Authors:
Jianhua Zhou School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, People's Republic of China

Search for other papers by Jianhua Zhou in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2104-8279
,
Meiyu Bai School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, People's Republic of China

Search for other papers by Meiyu Bai in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0009-0009-3701-8881
,
Tubei Li School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou, People's Republic of China

Search for other papers by Tubei Li in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0009-0001-0506-2767
,
Tiantian Bi Department of Preschool Education, College of Home Economics, Hebei Normal University, Shijiazhuang, China

Search for other papers by Tiantian Bi in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6272-4202
, and
Xue Gong Department of Psychology, Normal College, Qingdao University, Qingdao, China

Search for other papers by Xue Gong in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-2622-7196
Open access

Abstract

Background and aims

Adolescent problematic gaming is a global public health issue, and is associated with numerous negative outcomes. The Big Two personality traits, neuroticism and extraversion, have been identified as significant predictors of problematic gaming in adolescents. However, most previous studies have been cross-sectional, limiting the ability to explore their mutual influences or causality inference. This study addresses this gap by employing a longitudinal design and utilizing the Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model (RI-CLPM) to examine the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming at the within-person level.

Methods

This study included 3,307 students (Mean age = 11.30, SD = 0.48, 43.6% being girls). Participants were assessed annually, completing a total of four assessments over the course of the study.

Results

The RI-CLPM analyses revealed that neuroticism and problematic gaming significantly predict each other. Extraversion acts as a protective factor against adolescent problematic gaming, whereas problematic gaming leads to a decrease in extraversion levels. Additionally, the longitudinal relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming exhibit significant sex differences.

Discussion and conclusions

This study provides insights into the interplay between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming in adolescents. These findings emphasize the need for prevention and intervention strategies that address personality traits as risk factors while recognizing how problematic gaming can influence personality, promoting a more holistic approach. The observed sex differences highlight the importance of integrating sex-specific considerations in interventions.

Abstract

Background and aims

Adolescent problematic gaming is a global public health issue, and is associated with numerous negative outcomes. The Big Two personality traits, neuroticism and extraversion, have been identified as significant predictors of problematic gaming in adolescents. However, most previous studies have been cross-sectional, limiting the ability to explore their mutual influences or causality inference. This study addresses this gap by employing a longitudinal design and utilizing the Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model (RI-CLPM) to examine the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming at the within-person level.

Methods

This study included 3,307 students (Mean age = 11.30, SD = 0.48, 43.6% being girls). Participants were assessed annually, completing a total of four assessments over the course of the study.

Results

The RI-CLPM analyses revealed that neuroticism and problematic gaming significantly predict each other. Extraversion acts as a protective factor against adolescent problematic gaming, whereas problematic gaming leads to a decrease in extraversion levels. Additionally, the longitudinal relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming exhibit significant sex differences.

Discussion and conclusions

This study provides insights into the interplay between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming in adolescents. These findings emphasize the need for prevention and intervention strategies that address personality traits as risk factors while recognizing how problematic gaming can influence personality, promoting a more holistic approach. The observed sex differences highlight the importance of integrating sex-specific considerations in interventions.

Introduction

The increasingly pervasive presence of online gaming in daily life is reshaping the landscape of adolescent socialization (Thulin, Vilhelmson, & Schwanen, 2020). As online gaming becomes increasingly embedded in the social and recreational activities of young people, facilitated by technological advancements and the expansion of online platforms, its influence extends beyond mere entertainment, raising significant concerns among educators, parents, and mental health professionals regarding its impacts on psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Chan et al., 2022; Purwaningsih & Nurmala, 2021; Rosendo-Rios, Trott, & Shukla, 2022). These concerns center on problematic gaming (e.g., Männikkö, Ruotsalainen, Miettunen, Pontes, & Kääriäinen, 2020). Research has established that personality traits, particularly neuroticism and extraversion, are significant predictors of problematic gaming; however, the potential for problematic gaming to influence the development of these personality traits in adolescents remains largely overlooked (e.g., Chew, 2022; Gervasi et al., 2017; Şalvarlı & Griffiths, 2021). Thus, comprehensive investigations are needed to understand the dynamic interplay between neuroticism, extraversion and problematic gaming in adolescents.

Problematic gaming is characterized by excessive and compulsive gaming activities, which have been empirically observed to correlate with a variety of psychological difficulties including anxiety (e.g., Wang et al., 2017), depression (e.g., Männikkö et al., 2020), and social withdrawal (e.g., Giardina et al., 2024), attention disorders (e.g., Gao et al., 2021), sleep disturbances (Kristensen, Pallesen, King, Hysing, & Erevik, 2021) and even self-harm (e.g., Pan & Yeh, 2018) and suicidal behaviors (e.g., Erevik et al., 2022). Adding to the severity of these concerns, the most extreme cases of problematic gaming have led to the formal recognition of Internet gaming disorder (IGD) (e.g., Petry, Rehbein, Ko, & O’Brien, 2015). Defined as the uncontrollable, excessive, and compulsive use of online games that significantly impairs social and personal functioning, IGD has been included as a tentative disorder requiring additional research in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5-TR; APA., 2022). The International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11) also officially classified gaming disorder as an addictive behavior disorder (WHO, 2019). Our study uses the broader term “problematic gaming” because it encompasses a wider range of behaviors that may negatively impact an individual's psychological and social functioning, even if they do not meet the strict diagnostic criteria for IGD in DSM-5 or ICD-11 (e.g., Ang, Chong, Chye, & Huan, 2012). This approach allows us to capture less severe cases that might still benefit from early intervention, providing a more inclusive understanding of gaming-related issues. Moreover, using “problematic gaming” offers flexibility, as it is not confined to specific diagnostic standards, making it more adaptable across different cultural contexts and individual differences (Fernandes, Maia, & Pontes, 2019).

Recent systematic reviews (Long et al., 2018; Nielsen, Favez, & Rigter, 2020) and meta-analyses (Liao, Chen, Huang, & Shen, 2022) indicate that the prevalence rates of problematic gaming among adolescents range from 3.5% to 30.4%. While these figures differ across studies, they consistently reflect a significant concern that resonates globally (Feng, Ramo, Chan, & Bourgeois, 2017; Kim, Song et al., 2022). Given the serious clinical consequences associated with problematic gaming in youth, it is essential to understand its etiological risk factors to develop effective interventions and preventive strategies. Personality traits, particularly the Big Two dimensions of neuroticism and extraversion, have been well-identified as key determinants of adolescent problematic gaming (see Gervasi et al., 2017). However, the majority of previous research has been cross-sectional in nature (see Chew, 2022; Şalvarlı & Griffiths, 2021), thereby limiting the ability to draw causal conclusions about the relation between personality traits and adolescent problematic gaming. Additionally, few studies have accounted for the directionality and potential reciprocal dynamics between personality traits and problematic gaming, making it challenging to ascertain how Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming influence each other over time.

Early adolescence represents a dynamic period in which personality traits undergo significant development (e.g., Borghuis et al., 2017). Excessive involvement in online gaming during this critical phase may impact the developmental trajectory of personality in significant ways. For instance, the compulsive nature of problematic gaming may enhance traits like neuroticism by escalating stress and anxiety, while simultaneously stifling the growth of extraversion by limiting opportunities for real-life social interactions and engagement. Investigating the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming not only deepens our understanding of the co-development of adolescent personality and gaming behaviors but also aids in designing interventions that both foster sound personality development and reduce problematic gaming among adolescents. Thus, our research aims to examine the reciprocal relation between Big Two personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and extraversion) and problematic gaming by employing a longitudinal design in a sample of Chinese adolescents.

Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming

The Big Two personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and extraversion) are considered the most fundamental and widely relevant factors in describing personality (Eysenck, 1967). Neuroticism measures an individual's emotional stability, with those high in neuroticism experiencing greater emotional distress, manifesting as heightened feelings of anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, and depressed mood, and more intense emotional reactions to everyday stresses of life (Eysenck, 1990). Extraversion encompasses traits such as sociability, activity, and assertiveness, venturesome and dominant (Eysenck, 1990). Extraverts often display high levels of energy and enthusiasm in social interactions, generally maintaining a positive and optimistic outlook (Eysenck, 1990). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (Brand et al., 2019) model highlights that personality traits, as a key predisposing factor, can influence problematic gaming by impacting internal mechanisms such as emotional and cognitive responses, executive control, and the rewarding experiences associated with gaming. Personality traits may shape how individuals perceive and react to gaming stimuli, manage their impulses, and experience gratification, ultimately affecting their engagement and potential development of problematic gaming behaviors (e.g., King & Delfabbro, 2018). A comprehensive review (see Şalvarlı & Griffiths, 2021) incorporating 21 empirical studies has found that most of the research consistently shows that neuroticism is significantly positively correlated with problematic gaming, and extraversion is significantly negatively correlated, whereas some studies found no significant relations between problematic gaming and the Big Two personality traits. Additionally, a recent meta-analysis (Akbari et al., 2021) including 25,634 young individuals revealed a modest but significant positive correlation between neuroticism and problematic gaming (r = 0.21, 95% CI = 0.16–0.25), and a slight yet significant negative correlation between extraversion and problematic gaming (r = −0.12, 95% CI = −0.16 to −0.09). Another meta-analysis found similar results, reinforcing the relations between these personality traits and problematic gaming (Chew, 2022).

