erzengin, 2011). Positive and negative affect as well as psychiatric distress are also known to promote addictive food consumption (Babayiğit, Alçalar, & Bahadır, 2013; Meule, Lutz, Vögele, & Kübler, 2012), with studies emphasizing the impact of mood on eating and overeating patterns (Sevincer, Ince, Taymur, & Konuk, 2016).

Addictive food consumption has also been associated with specific psychological dysfunctional characteristics. It has been suggested that the ability to identify and describe feelings constitute a preventive factor for individuals to respond to distress. For instance, a preliminary study indicated that individuals with addictive eating patterns had higher scores on two specific alexithymia-related traits related to difficulty in describing and identifying feelings (Brunault et al., 2018). Another potentially relevant psychological correlate of addictive eating is dyfunctional attachment. Despite the little empirical evidence that associates attachment avoidance and anxiety to food addiction, maladaptive attachment is associated with psychopathology and difficulties in identifying and describing feelings (Doina & Ioana, 2015; Lyvers, Edwards, & Thorberg, 2017). Furthermore, it appears that individuals engage in compulsive overeating to deal with their negative emotions that relate to unsuccesful relationships (e.g., insecure attachment) (Hertz, Addad, & Ronel, 2012). A recent meta-analytic study concluded that anxious and avoidant attachment styles were associated with binge eating (Faber, Dube, & Knaeuper, 2018). In line with the aforementioned literature, it has also been suggested that eating disorder symptoms are associated with avoidance over intimacy and abandonment-related anxiety (Faber et al., 2018; Gonçalves et al., 2019). Consequently, the lack of trusting and reliable relationships characterizes compulsive eating behaviors in the general population (Faber et al., 2018).

The present study

The present study, conducted using a large Turkish sample, had the following aims: (i) to advance the available knowledge by testing the measurement properties of a newly developed food addiction scale employed at both the scale level and the item level using a combination of CTT, EFA, CFA, and IRT analyses; (ii) to determine the prevalence of addictive food consumption in the large community sample based on the short screening test for addictive food consumption applied and; (iii) to identify specific psychological correlates (psychiatric symptoms, affect, alexithymia, attachment) for addictive food consumption. Relatively few studies have been conducted in Turkey (or internationally) to address these issues, and those that have been published have had significant limitations (e.g., small samples, self-selected participants, niche and non-representative populations) making such findings far from reliable or definitive (e.g., Alpaslan et al., 2015; Özkan et al., 2017; Sevincer et al., 2016; Ünal et al., 2017).

Methods

Participants and procedure

The study was carried out in 79 different cities (out of 81) all over Turkey. The sample was planned based on the NUTS (nomenclature of territorial units for statistics) classification. The NUTS is a hierarchical system for dividing up the economic territory of the European Union which divided Turkey to 26 regions. Inclusion criteria for participation was being over 18 years of age, and not having a mental illness that prevents the individual from completing the questionnaires. In each city, more than 200 and less than 2,000 individuals (based on the population of the city) were approached and informed about the study and asked for their participation by the research team (in total, 125 postgraduate students participated in the recruitment of participants). Those who were willing to complete the questionnaires participated in the study. Participants were not offered any incentive for participation in the study. Data used in this study were collected as part of a much bigger epidemiological study examining multiple addictive behaviors, some of which have been published elsewhere (Ünübol et al., 2020).

Initially, 24,494 adults from Turkish community completed a series of paper-and-pencil questionnaires. The final sample comprised 24,380 participants (12,249 men and 12,131 women; Mage = 31.79 years, SDage = 10.86; range = 18–81 years) who were deemed to have given reliable responses. Among the sample, 6.2% of the participants had primary education (N = 1,510), 5.9% of the participants had secondary education (N = 1,433), 26.1% of the participants had high school education (N = 6,355), 54.7% of the participants had university education (N = 13,333), and 7.1% of the participants had masters education (N = 1,735). Although the sample was large, it was not necessarily representative of the Turkish community. The original sample was randomly divided into two independent subsamples. The first sample (N = 12,096, 49.6%) was used to carry out exploratory factor analysis (EFA) whereas confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was applied using the second sample (N = 12,284, 50.4%).

Measures

Demographic variables: Participants' demographic characteristics were determined using a sociodemographic information form. This form included gender (men, women), age, education status (high school and/or lower degree, bachelor and/or higher degree), and alcohol use (past 30 days).

Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire (FARQ): The FARQ was developed to assess addictive food consumption (see Appendix). The items were generated and formulated by the research team utilizing the components model of addiction, which is widely accepted and used model in behavioral addictions field. The scale items were not tested using a pilot study given how widely these items have been used in previously published studies. The scale comprises six items that assess components of six addiction-like symptoms (salience, withdrawal, mood modification, conflict, tolerance, relapse) outlined in the components model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005). Items (0 = never, 10 = always) were averaged to create an index of food addiction. The maximum score obtained from the scale was 60 (Cronbach's α = 0.90). The fifth item had the lowest mean score (M = 2.13; SD = 2.96), following it, Item 6 (M = 2.62; SD = 3.18), Item 4 (M = 3.06; SD = 3.26), Item 3 (M = 3.63; SD = 3.27), Item 2 (M = 4.50; SD = 3.23), and Item 1 (M = 4.94; SD = 3.10) from lowest to the highest respectively. Item-total correlations were also high, ranging between 0.65 for Item 1 and 0.79 for Item 3. The deletion of each item would result in the decrease in the internal consistency coefficient of Cronbach's alpha. This scale was designed and implemented in the study for several imperative reasons: (a) to provide a sufficient psychometric examination, as the psychometric properties of 25-item YFAS have never been comprehensively assessed with the combination of both CTT and IRT procedures and; (b) to achieve higher construct validity by better alligning with the six broadly used criteria to describe behavioral addictions because the YFAS does not assess all core components of addiction.

Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI): The Turkish form (Sahin & Durak, 1994) of the 53-item BSI (Derogatis & Spencer, 1993) was used to assess anxiety (e.g., “Feeling tense or keyed up”), depression (e.g., “Thoughts of ending your life”), negative self concept (e.g., “Feelings of worthlessness”), somatization (e.g., “Pains in the heart or chest”), and hostility (e.g., “Temper outbursts that you could not control”). Originally, the BSI did not contain a negative self-concept subscale. However, after bidirectional translation by bilingual translators, the Turkish adaptation study included this subscale in the Turkish form. Items (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always) were averaged to create an index of the Global Severity Index (GSI) for the assessment of general psychiatric distress (α = 0.95) because there were strong correlations (that were causing multicollinearity in the regression analysis) among anxiety, depression, and negative self-concept (r > 0.79).

Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20): The Turkish form (Güleç et al., 2009) of the 20-item TAS-20 (Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994) was used to assess difficulty identifying feelings (e.g., “I am often puzzled by sensations in my body”), difficulty describing feelings (e.g., “It is difficult for me to reveal my innermost feelings, even to close friends”), and externally-oriented thinking (e.g., “I prefer talking to people about their daily activities rather than their feelings”). Items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) for each scale were averaged to create indices of difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and externally-oriented thinking. Difficulty identifying and describing feelings were grouped as a single dimension (α = 0.83) and EOT was excluded from the analyses because of the (i) correlation between the two variables (r = 0.62; P < 0.001), (ii) the recent studies questioning the usefulness of distinguishing these constructs, and (iii) the arguments on whether EOT represents alexithymia (Müller, Bühner, & Ellgring, 2003). The reliability coefficient for the EOT was adequate (α = 0.67).

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): The Turkish form (Gençöz, 2000) of the 20-item PANAS (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) was used to assess positive (e.g., “Inspired”, “Active”) and negative affect (e.g., “Nervous”, “Jittery”) at a given point in time. Participants were instructed to consider their past two weeks before giving their answers. Items (1 = very slightly, 5 = extremely) were averaged to create indices of positive affect (α = 0.85) and negative affect (α = 0.83).

Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R): The Turkish form (Selçuk, Günaydın, Sümer, & Uysal, 2005) of 36-item ECR-R (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) was used to assess anxious (e.g., “My romantic partner makes me doubt myself”) and avoidant attachment (e.g., “I don't feel comfortable opening up to romantic partners”). Items (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) were averaged to create indices of anxious (α = 0.83) and avoidant attachment (α = 0.85).

Statistical analysis

The data-analytic strategy addressed the following steps: (i) validation of the FARQ; (ii) IRT psychometric examination at the scale and item level (considering the food addiction measure employed); (iii) estimation of the prevalence of addictive food consumption; and (iv) investigation of socio-demographic and psychological correlates of addictive food consumption. Initially, psychometric properties of the FARQ were evaluated using CTT, EFA, and CFA. Root mean square residuals (RMSEA), standardized root mean square residuals (SRMR), comparative fit index (CFI), and goodness of fit index (GFI) were checked to determine goodness of fit in CFA. According to Hu and Bentler (1999), RMSEA and SRMR lower than 0.05 indicate good fit and RMSEA and SRMR lower than 0.08 suggest adequate fit; CFI and GFI higher than 0.95 is good and CFI and GFI higher than 0.90 is acceptable.