Despite previous research examining the relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming, the majority of studies have employed cross-sectional study designs (see Akbari et al., 2021; Chew, 2022; Şalvarlı & Griffiths, 2021), which carry important limitations. Primarily, cross-sectional studies can only provide snapshots of data at a single point in time, thereby constraining the ability to determine causality or the direction of the relations between variables (Savitz & Wellenius, 2023). Adolescence is a period marked by significant personality development, as individuals begin to form their own identities through exploring different roles, beliefs, and values, which contribute to their sense of self and emerging personality (e.g., Syed & Seiffge-Krenke, 2013). Furthermore, as adolescents are in the midst of shaping their identities and refining their coping mechanisms, their heightened vulnerability to external influences can make them particularly susceptible to the intense emotional experiences and virtual social interactions provided by gaming (e.g., Kim, Nam, & Keum, 2022; Macur & Pontes, 2021). Overexposure to these influences can potentially solidify specific emotional responses and behaviors (e.g., Swingle, 2016), thereby significantly impacting their ongoing personality development. Thus, problematic gaming may also significantly influence adolescent personality traits.

Theoretically, online games often feature immediate rewards and punishments, creating an environment in which players face continuous challenges, competitive pressures, and feelings of frustration, significantly increasing stress levels (e.g., Ojha et al., 2023; Przybylski, Deci, Rigby, & Ryan, 2014). Prolonged exposure to this stress exacerbates inherent anxieties, leading to heightened emotional reactions and creating a persistent feedback loop that increases sensitivity to negative feedback (e.g., Lee et al., 2015; Nesi et al., 2022; Raiha et al., 2020; Weinstein, Livny, & Weizman, 2017). Consequently, this heightened sensitivity fuels feelings of frustration and emotional instability. On the other hand, while extraversion typically promotes real-world social interactions, excessive gaming can replace these with online interactions, potentially reducing real-life social engagement (e.g., Tullett-Prado, Stavropoulos, Mueller, Sharples, & Footitt, 2021). Consequently, constant gaming can limit opportunities to develop social skills in real-world scenarios, such as navigating group dynamics or interpreting nonverbal cues (e.g., Hygen et al., 2020). Over time, this preference for gaming interactions can diminish the desire and ability to engage in social activities outside of gaming, further eroding extraverted traits and overall social confidence and competence. Overall, problematic gaming may significantly impact the formation and development of neuroticism and extraversion in adolescents. Therefore, there may be a bidirectional relation between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming. However, previous cross-sectional studies inherently limit the understanding of how personality traits and problematic gaming may influence each other dynamically over time. More longitudinal research is needed to investigate these reciprocal interactions, which would allow for a clearer delineation of causality and the evolution of Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming over time.

Sex differences

When exploring the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits—neuroticism and extraversion—and problematic gaming, it's important to consider potential sex differences. These relations may vary significantly between males and females, influenced by varying behavioral patterns and social expectations prevalent among different sexes. Studies suggest that females typically score higher on measures of neuroticism and extraversion than males (e.g., Lippa, 2010; Slobodskaya & Kornienko, 2021; Weisberg, DeYoung, & Hirsh, 2011). Previous research has also found that levels of problematic gaming are significantly higher among males compared to females (e.g., Fam, 2018; Stevens, Dorstyn, Delfabbro, & King, 2021).

From the perspective of gender socialization theory (Fagot, Rodgers, & Leinbach, 2000), societal expectations and cultural norms significantly shape the behavioral development of males and females, influencing how they interact with environments such as gaming. Typically, males are socialized to embody traits such as competitiveness, assertiveness, and stoicism—qualities that are often valorized in many cultures (e.g., Schrock & Schwalbe, 2009). For females, societal expectations often emphasize traits such as being quiet and submissive, which are traditionally associated with femininity (e.g., Mahalik et al., 2005). If a male exhibits high neuroticism, which includes traits such as emotional sensitivity, anxiety, and insecurity, this can clash with the traditional masculine image of emotional control and resilience (Mahalik, Burns, & Syzdek, 2007). Similarly, low extraversion, characterized by introversion and shyness, may also deviate from the expected assertive and sociable male role (Addis & Mahalik, 2003). These discrepancies between actual personality traits and socially expected ones can lead to psychological stress and internal conflict (Löckenhoff et al., 2014). For some males, problematic gaming may become an effective coping mechanism—a way to escape from the discomfort of not meeting these traditional expectations or to find a domain where they can feel competent and in control (Su, Han, Yu, Wu, & Potenza, 2020). Therefore, it seems reasonable to expect that the effect of the Big Two personality traits on problematic gaming may be more pronounced among males than females.

Given the differences in how males and females respond to the dynamics of problematic gaming, particularly in relation to reward sensitivity and craving, problematic gaming can differentially influence the development or exacerbation of the Big Two personality traits across sexes. The higher sensitivity to gaming-related rewards observed in males can enhance the pleasure and excitement derived from gaming successes, which can lead to more frequent and intense gaming sessions as males seek out these rewarding experiences (e.g., Dong & Potenza, 2022). This pattern of behavior may gradually impact their personality traits, potentially increasing neuroticism due to the stress and frustration associated with the craving and pursuit of these gaming rewards (e.g., Snodgrass et al., 2014). Moreover, as males may become more engaged in online gaming, their extraversion could diminish over time due to reduced real-world interactions and the erosion of traditional social activities (e.g., Uz & Cagiltay, 2015). Females may not experience the same level of acute cravings or reward sensitivity from gaming. Instead, their engagement in problematic gaming might be driven more by social interactions or the narrative aspects of the games (e.g., Lopez-Fernandez, Williams, Griffiths, & Kuss, 2019; Su et al., 2020). The cooperative and story-focused gaming preferred by many females may contribute to a more balanced emotional state (Phan, Jardina, Hoyle, & Chaparro, 2012), potentially reducing the likelihood of developing high levels of neuroticism. Engaging in rich, interactive game narratives can also help keep female gamers connected to a social network (Su et al., 2020), which might prevent a decrease in extraversion through regular socialization, even in a virtual setting. Consequently, the modality of engagement in gaming is pivotal for differentially influencing the Big Two personality traits across sexes. Problematic gaming may predispose males to heightened levels of neuroticism and diminished extraversion, but females are likely to experience a lesser degree of impact on these traits.

The current study

The present research aims to elucidate the complex and reciprocal relations between the Big Two personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and extraversion) and problematic gaming among Chinese adolescents through a longitudinal study design. This approach seeks to overcome the limitations of previous cross-sectional studies, which have largely been unable to determine the causal directions and the dynamic interplay between personality traits and gaming behaviors over time. Given the rapid integration of online gaming into the daily social and recreational activities of youth, understanding the interplay between problematic gaming and personality traits is crucial for developing interventions specifically tailored to mitigate the risks associated with gaming behaviors and support healthy personality development. Specifically, this study formulates two hypotheses. First, based on existing research that identifies significant positive correlations between neuroticism and problematic gaming, and negative correlations between extraversion and problematic gaming, it is hypothesized that neuroticism will significantly positively predict problematic gaming behaviors and vice versa. Meanwhile, we expect extraversion to negatively predict problematic gaming and vice versa. Second, considering the theoretical foundations of gender role theory and the differences in gaming engagement modalities, this study hypothesized that the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming will be more pronounced in males than in females.

In order to rigorously analyze longitudinal data and capture dynamic interactions between personality traits and problematic gaming over time, our study employs an advanced statistical technique known as the Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Model (RI-CLPM, e.g., Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman, 2015). This model represents a significant advancement over traditional cross-lagged panel models (CLPM), which do not typically distinguish between within-individual effects and between-individual differences (e.g., Tseng, 2024). Traditional CLPM often conflates these two sources of variation, potentially leading to biased estimates in interpreting the causal relations between variables over time (e.g., Tseng, 2024). The RI-CLPM addresses this limitation by incorporating random intercepts, which account for stable, between-individual differences that may influence the observed measures across time points (e.g., Hamaker et al., 2015). This feature allows for a more precise assessment of within-individual dynamics by isolating the fluctuations specific to an individual from those consistent across the population (e.g., Tseng, 2024). By employing the RI-CLPM, our study can more accurately parse out the within-person effects of the reciprocal relations between Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming over time, offering clearer insights into the nature of these longitudinal relations.

Methods

Participants

Participants in our study were drawn from three schools adhering to the nine-year compulsory education system located in a northwestern city. With assistance from local educational authorities, these schools were invited to participate in the study, and all three agreed to take part. According to information from local education authorities, these schools showed no significant differences in major characteristics (e.g., enrollment rates, student-teacher ratios, average academic performance, or resource availability such as facilities and extracurricular programs) compared to other schools in China, and were representative of such institutions in China. At the first time point (T1), a total of 3,307 students aged 11–13 years (Mean age = 11.30, SD = 0.48) participated in the study, with 43.6% of them being girls. The research received approval from the Northwest Normal University's ethical review board, relevant local education authorities, school principals, and teachers. Prior to data collection, written informed consent was obtained from the guardians of all participating students. Participants were assessed annually, completing a total of four assessments throughout the study. Most participants in our sample were from middle-income families compared to the overall income levels in their province, and over eighty-three percent of the participants' parents had achieved at least a high school education.

The sample sizes for the subsequent assessments were 3,126 students at the second assessment (T2), 3,105 students at the third assessment (T3), and 2,984 students at the fourth assessment (T4), corresponding to retention rates of 94.5%, 93.9%, and 90.2%, respectively. To address potential issues related to missing data, a Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test was conducted. The normalized chi-square (χ2/df) value from this MCAR test was 1.22 (p < 0.05), suggesting a potential deviation from randomness in missingness patterns (Nicholson, Deboeck, & Howard, 2017). For the statistical analysis of the model, the Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) approach was employed. FIML is advantageous in that it utilizes all available information to estimate parameters, thus maximizing the use of the data. This approach is particularly effective under the assumption of missing at random data (Little & Rubin, 2002), allowing for more accurate and robust estimations even with some missing data points.

Procedure

The data collection procedure was meticulously organized to ensure uniformity and confidentiality. All four waves of data collection were conducted in regular classrooms to maintain a familiar environment and minimize disruptions. Each classroom was assigned two trained graduate research assistants who provided both verbal and written instructions on how to complete the assessments. The students were allowed to take as much time as needed to ensure they understood the questions and could answer them thoughtfully. The demographic survey, including questions about socioeconomic status (SES), was completed online by parents under the guidance of the class teacher. To maintain confidentiality, participants were reassured that all responses would be securely stored and only accessible to the research team. As a token of appreciation, each student received a small gift valued at approximately one US dollar.

Measures

All questionnaires were administered in Chinese to ensure participants' comprehension. The scales used in this study were originally developed in English and had been translated into Chinese in prior research. These translated versions have been validated, demonstrating strong reliability and validity among Chinese adolescents, as detailed in the following sections.

Big Two personality traits

Personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism were assessed using the Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire Revised (JEPQR-A; Francis, 1996). The JEPQR-A has demonstrated robust reliability and validity in Chinese youth, as corroborated by prior research (Tian, Jiang, & Huebner, 2019). The extraversion subscale consists of six items, such as “Do you like going out a lot?” Similarly, the neuroticism subscale includes six items, one example being “Do you find it hard to get to sleep at night because you are worrying about things?” Responses were collected in a binary format, with “No” scored as 0 and “Yes” scored as 1. The mean score for each subscale was calculated by summing the responses, with higher scores indicating greater levels of extraversion and neuroticism, respectively. Across the four measurement points (T1 to T4), the Cronbach's alpha for the extraversion subscale ranged from 0.64 to 0.74, and for the neuroticism subscale, it ranged from 0.74 to 0.80.

Problematic gaming

Symptoms of problematic gaming were assessed using the Pathological Online Gaming Use Scale (POGU; Gentile, 2009). The POGU was developed based on the DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling, following similar diagnostic approaches used for other disorders by considering gaming to be pathological if the gamer exhibited at least half (6) of the symptoms (Gentile, 2009). These symptoms align with DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling and share core characteristics with other definitions of addiction, such as Brown's core facets: salience, euphoria or relief, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflict, and relapse (Brown, 1991; Gentile, 2009). Earlier research has confirmed the psychometric properties of the POGU among Chinese adolescent populations (e.g., Zhao, Li, Zhou, Nie, & Zhou, 2020). The POGU is composed of 11 items designed to capture various dimensions of gaming behavior that may indicate problematic or addictive tendencies. Example items include: “Have you tried to play video games less often or for shorter periods of time, but are unsuccessful?” and “Do you need to spend more and more time and/or money on video games in order to feel the same amount of excitement”. Participants responded to each item on a three-point scale (“No,” “Sometimes,” “Yes”), scored as 0, 1, and 2, respectively. A higher average score across the items indicates more severe problematic gaming behaviors. In our study, Cronbach's alpha for the POGU ranged from 0.91 to 0.94 across the four measurement points (T1 to T4).

Covariates

At the initial data collection point (T1), detailed demographic information, including age, sex, and socioeconomic status (SES), was gathered from student participants and their parents. Students self-reported their age and sex (0 = male, 1 = female). Family socioeconomic status was assessed by having parents report their education levels and household monthly income through an online questionnaire. Parental education was measured on a scale ranging from 0 (no formal education) to 7 (doctoral degree). Household income levels were categorized on a scale from 1 (less than 1,000 RMB, ∼143 USD) to 9 (over 80,000 RMB, ∼11,435 USD). These scores were averaged to create an overall SES index for each family.

Data analysis

Preliminary analysis

All analyses were conducted using Mplus 8.3. Descriptive statistics, such as means and standard deviations, were computed for all variables. Subsequently, correlational analyses were performed to examine the bivariate correlations among study variables.

RI-CLPM analysis

The RI-CLPM was constructed using the mean scores of all observed variables. The RI-CLPM aims to identify trait factors, with random intercepts accounting for consistent between-person differences (Hamaker et al., 2015). This model facilitates the analysis of how deviations from an individual's average trait level in one construct affect deviations in another construct at the within-person level. The model estimates autoregressive and cross-lagged effects using residual scores obtained after extracting random intercepts (traits) rather than directly from observed scores. Following Mulder and Hamaker's (2021) approach, random intercepts were modeled by regressing the observed scores onto their latent factors, with factor loadings fixed at one. This process isolates the between-person component (random intercept), and the residual variance of the observed scores represents within-person fluctuations, which are then used to estimate lagged effects between variables. At T1, the covariances among the within-person components are freely estimated, and at subsequent time points (T2, T3, and T4), all covariances among the within-person residuals are also freely estimated (Orth, Clark, Donnellan, & Robins, 2021). The RI-CLPM is tested using the Maximum Likelihood Robust (MLR) estimator to adjust for any non-normality. Controls for sex, age, and SES were applied to all observed variables at T1 and on the random intercepts. Model fit was evaluated with the chi-square statistic, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). A CFI and TLI greater than 0.90, an RMSEA less than 0.08, and an SRMR less than 0.08 indicate a good model fit (Marsh, Hau, & Wen, 2004).

To further identify the most parsimonious model, we engaged in model comparison by systematically constraining the autoregressive and cross-lagged effects to be equal across time points. This step involves a series of nested model comparisons to test whether the simplification of parameters affects the model fit. The analysis began with a fully unconstrained model where all parameters (autoregressive and cross-lagged effects) were estimated freely. Next, we progressively constrained the autoregressive and cross-lagged effects to be equal over time. The S-Bχ2 test was applied to compare the fit of two models, an effective method for comparing model fit across different models under conditions of non-normality (Satorra & Bentler, 2010).

Sex differences

To assess whether the effects in the final RI-CLPM varied by sex, we utilized a multiple-group model comparison approach. We applied the S-Bχ2 test to compare the fit of two models. In the first model, all lagged paths in the final RI-CLPM were freely estimated for each sex. In the second model, all lagged paths were constrained to be equal across sexes.

Ethics

Study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the School of Psychology Research Ethics Committee at Northwest Normal University. The cognizant education authorities, school boards, and teachers at each of the participating schools also approved the implementation of this study. Additionally, all parents signed a written informed consent form allowing their child's participation, and students provided their assent before data collection. All procedures involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for all study variables. Correlational analyses indicated that neuroticism was significantly positively correlated with problematic gaming over time, whereas extraversion was significantly negatively correlated with problematic gaming over time. According to Gentile's (2009) guidelines, individuals were classified as exhibiting pathological online game use if they met at least 6 of the 11 criteria on the symptom checklist. Consistent with this approach, responses were assigned scores of “yes” = 1, “sometimes” = 0.5, and “no” = 0. The cumulative scores of the problematic gaming scale were then calculated for each time point (T1-T4). Based on these criteria, the number of participants identified as exhibiting problematic gaming behavior at T1, T2, T3, and T4 was 170, 157, 160, and 165, respectively.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and correlations of study variables

VariablesMSD1234567891011
1. T1_problematic gaming0.280.15
2. T2_problematic gaming0.270.150.52
3. T3_problematic gaming0.250.160.430.58
4. T4_problematic gaming0.250.170.370.490.60
5. T1_neuroticism0.300.090.420.330.270.22
6. T2_neuroticism0.280.090.370.470.390.330.39
7. T3_neuroticism0.290.090.300.400.500.400.380.44
8. T4_neuroticism0.300.100.250.310.350.460.290.390.44
9. T1_extraversion0.790.05−0.22−0.14−0.13−0.08−0.22−0.21−0.16−0.11
10. T2_extraversion0.800.05−0.22−0.26−0.21−0.14−0.20−0.19−0.23−0.160.40
11. T3_extraversion0.790.05−0.20−0.23−0.30−0.24−0.24−0.28−0.32−0.220.370.45
12. T4_extraversion0.790.06−0.20−0.22−0.25−0.30−0.20−0.20−0.26−0.300.340.400.48

All correlation coefficients were significant at p < 0.001.