The psychometric properties of the scale (in addition to CTT, EFA, and CFA means described above) were also assessed with the application of a sequence of polytomous IRT models. The assumptions of unidimensionality and local independence or the application of IRT models were addressed prior to the implementation of the analysis. Unidimensionality assumes that item correlations are attributed to a single latent factor (and aside of IRT processes, was assessed here via the CTT, EFA, and CFA analyses applied). Local independence requires that item scores are not correlated for the same level of the latent trait, therefore implying the unidimensionality assumption (and was assessed here with item residual correlations <0.1; De Ayala, 2013). The IRT models involved the unidimensional graded model, the generalized partial credit model, and the nominal model (Cai et al., 2011). The graded response model addresses ordered responsed items and is the model of choice for Likert scale instruments (Cai et al., 2011). The generalized partial credit model (GPCM) is similar to the graded model, while it is flexible with both linear and categorical (classes) latent traits (Muraki & Muraki, 2016). Finally, the nominal model addresses item responses as ordered categories and employs polychoric matrices, therefore it is an appropriate alternative for modeling responses to items with more than two categories (DeMars, 2003). The unidimensional 2PL and 1PL IRT modifications of the highest fit polytomous model were then compared using χ2loglikelihood difference to assess whether significant parameter variations (α) occurred (Cai et al., 2011; Embretson & Reise 2013). The optimum fit model was determined taking concurrently into consideration: (i) the loglikelihood index of fit (De Ayala, 2013); (ii) the RMSEA (0.05 and lower = sufficient fit; Hu & Bentler, 1999); (iii) the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and; (iv) the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; smaller values of both BIC and AIC indicate better fit; De Ayala, 2013). These fit indices were preferred given: (i) the high sample of the current study (N > 20,000), and (ii) past recommendations suggesting that χ2 based indices tend to be inflated by large sample sizes (therefore, greater emphasis should be given to fit indices such as RMSEA, AIC, BIC; Stone & Zhang 2003). The item parameters in IRT were then visually examined via the Item Characterictic Curves (ICC; α, β) and the item reliability by the item information function (IIF; IRTPRO; Cai et al., 2011). Similarly, at the scale level, the test reliability was assessed with the Test Information Function (TIF) and the overall test performance via the Test Characteristic Curve (TCC; Cai et al., 2011). The latter concurrently enables the matching of raw-scale and trait scores (i.e., raw scores can be automatically converted into latent scaled scores). Therefore, the TCC may determine cut-off points, without the use of a gold standard, guided by the raw score that corresponds with a level of two standard deviations above the mean of the latent factor (Embretson & Reise 2013).

In the final step, Pearson's correlation and hierarchical regression analyses were applied to explore correlation coefficients among study variables to predict addictive food consumption (dependent variable) based on socio-demographic and psychological variables (independent predictors). In the regression analysis, all variables were included in the model as observed variables. It was confirmed that there were no multicollinearity via examining variance inflation factor (VIF) and tolerance values. Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS 23.0, AMOS 23.0, and Mplus 7.0 software.

Ethics

Ethical approval for the study was taken from first author's institutional review board and complied with the Helsinki declaration.

Results

Scale development

EFA with Sample 1 (N = 12,096) indicated that FARQ had a unidimensional structure. In the EFA, principal axis factoring estimation method was used. Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure and Barlett's test of sphericity (0.87; P < 0.001) as well as extracted one-factor solution (explaining 65.79% of the variance) suggesting that the results were above the thresholds (Kline, 2011). Extracted communalities (ranged between 0.57 and 0.75) and factor loadings (ranged between 0.69 and 0.85) illustrated that all scale items had high loadings. A one-factor solution was retrieved based on the scree plot in which the factors that had an Eigenvalue higher than 1 were extracted. Next, a CFA with Sample 2 (N = 12,284) confirmed the fit adequacy of the one-factor solution. In the CFA, maximum likelihood discrepancy estimation method was used. The observed indicator variables (i.e. items of the scale) of the latent variables were specified as continuous indicators. Goodness of fit indices suggested mostly good fit to the data (χ2 = 971.45, df = 7, P < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.11 CI 90% [0.10, 0.11], SRMR = 0.03, CFI = 0.97, GFI = 0.98). Standardized factor loadings, ranging between 0.66 and 0.87 (Table 1), indicated all items had significant role in the scale.

Table 1.

Communalities, factor loadings and standardized factor loadings of the FARQ items

Exploratory factor analysis

(N = 12,096)
Confirmatory factor analysis

(N = 12,284)
CommunalitiesFactor loadingsStandardized factor loadings
Item 10.570.690.66
Item 20.680.780.77
Item 30.750.850.87
Item 40.710.810.81
Item 50.580.700.66
Item 60.670.780.73

Psychometric IRT properties

The graded model estimation (χ2loglikelihood = 518269.12; RMSEA = 0.06; BIC = 518936.13; AIC = 518401.12) showed better fit compared to the generalized partial credit model (χ2loglikelihood = 523426.46; RMSEA = 0.06; BIC = 524093.47; AIC = 523558.46) and the nominal model (χ2loglikelihood = 522662.15; RMSEA = 0.06; BIC = 523874.88; AIC = 522902.15). Significant α parameter variations (as assessed with χ2loglikelihood differences) were confirmed. When the item discrimination parameters were restrained to be equal, there was a significant drop of fit (Δχ2loglikelihood = 2,464.3, df = 5, P < 0.0001). Discrimination parameters were later constrained independently for each item (hybrid models) with successive χ2 differences confirming the initial graded model (with all the item discrimination parameters relaxed) as the one with the optimum fit. Discrimination parameters for all items ranged at the very high range (0 = non discriminative; 0.01–0.34 = very low; 0.35–0.64 = low; 0.65–1.34 = moderate; 1.35–1.69 = high; >1.70=very high; Baker, 2001) between 2.02 (α salience) and 3.8 (α tolerance). Similarly, factor loadings ranged in the high range between 0.76 (λ salience) and 0.91 (λ tοlerance; Thompson, 2007). The descending sequence of the items' discrimination power and loadings was “tolerance,” “withdrawal,” “relapse,” “mood modification,” “conflict,” and “salience” (see Table 2). Considering the item difficulty parameters (β), there was a considerable level of fluctuations between the different thresholds across the six items. Indicatively, for the first threshold the ascending item sequence of difficulty was “salience,” “mood modification,” “tolerance,” “withdrawal,” and “relapse.” Considering the third threshold, this alternated to “salience,” “mood modification,” “tolerance,” “withdrawal,” and “relapse.”. Finally, considering the tenth threshold, the ascending difficulty sequence was “conflict,” “relapse,” “withdrawal,” “salience,” “tolerance” and “mood modification.” Nevertheless, the threshold difficulty parameters progressively increased between the first and the last threshold across all items (see Table 2 and Fig. 1). Conclusively, IRT analyses indicated that: (i) while increasing item scores correctly described increasing levels of addictive food consumption behaviors across all items, the rate of these increases is different across the criteria, and (ii) different thresholds perform differently across items considering their level of difficulty.

Table 2.

Item discrimination, difficulty and loadings of Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire

ItemCriterionαβ1β2β3β4β5β6β7β8β9β10Spreadλ (Loadings)
1Salience2.02−1.4−1.17−0.86−0.5−0.280.40.650.961.341.613.010.76
2Mood Modification2.71−1.02−0.81−0.54−0.3−0.050.470.680.951.261.52.520.85
3Tolerance3.8−0.53−0.38−0.180.040.230.660.861.091.341.552.080.91
4Withdrawal3.23−0.28−0.150.040.240.420.831.031.251.51.691.970.88
5Conflict2.290.110.260.440.640.831.231.431.671.942.192.080.8
6Relapse2.82−0.080.040.210.40.570.981.181.41.651.851.930.86

Note: α defines the capacity of an item to discriminate between varying levels of food addiction intensity (θ). β defines the level of food addiction intensity, where subsequent response rates are more probable than their previous rate. Spread is the range of difficulty parameters across the different likert points. λ defines the amount of variance of an item explained by the latent factor.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Item characteristic curves

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00052

Considering the items' reliability across the different levels of the latent trait, controlling concurrently for the different levels of items' difficulty, meaningful variations were confirmed. More specifically, the IIFs of “salience” provided the highest level of information/reliability in the range between minus/plus 1.6 SDs below and above the mean, placing it in the area between minus/plus 0.8 SDs below and above the mean. “Mood modification” provided considerably higher information in the area between −1.2 SDs below the mean to 1.6 SDs above the mean. “Tolerance” resulted to more reliable information for respondents in the area between −0.8 SDs below to 2 SDs above the mean. “Withdrawal” information quality picked in the area −0.4 SDs below to 2 SDs above the mean. “Conflict” provided better information in the area between the mean and 2.4 SDs above the mean. Finally, “relapse” provided better and more reliable information in the area between the mean and 2 SDs above the mean (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

Item information function curves

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00052

Considering the performance of the scale as whole, this is visualized by the TCC and the TIF figures. The TCC graph illustrates that the trait of food addiction engagement inclined steeply, as the total score reported increased (in particular from 10 to 50; see Fig. 3). Considering the information provided by the scale as a whole, improved information (TIF) scores were around −1.2 SDs below the mean, up to about +2.4 SDs above the mean (see Fig. 4).

Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.

Test characteristic curve

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00052

Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.

Test information curve

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 9, 3; 10.1556/2006.2020.00052

These propose that the scale (as a whole) provides a sufficient and reliable psychometric measure for assessing individuals with high and low levels of the addictive food behaviors. Guided by the TCC, the addictive food consumption behavior at a level of 2 SDs above the mean trait level corresponded with a raw score of 58, and based on this, it could be suggested as a conditional (before clinical assessment confirmation) diagnostic cut-off point.1 Consequently, 2.3% of the participants were at risk for presenting addictive food consumption pattern.

Correlates of addictive food consumption

Mean scores, standard deviations, and correlation coefficients of the study variables are shown in Table 3. According to Cohen (1988), correlation effect sizes range from small (r = 0.10), medium (r = 0.30), to large (r = 0.50). FARQ scores were weakly correlated with psychiatric distress, anxiety, depression, negative self-concept, somatization, hostility, alexithymia, negative affect, anxious attachment, difficulty identifying feelings, and difficulty describing feelings. The correlation of FARQ with positive affect (r = 0.06, P < 0.001) and avoidant attachment (r = 0.06, P < 0.001) most likely achieved statistical significance due to the large sample size.

Table 3.

Mean scores, standard deviations, and Pearson's correlations coefficients of the study variables (N = 24,380)

1234567891011121314

1. AFC

2. Psychiatric distress

0.24**

3. Anxiety

0.22**0.93**

4. Depression

0.19**0.91**0.79**

5. NSC

0.22**0.91**0.82**0.80**

6. Somatization

0.18**0.81**0.73**0.67**0.65**

7. Hostility

0.27**0.80**0.69**0.65**0.67**0.57**

8. Alexithymia

0.20**0.65**0.61**0.60**0.61**0.50**0.51**

9. DIF

0.21**0.67**0.64**0.61**0.62**0.53**0.52**0.93**

10. DDF

0.14**0.50**0.47**0.46**0.49**0.36**0.37**0.74**0.62**

11. Positive affect

0.06**−0.15**−0.14**−0.17**−0.13**−0.12**−0.04**−0.10**−0.14**−0.18**

12. Negative affect

0.18**0.58**0.54**0.53**0.52**0.44**0.51**0.46**0.49**0.36**−0.10**

13. Avoidant attachment

0.06**0.24**0.23**0.22**0.22**0.20**0.17**0.20**0.25**0.25**−0.28**0.23**

14. Anxious attachment

0.18**0.44**0.40**0.41**0.44**0.30**0.34**0.41**0.41**0.35**−0.10**0.37**0.21**
M20.8898.2022.8924.1421.5815.2014.3926.7814.0712.1830.4219.4660.2760.21
SD15.4229.047.578.317.294.944.747.615.363.277.976.8319.2318.36

Note. AFC = Addictive food consumption; NSC = Negative self-concept; DIF = Difficulty identifying feelings; DDF = Difficulty describing feelings. *P < 0.01, **P < 0.001

Hierarchical regression analysis

The results of hierarchical regression analysis are presented in Table 4. Gender, age, education status, and alcohol use were included into the equation in Block 1. Psychiatric distress, positive and negative affect, alexithymia, and adult attachment styles comprised Block 2. Being male (β = −0.06, P < 0.001), being an alcohol user (β = −0.02, P < 0.01), psychiatric distress (β = 0.15, P < 0.05), positive affect (β = 0.10, P < 0.001), negative affect (β = 0.03, P < 0.001), alexithymia (β = 0.04, P < 0.001), and anxious attachment (β = 0.08, P < 0.001) were positively related to FARQ scores, whereas age (β = −0.13, P < 0.001) and having a bachelor and/or higher degree (β = −0.03, P < 0.001) were negatively associated. It should be noted that the predictive effects of education level, alcohol use, negative affect, and alexithymia were very small. These effects might have become statistically significant due to the large sample size. The regression model predicted 10% of the variance in addictive food consumption (F10,24,206 = 264.64, P < 0.001).

Table 4.

Hierarchical regression analysis predicting addictive food consumption (N = 24,380)

ModelBSEβtΔR2
Block 1 (R2Adjusted = 0.04; F(4,24,212) = 242.01; P < 0.001)0.04
Gendera−1.880.19−0.06−9.77**
Age−0.190.01−0.13−20.87**
Educationb−0.880.20−0.03−4.37**
Alcohol usec−0.700.21−0.02−3.39*
Block 2 (R2Adjusted = 0.10; F(10,24,206) = 264.64; P < 0.001)0.06
Psychiatric distress0.080.010.1516.30**
Positive affect0.190.010.1015.52**
Negative affect0.070.020.033.80**
Alexithymia0.090.020.045.32**
Avoidant attachment0.120.100.011.22
Anxious attachment1.180.110.0811.12**

Note. B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = Standard error; β = standardized regression coefficient; (a) Men = 1, Women = 2; (b) High school and lower degree = 1, Bachelor and/or higher degree = 2; (c) Yes = 1, No = 2; *P < 0.01, **P < 0.001

Discussion

After using CTT, EFA, CFA, and IRT methods testing the psychometric properties of a newly developed short scale assessing addictive food consumption (i.e., Food Addiction Risk Questionnaire; FARQ) and estimated its prevalence in a large sample of Turkish volunteers (N = 24,380), the present study investigated the prevalence and psychological correlates of addictive food consumption using a largescale national sample. Being male, being an alcohol user, younger age, psychiatric distress, positive and negative affect, alexithymia, and anxious attachment were identified as correlates of addictive food consumption. In contrast, having a bachelor and/or higher degree was identified as a protective factor.

Prevalence

Based on the findings, the estimated prevalence of addictive food consumption in the present sample was 2.3%. This is much lower than some studies that have used reasonably sized samples. For instance, 7.9% of a representative German community sample aged 18–65 years were classed as food addicts (Hauck et al., 2017). In a nationally representative sample of an American community aged 18–65+ years, 15% were classified as food addicts (Schulte & Gearhardt, 2018). The prevalence of food addiction in a larger sample of Dutch adolescents was 2.6% (Mies et al., 2017), whereas it was 4.3% in a large Brazilian non-clinical sample aged 18–30+ years (Nunes-Neto et al., 2018). Even though addictive food consumption appears to be present among some individuals in developing countries (e.g., low and middle-income countries), it is not as prevalent as in more developed countries. Moreover, it should be taken into account that the present sample size was much bigger than previous representative and largescale studies, and a possible difference compared to previous studies would have arisen from the different scale used in the present study.

Scale and items: discrimination, difficulty and reliability

IRT findings, EFA, and CFA confirmed the uni-dimensionality of the behavior and indicated variations considering the discrimination, difficulty, and information functions of the items across the different thresholds taking into consideration the different levels of the underlying trait. More specifically, the descending succession of the items' discrimination power was “tolerance” “withdrawal,” “relapse,” “mood modification,” “conflict” and “salience.” This corresponds with past IRT studies considering different forms of excessive behaviors that also advocated different discrimination power across the criteria assessed (Gomez et al., 2019). Furthermore, whilst the item difficulty parameter gradually increased between the first and the last point of the Likert scale employed across all items, the sequence of items' difficulties differed considering the different item thresholds. Considering the final threshold, which reflected the higher level of the behavior, the ascending difficulty sequence was “conflict,” “relapse,” “withdrawal,” “salience,” “tolerance” and “mood modification.” Therefore, it is proposed that different items should be interpreted/considered with different clinical importance when it comes to assessment of this potentially problematic behavior.

Furthermore, differences were identified considering the level of information precision provided by each of the criteria. More specifically, findings showed that “salience” provided the highest level of information/reliability in the range between minus/plus 1.6 SDs below and above the mean. “Mood modification” provided considerably higher information in the area between −1.2 SDs below the mean to 1.6SDs above the mean. “Tolerance” resulted in more reliable information for participants in the area between −0.8 SDs below to 2 SDs above the mean. “Withdrawal” provided better information in the area of −0.4 SDs below to 2 SDs above the mean. “Conflict” provided better information in the area between the mean and 2.4 SDs above the mean. Finally, “relapse” provided better and more reliable information in the area between the mean and 2 SDs above the mean. While all the criteria tended to provide better information between the mean and 2 SDs above the mean, the level of information tended to be better for “tolerance” and “withdrawal”. Considering the scale, improved information (TIF) performance was found around −1.2 SDs below the mean, and up to about +2.4 SDs above the mean.

Nevertheless, differences occured considering the specific items' information capacity. More specifically, for higher food addiction severity (2 SDs above the latent mean), the following descending three-item sequence should be considered prioritized: (a) tolerance; (b) withdrawal, and (c) relapse. Interestingly, for those with lower level of food addiction severity (2–3 SDs below the latent mean), first salience, and then mood modification, appear to provide more reliable information compared to other items (although their overall item information capacity was more symmetrically distributed across high and low levels), while conflict and relapse should be omitted given their almost zero information potential (for those with lower levels of food addiction, 2 to 3 SDs below the latent mean). İn relation to the scale as a whole, food addiction behavior inclined steeply, as the total score reported tended to elevate. This favors FARQ as a sufficient psychometric measure for assessing individuals with high and low levels of food addiction behavior.2

Food addiction correlates

Psychiatric symptoms were positively related to addictive food consumption. This is consistent with the prior studies asserting that psychiatric distress is positively correlated with addictive food consumption (Nunes-Neto et al., 2018). Addictive food consumption with binge eating may be an indicator of a higher risk of comorbid psychopathology and emotional disturbances (Imperatori et al., 2014). It may be that individuals use addictive food consumption as a means to alleviate negative emotions related to individuals' emotion dysregulation and mood disorder symptoms (Gearhardt et al., 2012).

As expected, negative affect was found to be positively associated with addictive food consumption. Negative affect exacerbating addictive food consumption is consistent with the prior literature reporting that experiencing negative emotional distress elevates food consumption because emotional distress diminishes self-control by enhancing the brain's reward sensitivity to appetitive stimuli (Wagner, Boswell, Kelley, & Heatherton, 2012). Positive affect was positively related to addictive food consumption. This contradicts previous studies suggesting that positive mood is a protective influence in reducing overeating (Cardi, Leppanen, Leslie, Esposito, & Treasure, 2019). However, the present finding is in line with a number of studies showing that men have higher positive affect-induced smoking than women, and that affective triggers may vary in addictive behaviors (Messer, Siegel, Bertin, & Erblich, 2018). A recent meta-analysis study supported this notion reporting that while negative emotions increased eating in restrained eaters, positive emotions increased eating more generally (Evers, Dingemans, Junghans, & Boeve, 2018). Furthermore, both positive and negative affect being related to elevated addictive food consumption may be explained with age. While emerging adults in the study had higher negative affect, older participants had significantly higher positive affect, indicating that younger and older individuals engaged in higher addictive food consumption with different emotional states.

Results showed that alexithymia was positively and significantly related to addictive food consumption. This is in line with one of the few studies on the subject which indicated that scores on both difficulty describing and identifying feelings were significantly higher among food addicts compared to those without addictive food consumption (Brunault et al., 2018). However, this study did not adjust for the interactions among these subdimensions and other psychopathological factors. The present study found that the ability to identify and describe one's own feelings appeared to be a robust protective factor against developing addictive food consumption.

Anxious attachment (but not avoidant attachment) was positively related to more addictive food consumption. It may be that anxiously-attached individuals attempt to cope with distress and negative emotions by relying on emotion-focused coping, whereas avoidant individuals deal with their negative state by distancing coping, and responding to stress with increased somatization instead of elevated anxiety and depression (Mikulincer & Orbach, 1995). Therefore, the present finding that only attachment anxiety was related to addictive food consumption is supported by the notion that emotional eating and using food for positive mood modification are associated with addictive food consumption (Gearhardt et al., 2012; Gonçalves et al., 2019). Consequently, being unable to form trusting and reliable relationships with significant others appears to predict unhealthy eating behaviors (Faber et al., 2018).

Unlike existing studies which generally identified higher addictive food consumption and eating disorder symptoms in women (Berenson et al., 2015; Fouladi et al., 2015; Nunes-Neto et al., 2018; Schulte & Gearhardt, 2018), men reported higher addictive pattern of eating in the current study. This unexpected finding may be related to the assessment tool used, directly derived from the component model of addiction (Griffiths, 2005), whereas most previous studies reported gender differences based on the YFAS (Pursey et al., 2014) which does not assess all aspect of component model of addiction (e.g., salience).

Limitations

The present study has some limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. First, even though the sample size was very large, it was not technically a nationally representative of Turkish community (although given the sampling points was a good proxy), which prevents the generalizability of the findings. Future studies should replicate the present findings recruiting more representative study groups from Turkey and/or using samples that represent other developing countries of which food addiction has been less examined. Second, the present study adopted a cross-sectional design, which prevents the determination of causal relationships among variables. Future studies should test the present findings using more in-depth methods including longitudinal and qualitative designs. Third, self-report questionnaires used in the present study for data collection are susceptible to various methodological biases including those relating to social desirability and memory recall. Finally, some of the scale items combine multiple statements, therefore some participants may have had some difficulty in specifically determining whether their given responses related to specific statements. Furthermore, phrasing of the items were deliberately very general (because the same items were used to assess multiple addictive behaviors) and only the instructions at the beginning of the scale stated that these statements were related to food consumption behavior rather than the items themselves. In addition to these, validity of the scale needs to be been determined by examining the association of the new scale and other food addiction scales (e.g., YFAS). These aforementioned limitations should be taken into account when using the newly developed scale.

Conclusion

Despite these limitations, the present study is the first to investigate the discrimination power of the different food addiction criteria, alongside the prevalence and the psychological correlates of addictive food consumption using a very large Turkish community sample. A small group of participants were identified as at risk for addictive food consumption (2.3%). Addictive food consumption was associated with psychiatric symptoms (i.e. anxiety, hostility), difficulty identifying feelings, affect, and anxious attachment while adjusting for socio-demographics. These findings indicate that using a wide range of variables is important to have a clearer understanding of the possible mechanisms that may exacerbate individuals' addictive food consumption. Future studies should consider possible mediating and moderating effects of psychological variables to better explain this phenomenon. Even though the present results provide valuable data, further studies are needed to use these findings to develop effective prevention and intervention strategies.

Funding sources

No financial support was received for this study.

Authors' contribution

MDG, VS, and KK contributed to the design of the study, analysis of data, and writing of the paper. HÜ and GHS organized the participant recruitment and collected data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • Alpaslan, A. H., Soylu, N., Kadriye, A. V. C. I., Coşkun, K. Ş., Kocak, U., & Taş, H. U. (2015). Disordered eating attitudes, alexithymia and suicide probability among Turkish high school girls. Psychiatry Research, 226, 224229.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Pallesen, S., Bilder, R. M., Torsheim, T., & Aboujaoude, E. (2015). The Bergen shopping addiction scale: Reliability and validity of a brief screening test. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1374.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., Griffiths, M. D., Torsheim, T., & Sinha, R. (2018). The development and validation of the Bergen–Yale Sex Addiction Scale with a large national sample. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 144.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Torsheim, T., Brunborg, G. S., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a Facebook addiction scale. Psychological Reports, 110, 501517.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Atroszko, P. A., Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., & Pallesen, S. (2015). Study addiction—A new area of psychological study: Conceptualization, assessment, and preliminary empirical findings. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4, 7584.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Babayiğit, Z., Alçalar, N., & Bahadir, G. (2013). Investigation of psychopathology and cognitive aspects of binge eating disorder in obese women. Turkiye Klinikleri Journal of Medical Sciences, 33, 10771087.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 3340.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baker, F. B. (2001). The basics of item response theory. ERIC Clearinghouse on Assessment and Evaluation. Retrieved 15 April, 2020, from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED458219.pdf.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berenson, A. B., Laz, T. H., Pohlmeier, A. M., Rahman, M., & Cunningham, K. A. (2015). Prevalence of food addiction among low-income reproductive-aged women. Journal of Women's Health, 24, 740744.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brunault, P., Courtois, R., Gearhardt, A. N., Gaillard, P., Journiac, K., Cathelain, S., et al. (2017). Validation of the French version of the DSM-5 Yale food addiction scale in a nonclinical sample. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62, 199210.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brunault, P., Ducluzeau, P. H., Courtois, R., Bourbao-Tournois, C., Delbachian, I., Réveillère, C., et al. (2018). Food addiction is associated with higher neuroticism, lower conscientiousness, higher impulsivity, but lower extraversion in obese patient candidates for bariatric surgery. Substance Use & Misuse, 53, 19191923.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Burrows, T., Skinner, J., McKenna, R., & Rollo, M. (2017). Food addiction, binge eating disorder, and obesity: Is there a relationship?. Behavioral Sciences, 7, e54.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cai, L., Du Toit, S. H. C., & Thissen, D. (2011). IRTPRO: Flexible, multidimensional, multiple categorical IRT modeling [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cardi, V., Leppanen, J., Leslie, M., Esposito, M., & Treasure, J. (2019). The use of a positive mood induction video-clip to target eating behaviour in people with bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: An experimental study. Appetite, 133, 400404.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chao, A. M., Shaw, J. A., Pearl, R. L., Alamuddin, N., Hopkins, C. M., Bakizada, Z. M., et al. (2017). Prevalence and psychosocial correlates of food addiction in persons with obesity seeking weight reduction. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 73, 97104.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Davis, C. (2017). A commentary on the associations among ‘food addiction’, binge eating disorder, and obesity: Overlapping conditions with idiosyncratic clinical features. Appetite, 115, 38.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, C., Loxton, N. J., Levitan, R. D., Kaplan, A. S., Carter, J. C., & Kennedy, J. L. (2013). ‘Food addiction’and its association with a dopaminergic multilocus genetic profile. Physiology & Behavior, 118, 6369.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • De Ayala, R. J. (2013). The theory and practice of item response theory. Guilford Publications.