RI-CLPM of neuroticism and problematic gaming

The RI-CLPM for neuroticism and problematic gaming demonstrated excellent model fit. Table 2 presents the fit indices for both constrained and unconstrained RI-CLPMs, as well as the results of the S-Bχ2 difference tests. When comparing models, we found no significant difference in fit between the model with constrained cross-lagged paths and the fully unconstrained model. However, models with constraints on autoregressive paths showed significantly worse fit compared to the fully unconstrained model.

Table 2.

Fit statistics and model comparisons of RI-CLPMs for neuroticism and problematic gaming

ModelsFreely estimated pathsFixed time-invariant pathsS-Bχ2dfScaling Correction Factor for MLRRMSEASRMRCFITLIS-BΔχ2ΔdfComparison modelpSelected Model
M1aAll autoregressive and cross-lagged pathsNone71.65211.16670.0270.0170.9910.979
M1bAutoregressive paths of problematic gaming;Autoregressive paths of neuroticism84.74231.17650.0280.0190.9890.97612.592M1b vs. M1a0.002M1a
Lagged paths from neuroticism to problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from problematic gaming to neuroticism
M1cAutoregressive paths of neuroticism;Autoregressive paths of problematic gaming83.05231.23870.0280.0190.9900.9779.672M1c vs. M1a0.008M1a
Lagged paths from neuroticism to problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from problematic gaming to neuroticism
M1dAutoregressive paths of neuroticism;Lagged paths from neuroticism to problematic gaming74.99231.17880.0260.0180.9910.9803.682M1d vs. M1a0.159M1d
Autoregressive paths of problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from problematic gaming to neuroticism
M1eAutoregressive paths of neuroticism;Lagged paths from neuroticism to problematic gaming;80.20251.18230.0260.0200.9900.9806.222M1e vs. M1d0.072M1e
Autoregressive paths of problematic gamingLagged paths from problematic gaming to neuroticism

Note. Bold indicates final selected model.

Thus, the final model selected was the one with constrained cross-lagged paths due to its parsimony and good fit. Figure 1 presents the standardized path coefficients for the final RI-CLPM. At the between-person level, the random intercepts for neuroticism and problematic gaming were significantly correlated (r = 0.70, p < 0.001), indicating significant and stable between-person differences (see Fig. 1). This suggests significant between-person effects linking the stable variance components of these constructs. Additionally, the RI-CLPM showed that the paths from neuroticism at one time point to problematic gaming at the subsequent time point were statistically significant (βT1→T2 = 0.10, βT2→T3 = 0.09, βT3→T4 = 0.09, ps < 0.001). Similarly, the paths from problematic gaming at one time point to neuroticism at the subsequent time point were also statistically significant (βT1→T2 = 0.16, βT2→T3 = 0.15, βT3→T4 = 0.14, ps < 0.001).

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.

Standardized path coefficients of the final RI-CLPM for neuroticism and problematic gaming. Solid lines mean the path coefficients are statistically significant, whereas dotted lines mean the path coefficients are not statistically significant. For simplicity, control variables are not presented in the figure. N = neuroticism, PG = problematic gaming; RI = random intercept.

***p < 0.001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 2025; 10.1556/2006.2024.00069

RI-CLPM of extraversion and problematic gaming

The RI-CLPM for extraversion and problematic gaming demonstrated excellent model fit. Table 3 presents the fit indices for both constrained and unconstrained RI-CLPMs and the results of the S-Bχ2 difference tests. Upon comparison, models that constrained the autoregressive paths, as well as those that constrained the lagged paths from extraversion to problematic gaming, demonstrated significantly worse fit compared to the fully unconstrained model. We found no significant differences in fit between the model with constrained lagged paths from problematic gaming to extraversion and the fully unconstrained model. After comparing these models, we selected the model which constrained the lagged paths from problematic gaming to extraversion to be time-invariant, as the final model. This model demonstrated parsimony and good fit.

Table 3.

Fit statistics and model comparisons of RI-CLPMs for extraversion and problematic gaming

ModelsFreely estimated pathsFixed time-invariant pathsS-Bχ2dfScaling Correction Factor for MLRRMSEASRMRCFITLIS-BΔχ2ΔdfComparison modelpSelected Model
M2aAll autoregressive and cross-lagged pathsNone71.07211.16220.0270.0190.9910.976
M2bAutoregressive paths of problematic gaming;Autoregressive paths of extraversion98.83231.15760.0320.0400.9860.96728.672M1b vs. M1a0.000M1a
Lagged paths from extraversion to problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from problematic gaming to extraversion
M2cAutoregressive paths of extraversion;Autoregressive paths of problematic gaming83.76231.23840.0280.0220.9880.97410.372M1c vs. M1a0.006M1a
Lagged paths from extraversion to problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from problematic gaming to extraversion
M2dAutoregressive paths of extraversion;Lagged paths from problematic gaming to extraversion72.99231.16800.0260.0190.9910.9792.162M1d vs. M1a0.34M1d
Autoregressive paths of problematic gaming;
Lagged paths from extraversion to problematic gaming
M2eAutoregressive paths of neuroticism;Lagged paths from neuroticism to problematic gaming;80.20251.18230.0260.0200.9900.9806.222M1e vs. M1d0.072M1d
Autoregressive paths of problematic gamingLagged paths from problematic gaming to neuroticism

Note. Bold indicates final selected model.

Figure 2 presents the standardized path coefficients for the final RI-CLPM of extraversion and problematic gaming. At the between-person level, the random intercepts for extraversion and problematic gaming were significantly correlated (r = −0.39, p < 0.001), indicating significant and stable between-person differences (see Fig. 2). Additionally, in the final RI-CLPM, the lagged effects of extraversion on problematic gaming were not constrained to be equal across time (i.e., freely estimated). The predictive effect of extraversion at T1 on problematic gaming at T2 was not statistically significant, while the effects of extraversion at T2 on problematic gaming at T3 (β = −0.07, p < 0.001) and extraversion at T3 on problematic gaming at T4 (β = −0.09, p < 0.001) were both statistically significant. The paths from problematic gaming at one time point to extraversion at the subsequent time point, fixed to be time-invariant, were also statistically significant (βT1→T2 = −0.11, βT2→T3 = −0.10, βT3→T4 = −0.10, ps < 0.001).

Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.

Standardized path coefficients of the final RI-CLPM for extraversion and problematic gaming. Solid lines mean the path coefficients are statistically significant, whereas dotted lines mean the path coefficients are not statistically significant. For simplicity, control variables are not presented in the figure. E = extraversion, PG = problematic gaming; RI = random intercept.

**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 2025; 10.1556/2006.2024.00069

Sex difference

The multiple-group comparison revealed significant sex differences in the cross-lagged effects in the final RI-CLPM of neuroticism and problematic gaming (ΔS-Bχ2/df = 5.33, p < 0.01). As presented in Fig. 3, for boys, there were significant cross-lagged effects between neuroticism and problematic gaming over time. In contrast, for girls, only the predictive effect of problematic gaming on neuroticism was statistically significant, whereas the effect of neuroticism on problematic gaming was not statistically significant. Further Wald tests indicated that the effect of problematic gaming on neuroticism did not significantly differ between boys and girls (Wald χ2 = 2.07, df = 1, p = 0.15). Unsurprisingly, the predictive effect of neuroticism on problematic gaming was significantly greater in boys than in girls (Wald χ2 = 10.81, df = 1, p < 0.01).

Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.

Standardized path coefficients of the final RI-CLPM for neuroticism and problematic gaming in boys (upper half) and girls (lower half). Solid lines mean the path coefficients are significant, whereas dotted lines mean the path coefficients are not significant. For simplicity, control variables are not presented in the figure. N = neuroticism, PG = problematic gaming; RI = random intercept.

*p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 2025; 10.1556/2006.2024.00069

Moreover, the final RI-CLPM for extraversion and problematic gaming did not show significant sex differences (ΔS-Bχ2/df = 0.25, p = 0.78). This suggests that the longitudinal relations between extraversion and problematic gaming operate similarly for both boys and girls.

Sensitivity analyses

Building on the foundational analysis of RI-CLPM, this research follows the recommendations of recent scholars (e.g., Tseng, 2024) by incorporating eight additional statistical models to analyze longitudinal bidirectional relations, conduct sensitivity analyses, and confirm the stability of the results. First, the Dynamic Panel Model (DPM, Allison, Williams, & Moral-Benito, 2017), the Random Intercept Auto-Regressive Moving Average Model (RI-ARMA, Asparouhov & Muthén, 2023), and the Latent Curve Model with Structured Residuals (LCM-SR, Curran, Howard, Bainter, Lane, & McGinley, 2014) were employed. These three models appropriately estimated the data, and the primary results were largely consistent with those obtained from the RI-CLPM. Additionally, five other models were attempted but either failed to converge or had issues with the latent variable covariance matrix not being positive definite: the General Cross-Lagged Model (GCLM, Zyphur et al., 2020), the Factor Cross-Lagged Panel Model (Factor CLPM, McArdle, 2009), the Autoregressive Latent Trajectory Model (ALT, Curran & Bollen, 2001), the Latent Change Score Model (LCS, McArdle & Hamagami, 2001), and the Stable Trait Autoregressive Trait State Model (STARTS, Kenny & Zautra, 2001). These sensitivity analyses support the robustness and stability of the findings, as consistent results across convergent models reaffirm the bidirectional relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming, as well as extraversion and problematic gaming, observed in the primary RI-CLPM analysis.