  • DeMars, C. E. (2003). Sample size and the recovery of nominal response model item parameters. Applied Psychological Measurement, 27, 275288.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Derogatis, L. R., & Spencer, P. M. (1993). Brief symptom inventory: BSI. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

  • Doina & Ioana, A. (2015). The quality of attachment relationships and alexithymia. Journal of Education, 7, 130141.

  • Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2013). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Evers, C., Dingemans, A., Junghans, A. F., & Boevé, A. (2018). Feeling bad or feeling good, does emotion affect your consumption of food? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 195208.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faber, A., Dube, L., & Knaeuper, B. (2018). Attachment and eating: A meta-analytic review of the relevance of attachment for unhealthy and healthy eating behaviors in the general population. Appetite, 123, 410438.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farah, M. F., & Shahzad, M. F. (2020). Fast‐food addiction and anti‐consumption behaviour: The moderating role of consumer social responsibility. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 44, 379392.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fouladi, F., Mitchell, J. E., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., Crow, S., Hill, L., et al. (2015). Prevalence of alcohol and other substance use in patients with eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 23, 531536.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 350364.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Preliminary validation of the Yale food addiction scale. Appetite, 52, 430436.

  • Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2016). Development of the Yale food addiction scale version 2.0. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30, 113121.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A. N., White, M. A., Masheb, R. M., Morgan, P. T., Crosby, R. D., & Grilo, C. M. (2012). An examination of the food addiction construct in obese patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 657663.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A., White, M. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2011). Binge eating disorder and food addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 4, 201207.

  • Gençöz, T. (2000). Positive and negative affect Schedule: A study of validity and reliability. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 15, 1926.

  • Gomez, R., Stavropoulos, V., Beard, C., & Pontes, H. M. (2019). Item response theory analysis of the recoded internet gaming disorder scale-short-form (IGDS9-SF). International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17, 859879.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gonçalves, S., Vieira, A. I., Rodrigues, T., Machado, P. P., Brandão, I., Timóteo, S., et al. (2019). Adult attachment in eating disorders mediates the association between perceived invalidating childhood environments and eating psychopathology. Current Psychology. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00524-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Granero, R., Hilker, I., Agüera, Z., Jiménez‐Murcia, S., Sauchelli, S., Islam, M. A., et al. (2014). Food addiction in a Spanish sample of eating disorders: DSM‐5 diagnostic subtype differentiation and validation data. European Eating Disorders Review, 22, 389396.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Granero, R., Jiménez-Murcia, S., Gearhardt, A. N., Agüera, Z., Aymamí, N., Gómez-Peña, M., et al. (2018). Validation of the Spanish version of the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0 (YFAS 2.0) and clinical correlates in a sample of eating disorder, gambling disorder, and healthy control participants. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, e208.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10, 191197.

  • Güleç, H., Köse, S., Güleç, M. Y., Çitak, S., Evren, C., Borckardt, J., et al. (2009). Reliability and factorial validity of the Turkish version of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20). Klinik Psikofarmakoloji Bülteni, 19, 214220.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hauck, C., Weiß, A., Schulte, E. M., Meule, A., & Ellrott, T. (2017). Prevalence of ‘food addiction' as measured with the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0 in a representative German sample and its association with sex, age and weight categories. Obesity Facts, 10, 1224.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hebebrand, J., Albayrak, Ö., Adan, R., Antel, J., Dieguez, C., de Jong, J., et al. (2014). “Eating addiction”, rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 295306.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hertz, P., Addad, M., & Ronel, N. (2012). Attachment styles and changes among women members of overeaters anonymous who have recovered from binge-eating disorder. Health & Social Work, 37, 110122.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives, Structural Equation Modelling, 6, 155.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Imperatori, C., Fabbricatore, M., Vumbaca, V., Innamorati, M., Contardi, A., & Farina, B. (2016). Food Addiction: Definition, measurement and prevalence in healthy subjects and in patients with eating disorders. Rivista di Psichiatria, 51, 6065.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Imperatori, C., Innamorati, M., Contardi, A., Continisio, M., Tamburello, S., Lamis, D. A., et al. (2014). The association among food addiction, binge eating severity and psychopathology in obese and overweight patients attending low-energy-diet therapy. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55, 13581362.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kakoschke, N., Aarts, E., & Verdejo-García, A. (2018). The cognitive drivers of compulsive eating behavior: A mini-review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, e338.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keski-Rahkonen, A., & Mustelin, L. (2016). Epidemiology of eating disorders in Europe: Prevalence, incidence, comorbidity, course, consequences, and risk factors. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29, 340345.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.

  • Lyvers, M., Edwards, M., & Thorberg, F. (2017). Alexithymia, attachment and fear of intimacy in young adults. IAFOR Journal of Psychology & the Behavioral Sciences, 3, 111.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Messer, S., Siegel, A., Bertin, L., & Erblich, J. (2018). Sex differences in effect-triggered lapses during smoking cessation: A daily diary study. Addictive Behaviors, 87, 8285.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2014). Five years of the Yale food addiction scale: Taking stock and moving forward. Current Addiction Reports, 1, 193205.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Lutz, A., Vögele, C., & Kübler, A. (2012). Food cravings discriminate differentially between successful and unsuccessful dieters and non-dieters. Validation of the Food Cravings Questionnaires in German. Appetite, 58, 8897.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Müller, A., Gearhardt, A. N., & Blechert, J. (2017). German version of the Yale food addiction scale 2.0: Prevalence and correlates of ‘food addiction’ in students and obese individuals. Appetite, 115, 5461.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mies, G. W., Treur, J. L., Larsen, J. K., Halberstadt, J., Pasman, J. A., & Vink, J. M. (2017). The prevalence of food addiction in a large sample of adolescents and its association with addictive substances. Appetite, 118, 97105.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M., & Orbach, I. (1995). Attachment styles and repressive defensiveness: The accessibility and architecture of affective memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 917925.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Müller, J., Bühner, M., & Ellgring, H. (2003). Is there a reliable factorial structure in the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale?: A comparison of factor models in clinical and normal adult samples. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 561568.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Muraki, E., & Muraki, M. (2016). Generalized partial credit model. In Handbook of item response theory, Vol. 1 (pp. 155166). Chapman and Hall/CRC.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nunes-Neto, P. R., Köhler, C. A., Schuch, F. B., Solmi, M., Quevedo, J., Maes, M., et al. (2018). Food addiction: Prevalence, psychopathological correlates and associations with quality of life in a large sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 96, 145152.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orosz, G., Dombi, E., Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2016). Analyzing models of work addiction: Single factor and bi-factor models of the Bergen Work Addiction Scale. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14, 662671.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Özkan, İ., Devrim, A., & Bilgiç, P. (2017). Evaluation of the relationship between food addiction, nutritional status and depression in overweight and obese women. Journal of Nutrition and Dietetics, 45, 242249.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Penzenstadler, L., Soares, C., Karila, L., & Khazaal, Y. (2019). Systematic review of food addiction as measured with the Yale food addiction scale: Implications for the food addiction construct. Current Neuropharmacology, 17, 526538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pursey, K. M., Stanwell, P., Gearhardt, A. N., Collins, C. E., & Burrows, T. L. (2014). The prevalence of food addiction as assessed by the Yale food addiction scale: A systematic review. Nutrients, 6, 45524590.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sahin, N. H., & Durak, A. (1994). Kisa semptom Envanteri (brief symptom invetory-BSI): Turk Gencleri Icin Uyarlanmasi [A study of the brief symptom inventory in Turkish youth]. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 9, 4456.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmidt, U., Adan, R., Böhm, I., Campbell, I. C., Dingemans, A., Ehrlich, S., et al. (2016). Eating disorders: The big issue. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3, 313315.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2018). Associations of food addiction in a sample recruited to be nationally representative of the United States. European Eating Disorders Review, 26, 112119.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., Grilo, C. M., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2016). Shared and unique mechanisms underlying binge eating disorder and addictive disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 44, 125139.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., Potenza, M. N., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2017). A commentary on the “eating addiction” versus “food addiction” perspectives on addictive-like food consumption. Appetite, 115, 915.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Selçuk, E., Günaydın, G., Sümer, N., & Uysal, A. (2005). A new scale developed to measure adult attachment dimensions: Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) – psychometric evaluation in a Turkish sample. Türk Psikoloji Yazıları, 8, 111.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Şengör, G., & Gezer, C. (2019). Food addiction and its relationship with disordered eating behaviours and obesity. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 24, 10311039.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sevincer, G. M., Ince, E., Taymur, I., & Konuk, N. (2016). Night eating syndrome frequency in university students: Association with impulsivity, depression, and anxiety. Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 26, 238247.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stone, C. A., & Zhang, B. (2003). Assessing goodness of fit of item response theory models: A comparison of traditional and alternative procedures. Journal of Educational Measurement, 40, 331352.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sussman, S., Arpawong, T. E., Sun, P., Tsai, J., Rohrbach, L. A., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2014). Prevalence and co-occurrence of addictive behaviors among former alternative high school youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 3340.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Szabo, A., & Griffiths, M. D. (2004). The exercise addiction inventory: A new brief screening tool. Addiction Research and Theory, 12, 489499.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thompson, B. (2007). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis: Understanding concepts and applications. Applied Psychological Measurement, 31, 245248.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Torres, S., Camacho, M., Costa, P., Ribeiro, G., Santos, O., Vieira, F. M., et al. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the Yale food addiction scale. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 22, 259267.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ünal, E., Aydın, R., Gökler, M. E., & Ünsal, A. (2017). Eating disorders and anxiety among high school students in Western area of Turkey. International Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 4, 35133520.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ünübol, H., Koç, A. Ş., Sayar, G. H., Stavropoulos, V., Kircaburun, K., & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Measurement, profiles, prevalence, and psychological risk factors of problematic gaming among the Turkish community: A large-scale national study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00254-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vardar, E., & Erzengin, M. (2011). Prevalence rates and psychiatric comorbidities of eating disorders in adolescents. A two-stage community-based survey. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 22, 205212.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagner, D. D., Boswell, R. G., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2012). Inducing negative affect increases the reward value of appetizing foods in dieters. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 16251633.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Appendix

Please rate below items considering your food consumption behavior.

  1. This behavior has become the centre of my life, often comes to my mind, and I often engage in it. Even if I'm not doing it, I'm thinking about when I can. I get some relief when I plan to engage in the behavior. Sometimes there is an urge that is so powerful, I find a way to engage in the behavior, and I cannot prevent it. (Salience)

  2. Sometimes, when I'm bored, sometimes when I'm happy, I think of this behavior and I do it. Many times when my problems increase, my urge to engage in the behavior increases. If I'm unable to do it, my mind doesn't relax. It's like I'm alive. (Mood modification)

  3. Even if I engage in the behavior at the same rate, sometimes it's not enough for me, I have to engage in it more. I have to increase the time and amount I engage in the behavior or I won't relax. (Tolerance)

  4. When I'm in a place where it's impossible for me to engage in the behavior, or when I try to stop myself, or when someone sees my problem and tries to interfere, I feel symptoms of tension psychologically or physically. (Withdrawal)

  5. Engaging in the behavior causes me to have problems in my environment, my social life is badly affected, it can disrupt my work, and I often get criticized about it. (Conflict)

  6. If I do stop engaging in the behavior, it can be triggered again and I when it does it's like it never stopped in the first place. (Relapse)

1

Note 1: Participants scoring below a raw score of 58 were considered as not presenting with food addiction behaviors that exceed 2 SDs above the mean (which is deemed as diagnosable by the literature). However, they do present with food addiction behaviors below that level as visualized by Fig. 3. All the exact variations in the dimensional distribution of the latent behavior examined can be captured numerically. See table in Appendix at the end of this paper.

Summed Score

EAP[θ|x]

SD[θ|x]

Modeled Proportion

0−1.7110.5470.0684099
1−1.2940.4240.0225427
2−1.1990.4170.0284462
3−1.0910.4060.0293088
4−0.9740.3880.0261963
5−0.9230.3920.0326204
6−0.7760.3480.0259656
7−0.7030.3420.0264474
8−0.6260.3320.0258018
9−0.5460.3190.0243133
10−0.4880.3200.0247555
11−0.4080.3000.0230077
12−0.3480.2950.0226660
13−0.2880.2890.0221586
14−0.2290.2840.0214857
15−0.1750.2810.0212651
16−0.1190.2760.0206119
17−0.0680.2730.0202930
18−0.0170.2710.0199479
190.0330.2690.0195443
200.0820.2680.0192928
210.1300.2660.0188764
220.1760.2660.0185736
230.2230.2650.0182535
240.2680.2640.0178911
250.3140.2640.0175916
260.3590.2630.0171956
270.4030.2630.0168403
280.4480.2630.0164660
290.4920.2630.0160562
300.5360.2630.0156735
310.5800.2630.0152335
320.6240.2630.0148088
330.6680.2630.0143732
340.7120.2630.0139157
350.7560.2630.0134767
360.8010.2630.0130095
370.8460.2630.0125552
380.8910.2640.0121029
390.9360.2640.0116420
400.9820.2640.0112051
411.0280.2650.0107555
421.0750.2660.0103258
431.1230.2670.0099093
441.1720.2680.0094875
451.2220.2700.0091076
461.2720.2710.0087021
471.3240.2740.0083350
481.3780.2770.0079901
491.4310.2800.0076123
501.4950.2920.0073707
511.5450.2870.0069158
521.6090.2930.0066362
531.6760.3000.0063824
541.7420.3040.0059971
551.8380.3270.0060528
561.8940.3230.0052622
571.9850.3340.0050944
582.0900.3460.0049914
592.1890.3520.0041345
602.5210.4500.0081162

2

Note 2: Item response theory methods allow a more holistic and clinical evaluation of the items/questions asked. In particular, from a clinical perspective, the response that an individual may receive on a question is assumed to depend on the features (properties) of the question itself (i.e., content and phrasing) in conjunction with the characteristics of the participant (i.e., high or low intensity of the behavior assessed; Cai et al., 2011). In that line, IRT-differences considering specifc items' properties are assessed in a psychometric (and therefore unbiased) manner, and distinguishes questions in terms of their level of difficulty and discrimination power/accuracy. Unique item differences considering the participants are also reflected by the reliability variations across the different items, in such a way that an ndiciduals can safely select which questions to ask/(prioritize/rely more for) those with higher and those with lower levels of the behavior exhibited (in this case food addiction). Therefore, IRT (as a method) enables a more person-specific assessment planning, moving from assessment content questions not acknowledging the participant, “What to ask?” and “How much one can rely on the response received?” to “What question(s) should be best addressed by individuals with varying levels of the behavior?” or “How do the responses of individuals differ on the behavior's severity, and should they be weighted differently for different items?” (Cai et al., 2011; Gomez et al., 2019). Item Characteristic Curves and Item Information Function, based on psychometric evidence, maximize the assessment information retrieved by an item, taking concurrently into consideration the level that different participants exhibit/present the behavior assessed. This enables the detection of within scale (inter-item) differences, which are missed by other types of analyses. Given the phrasing of the items (i.e., multiple sentences as one item), the different sentences included in an item cannot be assessed independently here. Participants were instructed to address each item on a Likert scale (0 = never to 10 = always) to capture maximum variability.