Discussion

Adolescent problematic gaming has become a significant public health concern worldwide (e.g., Männikkö et al., 2020). The negative consequences associated with problematic gaming, such as academic problems, social isolation, and psychopathological symptoms, underscore the urgency of understanding the factors that contribute to problematic gaming (e.g., Ferguson, Coulson, & Barnett, 2011). The Big Two personality traits (i.e., neuroticism and extraversion) have been well-documented to play a pivotal role in shaping adolescents' susceptibility to problematic gaming (Akbari et al., 2021; Chew, 2022). However, most previous studies have been cross-sectional, limiting the ability to infer causality or examine the directionality of relations over time, making it difficult to understand the dynamic interplay between personality traits and problematic gaming behaviors. To address this research gap, we employed a longitudinal design and utilized the RI-CLPM to examine the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits and problematic gaming at the within-person level. Our analyses revealed a significant positive cross-lagged relation between neuroticism and problematic gaming. Similarly, there was a significant negative cross-lagged relation between extraversion and problematic gaming. Moreover, the longitudinal relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming exhibited significant sex differences.

Neuroticism and problematic gaming

Our study suggests that neuroticism may predict increases in problematic gaming over time, and similarly, problematic gaming might also predict increases in neuroticism among young people. Adolescents high in neuroticism are often overwhelmed by intense emotions such as anxiety, sadness, and frustration, which might make coping effectively challenging (Evans et al., 2016; Gomez, Holmberg, Bounds, Fullarton, & Gomez, 1999). Online games may provide an appealing escape, offering a virtual environment where these emotional discomforts could be temporarily set aside (Calleja, 2010). For adolescents with high neuroticism, gaming could become a refuge from real-life challenges, potentially serving as a coping mechanism to manage their emotional turbulence (Dieris-Hirche et al., 2020). This cycle could perpetuate as gaming becomes a habitual response to emotional distress. The immediate rewards and temporary escape from reality provided by gaming might lead to increased usage and, potentially, problematic gaming behaviors. Additionally, the increase in problematic gaming might exacerbate neuroticism in adolescents through several psychological mechanisms. As adolescents spend more time gaming, they might face prolonged exposure to interpersonal stress, which could act as a catalyst for their inherent anxieties (e.g., Ojha et al., 2023; Przybylski et al., 2014), possibly leading to heightened emotional reactivity. The constant exposure to the immediate rewards and punishments in games could create a feedback loop that might increase sensitivity to negative feedback (e.g., Lee et al., 2015; Nesi et al., 2022; Raiha et al., 2020; Weinstein et al., 2017), further fueling feelings of frustration and emotional instability.

Moreover, our findings suggest that significant sex differences may exist in the bidirectional relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming. Specifically, in boys, there seems to be a significant bidirectional relation between neuroticism and problematic gaming. In contrast, for girls, neuroticism does not significantly predict problematic gaming, although problematic gaming does seem to have a significant effect on increasing neuroticism, with this effect being similar to that observed in boys. The societal expectations placed on boys to exhibit emotional control and resilience can intensify the internal conflict experienced by those with high neuroticism (Löckenhoff et al., 2014; Mahalik et al., 2007). This conflict might drive boys to engage in problematic gaming as a means of coping with emotional sensitivity and anxiety, potentially creating a cycle of dependency on gaming for emotional regulation (Chumbley & Griffiths, 2006). The immediate rewards and sense of control provided by gaming offer a stark contrast to the emotional challenges faced in their everyday lives, making gaming an appealing, albeit maladaptive, coping mechanism (Desai, Zhao, & Szafron, 2016). On the other hand, boys are often more engaged in competitive and immersive gaming environments (e.g., Su et al., 2020). However, this constant exposure to competitive stress and the drive to achieve high performance may intensify boys' emotional reactions (Wang et al., 2022). Failures and negative feedback within these games can exacerbate feelings of frustration and anxiety, leading to increased emotional instability (Villani et al., 2018). Moreover, the immediate rewards and perceived control offered by gaming are especially attractive to boys, while the emotional toll of constant competition and the pressure to perform well can deepen their neurotic tendencies (e.g., Su et al., 2020), thus creating a vicious cycle that perpetuates both problematic gaming behaviors and heightened neuroticism among boys.

Our findings indicate that, in contrast to boys, neuroticism does not significantly predict problematic gaming in girls. Several factors may explain this difference. For instance, girls are generally socialized to seek and value social support more than boys (Belle, 1991). This means that girls might be more likely to turn to friends, family, or other social networks when dealing with emotional distress rather than relying solely on gaming as a coping mechanism (e.g., Liang, Zhou, Yuan, Shao, & Bian, 2016). The availability of these social supports can mitigate the need to use gaming as a primary way to manage negative emotions, thereby reducing the likelihood that neuroticism will lead to problematic gaming. Additionally, the societal expectations and norms for emotional expression in girls might also play a role. Girls are often encouraged to express their emotions and seek help (e.g., Deng, Chang, Yang, Huo, & Zhou, 2016), which may help them process and manage their feelings more effectively without resorting to gaming. These sex differences underscore the importance of understanding how boys and girls interact with gaming environments and cope with emotional distress. By recognizing these patterns, we can develop more effective, sex-sensitive interventions that address the specific needs of each group, thereby promoting healthier gaming habits and better mental health outcomes for adolescents.

Extraversion and problematic gaming

Our study suggests that problematic gaming might contribute to diminishing extraversion in youth over time. Adolescents who engage heavily in gaming might reduce their real-world social interactions, which may lead to social isolation (e.g., Nawaz, Nadeem, Rao, Fatima, & Shoaib, 2020). This reduced face-to-face engagement may limit their social skills and confidence, making them less inclined to participate in social activities. As they might prioritize gaming over spending time with friends and family, their social networks could weaken, potentially reducing the positive reinforcement from real-world interactions and thereby possibly diminishing their extraversion (Dieris-Hirche et al., 2020). Additionally, the negative emotional consequences commonly associated with problematic gaming, such as increased stress, anxiety, and depression (e.g., Männikkö et al., 2020), might further diminish extraversion in adolescents (Klimstra, Akse, Hale III, Raaijmakers, & Meeus, 2010; Morken, Wichstrøm, Steinsbekk, & Viddal, 2024). These negative emotions could make social interactions seem more daunting and less enjoyable, potentially reducing their motivation to engage with others (e.g., Elmer & Stadtfeld, 2020). As problematic gaming continues to exacerbate these negative emotional states, adolescents may find themselves increasingly overwhelmed by their emotions, leading to a further decline in extraversion.

Preliminary results from our study also indicate that higher levels of extraversion may act as a protective factor, thus reducing the likelihood of adolescents becoming addicted to online gaming. Extraversion, characterized by sociability, assertiveness, and a tendency to engage with others (Eysenck, 1990), might encourage the formation of strong social networks (e.g., William, Ling, & Woon, 2016). These networks could provide support, companionship, and a sense of belonging, possibly making extraverts less likely to turn to gaming as their primary means of social interaction and emotional support (de Hesselle, Rozgonjuk, Sindermann, Pontes, & Montag, 2021). The real-life community and connections they cultivate could act as a buffer against the lure of online gaming, providing them with a robust emotional foundation. Moreover, extraverts tend to participate in a wide range of activities which might provide numerous sources of satisfaction and accomplishment (Lu & Hu, 2005). The stimulation derived from these activities could fulfill their need for novelty and engagement, possibly making gaming just one of many interests rather than an all-consuming pastime. Additionally, extraverts' effective communication and assertiveness in social situations could lead to better stress management and conflict resolution skills (Sims, 2017). These abilities might help them navigate emotional challenges without resorting to gaming for escape. By actively engaging in diverse face-to-face social activities and maintaining strong interpersonal relationships, extraverts may achieve emotional regulation and fulfillment in ways that naturally protect against the risks of problematic gaming.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

Our study has several notable strengths. By employing a four-wave longitudinal design with one-year intervals, our research offers robust evidence for the bidirectional relationships between the Big Two personality traits (neuroticism and extraversion) and problematic gaming. This design allows for examining changes over time and establishing temporal precedence, which is crucial for understanding causality. Focusing on bidirectional relations enables a comprehensive understanding of how personality traits and problematic gaming influence each other, highlighting their dynamic interplay. Additionally, the large and diverse sample enhances the generalizability of our findings across different demographic groups, thereby increasing the relevance and applicability of the results to a wider adolescent population.

Despite these strengths, our study also has some limitations. One significant limitation is the use of the POGU scale to measure problematic gaming. The POGU is based on the DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling, which, despite providing valuable insights, may not fully align with the most current diagnostic standards for gaming disorder as outlined in the DSM-5 and ICD-11. The outdated basis of the POGU implies that it may not accurately capture the latest conceptualization of gaming disorder, potentially affecting the validity of our findings. Additionally, the POGU shares core features with Brown's (1991) components model of addiction, which focuses on aspects such as salience, euphoria, and withdrawal symptoms. While these features are important in assessing addictive behaviors, using older criteria may lead to an overemphasis on certain dimensions of gaming behavior while overlooking other dimensions now considered significant, such as gaming despite harms, conflict/interference due to gaming, or impaired control, as per the DSM-5 and ICD-11. The reliance on an outdated diagnostic framework can also introduce potential biases in identifying and categorizing problematic gaming behaviors, particularly in the context of a rapidly evolving digital environment. This discrepancy may also limit the generalizability and accuracy of the findings. As such, caution is warranted in interpreting the results. To address these concerns, future research should employ more contemporary assessment tools aligning with the latest diagnostic criteria for gaming disorder. This would improve the overall precision and relevance of findings, enhancing our understanding of the interplay between personality traits and gaming behaviors.

Moreover, the reliance on self-reported data in this study can introduce bias, as participants may underreport or overreport their gaming behaviors and personality traits, with social desirability and recall bias potentially affecting the accuracy of the responses. Relying solely on self-reported questionnaires limits the depth of our data. Incorporating other methods, such as behavioral observations or parent/teacher reports, could provide a more comprehensive understanding of participants' behaviors and traits. Additionally, the study was conducted within a Chinese cultural context, which may limit the generalizability of the findings to other cultural settings. Cultural differences in gaming behaviors and personality expression should be considered when interpreting the results. Expanding research to different cultural contexts would help identify cultural factors that influence the relation between personality traits and gaming behaviors, with comparative studies offering insights into how cultural norms and values shape these dynamics.

Another limitation is that our study focused solely on the Big Two personality traits—neuroticism and extraversion. Future research could explore the role of other personality dimensions, such as conscientiousness, agreeableness, and openness to experience, in relation to problematic gaming. Furthermore, our study did not collect data on general gaming engagement among participants. The absence of this information limits our ability to contextualize the prevalence of gaming within the sample, which could have provided valuable insight into the broader landscape of adolescent gaming behavior. We recommend that future research include data on general gaming engagement to provide a more comprehensive understanding of adolescents' gaming habits.

Implications

The findings from this study not only deepen our theoretical understanding of how personality traits and problematic gaming influence each other but also provide practical guidance for educators, parents, and mental health professionals working to support the healthy personality development of adolescents and mitigate the risks of problematic gaming. Firstly, the risk effect of neuroticism on adolescent problematic gaming underscores the need for targeted interventions focused on emotional regulation and stress management for adolescents exhibiting high levels of neuroticism. Research has demonstrated that mindfulness-based interventions (Armstrong & Rimes, 2016; Hanley, de Vibe, Solhaug, Gonzalez-Pons, & Garland, 2019) can significantly reduce levels of neuroticism. By fostering emotional regulation and enhancing self-awareness, mindfulness practices provide adolescents with healthier alternatives for managing stress and anxiety (Meiklejohn et al., 2012). When integrated into school mental health education curricula, these programs can teach coping strategies that help students manage their emotions more effectively and reduce their reliance on gaming as a coping mechanism (Sharma, Bhargav, Kumar, Digambhar, & Mani, 2021). Similarly, the protective role of extraversion against problematic gaming suggests that promoting social activities and encouraging face-to-face interactions can serve as protective factors. Schools and communities should create opportunities for adolescents to participate in sports, clubs, and other group activities that foster social connections and provide alternative sources of engagement and satisfaction outside of gaming (King & Delfabbro, 2014).

Moreover, the significant sex differences in the longitudinal relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming suggest that interventions should be tailored to address these differences. For boys, who exhibit a stronger bidirectional relation between neuroticism and problematic gaming, interventions should focus on providing alternative ways to achieve a sense of competence and control (Zhou, Zhang, & Gong, 2023). Encouraging boys to engage in various activities, such as team sports and community service, can help them build skills, gain confidence, and experience emotional fulfillment, while also providing physical and emotional benefits. These activities offer healthy avenues for developing a sense of competence and control, thereby reducing their reliance on gaming to meet these needs (Liu, Wang, Zhai, Luo, & Xin, 2023). For girls, who show a significant impact of problematic gaming on neuroticism but not the reverse, interventions should emphasize maintaining social support networks and engaging in cooperative, narrative-driven activities that provide emotional engagement without the high stress associated with competitive gaming (Su et al., 2020). Enhancing girls' access to supportive social environments and teaching effective coping strategies can help reduce the negative emotional impacts of problematic gaming.

Our research also found that problematic gaming has a significant impact on youth personality development. Therefore, it is crucial to reduce or prevent problematic gaming in adolescents to promote healthy personality development. Limiting screen time, setting clear boundaries for gaming sessions, and encouraging breaks can help prevent excessive gaming. Parents and caregivers should monitor gaming behavior and foster open communication about the potential risks of excessive gaming (Pornnoppadol et al., 2020). Providing adolescents with diverse recreational options and promoting a healthy lifestyle, including regular physical activity, can divert their attention from gaming and reduce the likelihood of developing problematic habits (Pornnoppadol et al., 2020). Additionally, schools can play a critical role by integrating digital literacy and responsible gaming education into their curricula (Bonnaire, Serehen, & Phan, 2019). Teaching students about the potential dangers of excessive gaming and equipping them with skills to balance their online and offline activities can foster healthier gaming practices (Walther, Hanewinkel, & Morgenstern, 2014). Overall, our study underscores the importance of a holistic approach to addressing problematic gaming, one that considers the individual's personality traits, emotional needs, and social environment. By implementing targeted interventions and promoting healthy, balanced lifestyles, we can reduce the risks associated with problematic gaming and support the well-being of adolescents.

Conclusions

This longitudinal study provides critical insights into the bidirectional relations between the Big Two personality traits—neuroticism and extraversion—and problematic gaming among adolescents. By employing the RI-CLPM, we were able to examine these relations at the within-person level, addressing the limitations of previous cross-sectional studies. Our findings reveal that adolescent neuroticism and problematic gaming mutually predict each other, suggesting a reinforcing cycle. In contrast, extraversion serves as a protective factor, reducing the risk of problematic gaming, while engagement in problematic gaming negatively impacts extraversion over time. Importantly, the study highlights significant sex differences in the longitudinal relations between neuroticism and problematic gaming. These results underscore the necessity of incorporating personality assessments into interventions aimed at reducing problematic gaming. By targeting neuroticism and fostering extraversion, such interventions could potentially break the cycle of problematic gaming and support healthier development among adolescents.

Funding sources

This research was supported by grants from the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (No. 2021ZD0203804), and Funds for National Natural Science Youth Foundation of China (No. 32300898), Funds for Humanities and Social Sciences Youth Foundation, Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China (No. 22YJC190031), and Funds for General Project of Social Science Planning of Gansu Province (No. 2022YB057).