  • Alpaslan, A. H., Soylu, N., Kadriye, A. V. C. I., Coşkun, K. Ş., Kocak, U., & Taş, H. U. (2015). Disordered eating attitudes, alexithymia and suicide probability among Turkish high school girls. Psychiatry Research, 226, 224229.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., Pallesen, S., Bilder, R. M., Torsheim, T., & Aboujaoude, E. (2015). The Bergen shopping addiction scale: Reliability and validity of a brief screening test. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1374.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Pallesen, S., Griffiths, M. D., Torsheim, T., & Sinha, R. (2018). The development and validation of the Bergen–Yale Sex Addiction Scale with a large national sample. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 144.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Andreassen, C. S., Torsheim, T., Brunborg, G. S., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a Facebook addiction scale. Psychological Reports, 110, 501517.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Atroszko, P. A., Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., & Pallesen, S. (2015). Study addiction—A new area of psychological study: Conceptualization, assessment, and preliminary empirical findings. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4, 7584.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Babayiğit, Z., Alçalar, N., & Bahadir, G. (2013). Investigation of psychopathology and cognitive aspects of binge eating disorder in obese women. Turkiye Klinikleri Journal of Medical Sciences, 33, 10771087.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., & Parker, J. D. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 3340.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baker, F. B. (2001). The basics of item response theory. ERIC Clearinghouse on Assessment and Evaluation. Retrieved 15 April, 2020, from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED458219.pdf.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Berenson, A. B., Laz, T. H., Pohlmeier, A. M., Rahman, M., & Cunningham, K. A. (2015). Prevalence of food addiction among low-income reproductive-aged women. Journal of Women's Health, 24, 740744.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brunault, P., Courtois, R., Gearhardt, A. N., Gaillard, P., Journiac, K., Cathelain, S., et al. (2017). Validation of the French version of the DSM-5 Yale food addiction scale in a nonclinical sample. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 62, 199210.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brunault, P., Ducluzeau, P. H., Courtois, R., Bourbao-Tournois, C., Delbachian, I., Réveillère, C., et al. (2018). Food addiction is associated with higher neuroticism, lower conscientiousness, higher impulsivity, but lower extraversion in obese patient candidates for bariatric surgery. Substance Use & Misuse, 53, 19191923.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Burrows, T., Skinner, J., McKenna, R., & Rollo, M. (2017). Food addiction, binge eating disorder, and obesity: Is there a relationship?. Behavioral Sciences, 7, e54.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cai, L., Du Toit, S. H. C., & Thissen, D. (2011). IRTPRO: Flexible, multidimensional, multiple categorical IRT modeling [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cardi, V., Leppanen, J., Leslie, M., Esposito, M., & Treasure, J. (2019). The use of a positive mood induction video-clip to target eating behaviour in people with bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder: An experimental study. Appetite, 133, 400404.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chao, A. M., Shaw, J. A., Pearl, R. L., Alamuddin, N., Hopkins, C. M., Bakizada, Z. M., et al. (2017). Prevalence and psychosocial correlates of food addiction in persons with obesity seeking weight reduction. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 73, 97104.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Davis, C. (2017). A commentary on the associations among ‘food addiction’, binge eating disorder, and obesity: Overlapping conditions with idiosyncratic clinical features. Appetite, 115, 38.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Davis, C., Loxton, N. J., Levitan, R. D., Kaplan, A. S., Carter, J. C., & Kennedy, J. L. (2013). ‘Food addiction’and its association with a dopaminergic multilocus genetic profile. Physiology & Behavior, 118, 6369.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • De Ayala, R. J. (2013). The theory and practice of item response theory. Guilford Publications.

  • DeMars, C. E. (2003). Sample size and the recovery of nominal response model item parameters. Applied Psychological Measurement, 27, 275288.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Derogatis, L. R., & Spencer, P. M. (1993). Brief symptom inventory: BSI. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

  • Doina & Ioana, A. (2015). The quality of attachment relationships and alexithymia. Journal of Education, 7, 130141.

  • Embretson, S. E., & Reise, S. P. (2013). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Evers, C., Dingemans, A., Junghans, A. F., & Boevé, A. (2018). Feeling bad or feeling good, does emotion affect your consumption of food? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 195208.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Faber, A., Dube, L., & Knaeuper, B. (2018). Attachment and eating: A meta-analytic review of the relevance of attachment for unhealthy and healthy eating behaviors in the general population. Appetite, 123, 410438.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farah, M. F., & Shahzad, M. F. (2020). Fast‐food addiction and anti‐consumption behaviour: The moderating role of consumer social responsibility. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 44, 379392.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fouladi, F., Mitchell, J. E., Crosby, R. D., Engel, S. G., Crow, S., Hill, L., et al. (2015). Prevalence of alcohol and other substance use in patients with eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 23, 531536.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 350364.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2009). Preliminary validation of the Yale food addiction scale. Appetite, 52, 430436.

  • Gearhardt, A. N., Corbin, W. R., & Brownell, K. D. (2016). Development of the Yale food addiction scale version 2.0. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30, 113121.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A. N., White, M. A., Masheb, R. M., Morgan, P. T., Crosby, R. D., & Grilo, C. M. (2012). An examination of the food addiction construct in obese patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 657663.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gearhardt, A., White, M. A., & Potenza, M. N. (2011). Binge eating disorder and food addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 4, 201207.

  • Gençöz, T. (2000). Positive and negative affect Schedule: A study of validity and reliability. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 15, 1926.

  • Gomez, R., Stavropoulos, V., Beard, C., & Pontes, H. M. (2019). Item response theory analysis of the recoded internet gaming disorder scale-short-form (IGDS9-SF). International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17, 859879.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gonçalves, S., Vieira, A. I., Rodrigues, T., Machado, P. P., Brandão, I., Timóteo, S., et al. (2019). Adult attachment in eating disorders mediates the association between perceived invalidating childhood environments and eating psychopathology. Current Psychology. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00524-2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Granero, R., Hilker, I., Agüera, Z., Jiménez‐Murcia, S., Sauchelli, S., Islam, M. A., et al. (2014). Food addiction in a Spanish sample of eating disorders: DSM‐5 diagnostic subtype differentiation and validation data. European Eating Disorders Review, 22, 389396.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Granero, R., Jiménez-Murcia, S., Gearhardt, A. N., Agüera, Z., Aymamí, N., Gómez-Peña, M., et al. (2018). Validation of the Spanish version of the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0 (YFAS 2.0) and clinical correlates in a sample of eating disorder, gambling disorder, and healthy control participants. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, e208.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, M. D. (2005). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10, 191197.

  • Güleç, H., Köse, S., Güleç, M. Y., Çitak, S., Evren, C., Borckardt, J., et al. (2009). Reliability and factorial validity of the Turkish version of the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20). Klinik Psikofarmakoloji Bülteni, 19, 214220.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hauck, C., Weiß, A., Schulte, E. M., Meule, A., & Ellrott, T. (2017). Prevalence of ‘food addiction' as measured with the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0 in a representative German sample and its association with sex, age and weight categories. Obesity Facts, 10, 1224.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hebebrand, J., Albayrak, Ö., Adan, R., Antel, J., Dieguez, C., de Jong, J., et al. (2014). “Eating addiction”, rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like eating behavior. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 295306.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hertz, P., Addad, M., & Ronel, N. (2012). Attachment styles and changes among women members of overeaters anonymous who have recovered from binge-eating disorder. Health & Social Work, 37, 110122.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives, Structural Equation Modelling, 6, 155.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Imperatori, C., Fabbricatore, M., Vumbaca, V., Innamorati, M., Contardi, A., & Farina, B. (2016). Food Addiction: Definition, measurement and prevalence in healthy subjects and in patients with eating disorders. Rivista di Psichiatria, 51, 6065.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Imperatori, C., Innamorati, M., Contardi, A., Continisio, M., Tamburello, S., Lamis, D. A., et al. (2014). The association among food addiction, binge eating severity and psychopathology in obese and overweight patients attending low-energy-diet therapy. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55, 13581362.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kakoschke, N., Aarts, E., & Verdejo-García, A. (2018). The cognitive drivers of compulsive eating behavior: A mini-review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, e338.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keski-Rahkonen, A., & Mustelin, L. (2016). Epidemiology of eating disorders in Europe: Prevalence, incidence, comorbidity, course, consequences, and risk factors. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29, 340345.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.