Authors' contribution

JZ: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, writing-original draft, writing-review & editing, visualization, supervision, funding acquisition. BM: methodology, formal analysis, writing-review & editing. TL: methodology, formal analysis, writing-review & editing. TB: methodology, formal analysis, writing-original draft, project administration. XG: methodology, writing-original draft, writing-review & editing.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • Addis, M. E., & Mahalik, J. R. (2003). Men, masculinity, and the contexts of help seeking. American Psychologist, 58(1), 514. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Akbari, M., Seydavi, M., Spada, M. M., Mohammadkhani, S., Jamshidi, S., Jamaloo, A., & Ayatmehr, F. (2021). The big five personality traits and online gaming: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(3), 611625. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Allison, P., Williams, R., & Moral-Benito, E. (2017). Maximum likelihood for cross-lagged panel models with fixed effects. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World, 3, 237802311771057. https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023117710578.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association [APA]. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed., Text revision (DSM-5-TR). Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ang, R. P., Chong, W. H., Chye, S., & Huan, V. S. (2012). Loneliness and generalized problematic internet use: Parents’ perceived knowledge of adolescents’ online activities as a moderator. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(4), 13421347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Armstrong, L., & Rimes, K. A. (2016). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for neuroticism (stress vulnerability): A pilot randomized study. Behavior Therapy, 47(3), 287298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.12.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2023). Residual structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 30(1), 131. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2022.2074422.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Belle, D. (1991). Gender differences in the social moderators of stress. In A. Monat, & R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (3rd ed., pp. 258274). Columbia University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bonnaire, C., Serehen, Z., & Phan, O. (2019). Effects of a prevention intervention concerning screens, and video games in middle-school students: Influences on beliefs and use. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(3), 537553. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.54.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Borghuis, J., Denissen, J. J., Oberski, D., Sijtsma, K., Meeus, W. H., Branje, S., & Bleidorn, W. (2017). Big Five personality stability, change, and codevelopment across adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(4), 641657. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000138.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brown, R. I. F. (1991). Gaming, gambling and other addictive play. In J. H. Kerr, & M. J. Apter (Eds.), Adult place: A reversal theory approach (pp. 101118). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Calleja, G. (2010). Digital games and escapism. Games and Culture: A Journal of Interactive Media, 5(4), 335353. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412009360412.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chan, G., Huo, Y., Kelly, S., Leung, J., Tisdale, C., & Gullo, M. (2022). The impact of eSports and online video gaming on lifestyle behaviours in youth: A systematic review. Computers in Human Behavior, 126, 106974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106974.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chew, P. K. (2022). A meta-analytic review of Internet gaming disorder and the Big Five personality factors. Addictive Behaviors, 126, 107193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107193.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chumbley, J., & Griffiths, M. (2006). Affect and the computer game player: The effect of gender, personality, and game reinforcement structure on affective responses to computer game-play. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(3), 308316. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curran, P. J., & Bollen, K. A. (2001). The best of both worlds: Combining autoregressive and latent curve models. In L. M. Collins, & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 105136). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curran, P. J., Howard, A. L., Bainter, S. A., Lane, S. T., & McGinley, J. S. (2014). The separation of between-person and within-person components of individual change over time: A latent curve model with structured residuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(5), 879894. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035297.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de Hesselle, L. C., Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Pontes, H. M., & Montag, C. (2021). The associations between Big Five personality traits, gaming motives, and self-reported time spent gaming. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110483.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deng, Y., Chang, L., Yang, M., Huo, M., & Zhou, R. (2016). Gender differences in emotional response: Inconsistency between experience and expressivity. Plos One, 11(6), e0158666. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158666.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Desai, N., Zhao, R., & Szafron, D. (2016). Effects of gender on perception and interpretation of video game character behavior and emotion. IEEE Transactions on Computational Intelligence and AI in Games, 9(4), 333341. https://doi.org/10.1109/TCIAIG.2016.2570006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dieris-Hirche, J., Pape, M., te Wildt, B. T., Kehyayan, A., Esch, M., Aicha, S., … Bottel, L. (2020). Problematic gaming behavior and the personality traits of video gamers: A cross-sectional survey. Computers in Human Behavior, 106, 106272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dong, G. H., & Potenza, M. N. (2022). Considering gender differences in the study and treatment of internet gaming disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 153, 2529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.06.057.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elmer, T., & Stadtfeld, C. (2020). Depressive symptoms are associated with social isolation in face-to-face interaction networks. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1444. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-58297-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Erevik, E. K., Landrø, H., Mattson, Å. L., Kristensen, J. H., Kaur, P., & Pallesen, S. (2022). Problem gaming and suicidality: A systematic literature review. Addictive behaviors reports, 15, 100419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2022.100419.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Evans, B. E., Stam, J., Huizink, A. C., Willemen, A. M., Westenberg, P. M., Branje, S., & van Lier, P. A. (2016). Neuroticism and extraversion in relation to physiological stress reactivity during adolescence. Biological Psychology, 117, 6779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.03.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eysenck, H. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 244276). The Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fagot, B. I., Rodgers, C. S., & Leinbach, M. D. (2000). Theories of gender socialization. In T. Eckes, & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 6589). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410605245-10.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fam, J. Y. (2018). Prevalence of internet gaming disorder in adolescents: A meta‐analysis across three decades. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(5), 524531. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12459.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Feng, W., Ramo, D. E., Chan, S. R., & Bourgeois, J. A. (2017). Internet gaming disorder: Trends in prevalence 1998–2016. Addictive Behaviors, 75, 1724. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.06.010.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(12), 15731578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fernandes, B., Maia, B. R., & Pontes, H. M. (2019). Internet addiction or problematic internet use? Which term should be used? Psicologia USP, 30, e190020. https://doi.org/10.1590/0103-6564E190020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Francis, L. J. (1996). The development of an abbreviated form of the revised Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (JEPQR-A) among 13–15 year olds. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 835844. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00159-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gao, X., Zhang, M., Yang, Z., Wen, M., Huang, H., Zheng, R., & Zhang, Y. (2021). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in internet gaming disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 679437. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.679437.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video–game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national-study. Psychological Science, 20(5), 594602. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gervasi, A. M., La Marca, L., Costanzo, A., Pace, U., Guglielmucci, F., & Schimmenti, A. (2017). Personality and internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of recent literature. Current Addiction Reports, 4, 293307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0159-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Giardina, A., Schimmenti, A., Starcevic, V., King, D. L., Di Blasi, M., & Billieux, J. (2024). Problematic gaming, social withdrawal, and escapism: The compensatory-dissociative online gaming (C-DOG) model. Computers in Human Behavior, 155, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2024.108187.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gomez, R., Holmberg, K., Bounds, J., Fullarton, C., & Gomez, A. (1999). Neuroticism and extraversion as predictors of coping styles during early adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1), 317. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00224-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanley, A. W., de Vibe, M., Solhaug, I., Gonzalez-Pons, K., & Garland, E. L. (2019). Mindfulness training reduces neuroticism over a 6-year longitudinal randomized control trial in Norwegian medical and psychology students. Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103859.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hygen, B. W., Belsky, J., Stenseng, F., Skalicka, V., Kvande, M. N., Zahl‐Thanem, T., & Wichstrøm, L. (2020). Time spent gaming and social competence in children: Reciprocal effects across childhood. Child Development, 91(3), 861875. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13243.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kenny, D. A., & Zautra, A. (2001). Trait-state models for longitudinal data. In L. M. Collins, & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 243263). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, D., Nam, J. K., & Keum, C. (2022). Adolescent Internet gaming addiction and personality characteristics by game genre. Plos One, 17(2), e0263645. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263645.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, H. S., Son, G., Roh, E. B., Ahn, W. Y., Kim, J., Shin, S. H., & Choi, K. H. (2022). Prevalence of gaming disorder: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 126, 107183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107183.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2014). The cognitive psychology of Internet gaming disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(4), 298308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2018). Internet gaming disorder: Theory, assessment, treatment, and prevention. Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/C2016-0-04107-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klimstra, T. A., Akse, J., Hale III, W. W., Raaijmakers, Q. A., & Meeus, W. H. (2010). Longitudinal associations between personality traits and problem behavior symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(2), 273284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kristensen, J. H., Pallesen, S., King, D. L., Hysing, M., & Erevik, E. K. (2021). Problematic gaming and sleep: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 675237. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.675237.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, J., Lee, S., Chun, J. W., Cho, H., Kim, D. J., & Jung, Y. C. (2015). Compromised prefrontal cognitive control over emotional interference in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18(11), 661668. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0231.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liang, L., Zhou, D., Yuan, C., Shao, A., & Bian, Y. (2016). Gender differences in the relationship between internet addiction and depression: A cross-lagged study in Chinese adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 463470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liao, Z., Chen, X., Huang, Q., & Shen, H. (2022). Prevalence of gaming disorder in East Asia: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(3), 727738. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lippa, R. A. (2010). Gender differences in personality and interests: When, where, and why? Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 10981110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data. New Jersey: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119013563.