  • Lyvers, M., Edwards, M., & Thorberg, F. (2017). Alexithymia, attachment and fear of intimacy in young adults. IAFOR Journal of Psychology & the Behavioral Sciences, 3, 111.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Messer, S., Siegel, A., Bertin, L., & Erblich, J. (2018). Sex differences in effect-triggered lapses during smoking cessation: A daily diary study. Addictive Behaviors, 87, 8285.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2014). Five years of the Yale food addiction scale: Taking stock and moving forward. Current Addiction Reports, 1, 193205.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Lutz, A., Vögele, C., & Kübler, A. (2012). Food cravings discriminate differentially between successful and unsuccessful dieters and non-dieters. Validation of the Food Cravings Questionnaires in German. Appetite, 58, 8897.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meule, A., Müller, A., Gearhardt, A. N., & Blechert, J. (2017). German version of the Yale food addiction scale 2.0: Prevalence and correlates of ‘food addiction’ in students and obese individuals. Appetite, 115, 5461.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mies, G. W., Treur, J. L., Larsen, J. K., Halberstadt, J., Pasman, J. A., & Vink, J. M. (2017). The prevalence of food addiction in a large sample of adolescents and its association with addictive substances. Appetite, 118, 97105.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M., & Orbach, I. (1995). Attachment styles and repressive defensiveness: The accessibility and architecture of affective memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 917925.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Müller, J., Bühner, M., & Ellgring, H. (2003). Is there a reliable factorial structure in the 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale?: A comparison of factor models in clinical and normal adult samples. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 55, 561568.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Muraki, E., & Muraki, M. (2016). Generalized partial credit model. In Handbook of item response theory, Vol. 1 (pp. 155166). Chapman and Hall/CRC.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nunes-Neto, P. R., Köhler, C. A., Schuch, F. B., Solmi, M., Quevedo, J., Maes, M., et al. (2018). Food addiction: Prevalence, psychopathological correlates and associations with quality of life in a large sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 96, 145152.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orosz, G., Dombi, E., Andreassen, C. S., Griffiths, M. D., & Demetrovics, Z. (2016). Analyzing models of work addiction: Single factor and bi-factor models of the Bergen Work Addiction Scale. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14, 662671.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Özkan, İ., Devrim, A., & Bilgiç, P. (2017). Evaluation of the relationship between food addiction, nutritional status and depression in overweight and obese women. Journal of Nutrition and Dietetics, 45, 242249.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Penzenstadler, L., Soares, C., Karila, L., & Khazaal, Y. (2019). Systematic review of food addiction as measured with the Yale food addiction scale: Implications for the food addiction construct. Current Neuropharmacology, 17, 526538.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pursey, K. M., Stanwell, P., Gearhardt, A. N., Collins, C. E., & Burrows, T. L. (2014). The prevalence of food addiction as assessed by the Yale food addiction scale: A systematic review. Nutrients, 6, 45524590.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sahin, N. H., & Durak, A. (1994). Kisa semptom Envanteri (brief symptom invetory-BSI): Turk Gencleri Icin Uyarlanmasi [A study of the brief symptom inventory in Turkish youth]. Türk Psikoloji Dergisi, 9, 4456.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmidt, U., Adan, R., Böhm, I., Campbell, I. C., Dingemans, A., Ehrlich, S., et al. (2016). Eating disorders: The big issue. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3, 313315.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2018). Associations of food addiction in a sample recruited to be nationally representative of the United States. European Eating Disorders Review, 26, 112119.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., Grilo, C. M., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2016). Shared and unique mechanisms underlying binge eating disorder and addictive disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 44, 125139.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schulte, E. M., Potenza, M. N., & Gearhardt, A. N. (2017). A commentary on the “eating addiction” versus “food addiction” perspectives on addictive-like food consumption. Appetite, 115, 915.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Selçuk, E., Günaydın, G., Sümer, N., & Uysal, A. (2005). A new scale developed to measure adult attachment dimensions: Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) – psychometric evaluation in a Turkish sample. Türk Psikoloji Yazıları, 8, 111.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Şengör, G., & Gezer, C. (2019). Food addiction and its relationship with disordered eating behaviours and obesity. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 24, 10311039.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sevincer, G. M., Ince, E., Taymur, I., & Konuk, N. (2016). Night eating syndrome frequency in university students: Association with impulsivity, depression, and anxiety. Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 26, 238247.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stone, C. A., & Zhang, B. (2003). Assessing goodness of fit of item response theory models: A comparison of traditional and alternative procedures. Journal of Educational Measurement, 40, 331352.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sussman, S., Arpawong, T. E., Sun, P., Tsai, J., Rohrbach, L. A., & Spruijt-Metz, D. (2014). Prevalence and co-occurrence of addictive behaviors among former alternative high school youth. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 3, 3340.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Szabo, A., & Griffiths, M. D. (2004). The exercise addiction inventory: A new brief screening tool. Addiction Research and Theory, 12, 489499.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thompson, B. (2007). Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis: Understanding concepts and applications. Applied Psychological Measurement, 31, 245248.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Torres, S., Camacho, M., Costa, P., Ribeiro, G., Santos, O., Vieira, F. M., et al. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the Yale food addiction scale. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 22, 259267.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ünal, E., Aydın, R., Gökler, M. E., & Ünsal, A. (2017). Eating disorders and anxiety among high school students in Western area of Turkey. International Journal of Research in Medical Sciences, 4, 35133520.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ünübol, H., Koç, A. Ş., Sayar, G. H., Stavropoulos, V., Kircaburun, K., & Griffiths, M. D. (2020). Measurement, profiles, prevalence, and psychological risk factors of problematic gaming among the Turkish community: A large-scale national study. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction. Epub ahead of print. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-020-00254-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vardar, E., & Erzengin, M. (2011). Prevalence rates and psychiatric comorbidities of eating disorders in adolescents. A two-stage community-based survey. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 22, 205212.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wagner, D. D., Boswell, R. G., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2012). Inducing negative affect increases the reward value of appetizing foods in dieters. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 16251633.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 10631070.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand
The author instruction is available in PDF.
Please, download the file from HERE

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2021  
Web of Science  
Total Cites
WoS
5223
Journal Impact Factor 7,772
Rank by Impact Factor Psychiatry SCIE 26/155
Psychiatry SSCI 19/142
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
7,130
5 Year
Impact Factor
9,026
Journal Citation Indicator 1,39
Rank by Journal Citation Indicator

Psychiatry 34/257

Scimago  
Scimago
H-index
56
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,951
Scimago Quartile Score Clinical Psychology (Q1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health (Q1)
Scopus  
Scopus
Cite Score
11,5
Scopus
CIte Score Rank
Clinical Psychology 5/292 (D1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 20/529 (D1)
Medicine (miscellaneous) 17/276 (D1)
Scopus
SNIP
2,184

2020  
Total Cites 4024
WoS
Journal
Impact Factor
6,756
Rank by Psychiatry (SSCI) 12/143 (Q1)
Impact Factor Psychiatry 19/156 (Q1)
Impact Factor 6,052
without
Journal Self Cites
5 Year 8,735
Impact Factor
Journal  1,48
Citation Indicator  
Rank by Journal  Psychiatry 24/250 (Q1)
Citation Indicator   
Citable 86
Items
Total 74
Articles
Total 12
Reviews
Scimago 47
H-index
Scimago 2,265
Journal Rank
Scimago Clinical Psychology Q1
Quartile Score Psychiatry and Mental Health Q1
  Medicine (miscellaneous) Q1
Scopus 3593/367=9,8
Scite Score  
Scopus Clinical Psychology 7/283 (Q1)
Scite Score Rank Psychiatry and Mental Health 22/502 (Q1)
Scopus 2,026
SNIP  
Days from  38
submission  
to 1st decision  
Days from  37
acceptance  
to publication  
Acceptance 31%
Rate  

2019  
Total Cites
WoS
2 184
Impact Factor 5,143
Impact Factor
without
Journal Self Cites
4,346
5 Year
Impact Factor
5,758
Immediacy
Index
0,587
Citable
Items
75
Total
Articles
67
Total
Reviews
8
Cited
Half-Life
3,3
Citing
Half-Life
6,8
Eigenfactor
Score
0,00597
Article Influence
Score
1,447
% Articles
in
Citable Items
89,33
Normalized
Eigenfactor
0,7294
Average
IF
Percentile
87,923
Scimago
H-index
37
Scimago
Journal Rank
1,767
Scopus
Scite Score
2540/376=6,8
Scopus
Scite Score Rank
Cllinical Psychology 16/275 (Q1)
Medicine (miscellenous) 31/219 (Q1)
Psychiatry and Mental Health 47/506 (Q1)
Scopus
SNIP
1,441
Acceptance
Rate
32%

 

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 850 EUR/article
Printed Color Illustrations 40 EUR (or 10 000 HUF) + VAT / piece
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Editorial Board / Advisory Board members: 50%
Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Canada)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Max W. ABBOTT (Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand)
  • Elias N. ABOUJAOUDE (Stanford University School of Medicine, USA)
  • Hojjat ADELI (Ohio State University, USA)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (University of Dundee, United Kingdom)
  • Alex BLASZCZYNSKI (University of Sidney, Australia)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Kenneth BLUM (University of Florida, USA)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Sam-Wook CHOI (Eulji University, Republic of Korea)
  • Damiaan DENYS (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Heather HAUSENBLAS (Jacksonville University, USA)
  • Tobias HAYER (University of Bremen, Germany)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Jaeseung JEONG (Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Emmanuel KUNTSCHE (La Trobe University, Australia)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Michel LEJOXEUX (Paris University, France)
  • Anikó MARÁZ (Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Astrid MÜLLER  (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Frederick GERARD MOELLER (University of Texas, USA)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Nancy PETRY (University of Connecticut, USA)
  • Bettina PIKÓ (University of Szeged, Hungary)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Rory C. REID (University of California Los Angeles, USA)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ferenc TÚRY (Semmelweis University, Hungary)
  • Alfred UHL (Austrian Federal Health Institute, Austria)
  • Róbert URBÁN  (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Johan VANDERLINDEN (University Psychiatric Center K.U.Leuven, Belgium)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN  (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Kimberly YOUNG (Center for Internet Addiction, USA)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2022 0 69 66
Nov 2022 0 51 43
Dec 2022 0 39 38
Jan 2023 0 35 22
Feb 2023 0 53 37
Mar 2023 0 30 26
Apr 2023 0 0 0