  • Liu, Y., Wang, X., Zhai, J., Luo, F., & Xin, T. (2023). The effect of neuroticism on problematic smartphone use: A mediation model of self-control for males and females. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 17, 1. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2023-1-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Löckenhoff, C. E., Chan, W., McCrae, R. R., De Fruyt, F., Jussim, L., De Bolle, M., & Terracciano, A. (2014). Gender stereotypes of personality: Universal and accurate? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(5), 675694. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022113520075.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Long, J., Liu, T., Liu, Y., Hao, W., Maurage, P., & Billieux, J. (2018). Prevalence and correlates of problematic online gaming: A systematic review of the evidence published in Chinese. Current Addiction Reports, 5, 359371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-018-0219-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Fernandez, O., Williams, A. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Kuss, D. J. (2019). Female gaming, gaming addiction, and the role of women within gaming culture: A narrative literature review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 449322. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00454.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lu, L., & Hu, C. H. (2005). Personality, leisure experiences and happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies: An Interdisciplinary Forum on Subjective Well-Being, 6(3), 325342. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-005-8628-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Macur, M., & Pontes, H. M. (2021). Internet gaming disorder in adolescence: Investigating profiles and associated risk factors. BMC Public Health, 21, 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11394-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mahalik, J. R., Burns, S. M., & Syzdek, M. (2007). Masculinity and perceived normative health behaviors as predictors of men's health behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 64(11), 22012209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.035.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mahalik, J. R., Morray, E. B., Coonerty-Femiano, A., Ludlow, L. H., Slattery, S. M., & Smiler, A. (2005). Development of the conformity to feminine norms inventory. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 52(7–8), 417435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-3709-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Männikkö, N., Ruotsalainen, H., Miettunen, J., Pontes, H. M., & Kääriäinen, M. (2020). Problematic gaming behaviour and health-related outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 25(1), 6781. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105317740414.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577605. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (2001). Latent difference score structural models for linear dynamic analyses with incomplete longitudinal data. In L. Collins, & A. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 137175). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meiklejohn, J., Phillips, C., Freedman, M. L., Griffin, M. L., Biegel, G., Roach, A., & Saltzman, A. (2012). Integrating mindfulness training into K-12 education: Fostering the resilience of teachers and students. Mindfulness, 3, 291307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0094-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morken, I. S., Wichstrøm, L., Steinsbekk, S., & Viddal, K. R. (2024). Depression and personality traits across adolescence—within-person analyses of a birth cohort. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-024-01188-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nawaz, M. W., Nadeem, T., Rao, S. L., Fatima, T., & Shoaib, S. (2020). Impact of PUBG game addiction on social isolation and narcissistic tendencies among gamers. Asian Journal of Social Sciences and Management Studies, 7(3), 166172. https://doi.org/10.20448/journal.500.2020.73.166.172.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nesi, J., Rothenberg, W. A., Bettis, A. H., Massing-Schaffer, M., Fox, K. A., Telzer, E. H., & Prinstein, M. J. (2022). Emotional responses to social media experiences among adolescents: Longitudinal associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 51(6), 907922. https://doi.org/10.1037/t87255-000.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nicholson, J. S., Deboeck, P. R., & Howard, W. (2017). Attrition in developmental psychology: A review of modern missing data reporting and practices. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41, 143153. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415618275.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nielsen, P., Favez, N., & Rigter, H. (2020). Parental and family factors associated with problematic gaming and problematic internet use in adolescents: A systematic literature review. Current Addiction Reports, 7, 365386. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-020-00320-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ojha, S. P., Shaikh, M., Gupta, M., Shinde, A., Waghmare, S., & Shukla, S. (2023, January). Study the effects of online gaming on different emotional behavior of the users. In International Conference on Research into design (pp. 445457). Singapore: Springer Nature Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-0293-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orth, U., Clark, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2021). Testing prospective effects in longitudinal research: Comparing seven competing cross-lagged models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 10131034. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000358.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pan, P. Y., & Yeh, C. B. (2018). Internet addiction among adolescents may predict self-harm/suicidal behavior: A prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 197, 262267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.01.046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Ko, C. H., & O’Brien, C. P. (2015). Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17, 72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0610-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Phan, M. H., Jardina, J. R., Hoyle, S., & Chaparro, B. S. (2012, September). Examining the role of gender in video game usage, preference, and behavior. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting (Vol. 56, No. 1, pp. 14961500). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pornnoppadol, C., Ratta-apha, W., Chanpen, S., Wattananond, S., Dumrongrungruang, N., Thongchoi, K., & Vasupanrajit, A. (2020). A comparative study of psychosocial interventions for internet gaming disorder among adolescents aged 13–17 years. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 18, 932948. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-9995-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Przybylski, A. K., Deci, E. L., Rigby, C. S., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Competence-impeding electronic games and players’ aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(3), 441457. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034820.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Purwaningsih, E., & Nurmala, I. (2021). The impact of online game addiction on adolescent mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Open Access Macedonian Journal of Medical Sciences (OAMJMS), 9(F), 260274. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2021.6234.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raiha, S., Yang, G., Wang, L., Dai, W., Wu, H., Meng, G., … Liu, X. (2020). Altered reward processing system in internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, Article 599141. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.599141.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rosendo-Rios, V., Trott, S., & Shukla, P. (2022). Systematic literature review online gaming addiction among children and young adults: A framework and research agenda. Addictive Behaviors, 129, 107238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107238.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Şalvarlı, Ş. İ., & Griffiths, M. D. (2021). Internet gaming disorder and its associated personality traits: A systematic review using PRISMA guidelines. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 19, 14201442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00081-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75, 243. https://doi:10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Savitz, D. A., & Wellenius, G. A. (2023). Can cross-sectional studies contribute to causal inference? It depends. American Journal of Epidemiology, 192(4), 514516. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwac037.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schrock, D., & Schwalbe, M. (2009). Men, masculinity, and manhood acts. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 277295. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115933.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sharma, M. K., Bhargav, H., Kumar, A., Digambhar, V., & Mani, T. A. (2021). Mindfulness-based interventions: Potentials for management of internet gaming disorder. International Journal of Yoga, 14(3), 244247. https://doi.org/10.4103/ijoy.ijoy_46_21.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sims, C. M. (2017). Do the big-five personality traits predict empathic listening and assertive communication? International Journal of Listening, 31(3), 163188. https://doi.org/10.1080/10904018.2016.1202770.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Slobodskaya, H. R., & Kornienko, O. S. (2021). Age and gender differences in personality traits from early childhood through adolescence. Journal of Personality, 89(5), 933950. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snodgrass, J. G., Lacy, M. G., Dengah II, H. F., Eisenhauer, S., Batchelder, G., & Cookson, R. J. (2014). A vacation from your mind: Problematic online gaming is a stress response. Computers in Human Behavior, 38, 248260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens, M. W., Dorstyn, D., Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2021). Global prevalence of gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 55(6), 553568. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867420962851.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Su, W., Han, X., Yu, H., Wu, Y., & Potenza, M. N. (2020). Do men become addicted to internet gaming and women to social media? A meta-analysis examining gender-related differences in specific internet addiction. Computers in Human Behavior, 113, 106480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106480.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swingle, M. K. (2016). i-Minds: How cell phones, computers, gaming, and social media are changing our brains, our behavior, and the evolution of our species. Gabriola Island, BC Canada: New Society Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Syed, M., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2013). Personality development from adolescence to emerging adulthood: Linking trajectories of ego development to the family context and identity formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 371384. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030070.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thulin, E., Vilhelmson, B., & Schwanen, T. (2020). Absent friends? Smartphones, mediated presence, and the recoupling of online social contact in everyday life. Annals of the American Association of Geographers, 110(1), 166183. https://doi.org/10.1080/24694452.2019.1629868.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tian, L., Jiang, S., & Huebner, E. S. (2019). The big two personality traits and adolescents’ complete mental health: The mediation role of perceived school stress. School Psychology, 34, 3242. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tseng, M. C. (2024). Fitting cross-lagged panel models with the residual structural equations approach. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 31(5), 923931. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2023.2296862.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tullett-Prado, D., Stavropoulos, V., Mueller, K., Sharples, J., & Footitt, T. A. (2021). Internet gaming disorder profiles and their associations with social engagement behaviours. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 138, 393403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.04.037.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uz, C., & Cagiltay, K. (2015). Social interactions and games. Digital Education Review, 27, 112. https://doi.org/10.1344/der.2015.27.1-12.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Villani, D., Carissoli, C., Triberti, S., Marchetti, A., Gilli, G., & Riva, G. (2018). Videogames for emotion regulation: A systematic review. Games for Health Journal, 7(2), 8599. https://doi.org/10.1089/g4h.2017.0108.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Walther, B., Hanewinkel, R., & Morgenstern, M. (2014). Effects of a brief school-based media literacy intervention on digital media use in adolescents: Cluster randomized controlled trial. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(9), 616623. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0173.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, Z. L., Song, K. R., Zhou, N., Potenza, M. N., Zhang, J. T., & Dong, G. H. (2022). Gender-related differences in involvement of addiction brain networks in internet gaming disorder: Relationships with craving and emotional regulation. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 118, 110574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2022.110574.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, C. Y., Wu, Y. C., Su, C. H., Lin, P. C., Ko, C. H., & Yen, J. Y. (2017). Association between Internet gaming disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 564571. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.088.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A., Livny, A., & Weizman, A. (2017). New developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 75, 314330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.040.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weisberg, Y. J., DeYoung, C. G., & Hirsh, J. B. (2011). Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big Five. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 178. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00178.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • William, T., Ling, N. Z., & Woon, L. H. (2016). Extraversion and agreeableness: Divergent routes to daily satisfaction with social relationships. Journal of Personality, 84(1), 121134. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12146.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization (WHO) (2019). ICD-11 mortality andmorbidity statistics. Mental, behavioural or neurodevelopmental disorders. Available at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhao, H., Li, X., Zhou, J., Nie, Q., & Zhou, J. (2020). The relationship between bullying victimization and online game addiction among Chinese early adolescents: The potential role of meaning in life and gender differences. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, 105261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105261.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, J., Zhang, L., & Gong, X. (2023). Longitudinal network relations between symptoms of problematic internet game use and internalizing and externalizing problems among Chinese early adolescents. Social Science & Medicine, 333, 116162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2023.116162.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zyphur, M. J., Allison, P. D., Tay, L., Voelkle, M. C., Preacher, K. J., Zhang, Z., … Diener, E. (2020). From data to causes I: Building a general cross-lagged panel model (GCLM). Organizational Research Methods, 23(4), 651687. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428119847278.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Addis, M. E., & Mahalik, J. R. (2003). Men, masculinity, and the contexts of help seeking. American Psychologist, 58(1), 514. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Akbari, M., Seydavi, M., Spada, M. M., Mohammadkhani, S., Jamshidi, S., Jamaloo, A., & Ayatmehr, F. (2021). The big five personality traits and online gaming: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 10(3), 611625. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2021.00050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Allison, P., Williams, R., & Moral-Benito, E. (2017). Maximum likelihood for cross-lagged panel models with fixed effects. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World, 3, 237802311771057. https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023117710578.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association [APA]. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th ed., Text revision (DSM-5-TR). Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ang, R. P., Chong, W. H., Chye, S., & Huan, V. S. (2012). Loneliness and generalized problematic internet use: Parents’ perceived knowledge of adolescents’ online activities as a moderator. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(4), 13421347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.02.019.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Armstrong, L., & Rimes, K. A. (2016). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for neuroticism (stress vulnerability): A pilot randomized study. Behavior Therapy, 47(3), 287298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2015.12.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2023). Residual structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 30(1), 131. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2022.2074422.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Belle, D. (1991). Gender differences in the social moderators of stress. In A. Monat, & R. S. Lazarus (Eds.), Stress and coping: An anthology (3rd ed., pp. 258274). Columbia University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bonnaire, C., Serehen, Z., & Phan, O. (2019). Effects of a prevention intervention concerning screens, and video games in middle-school students: Influences on beliefs and use. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 8(3), 537553. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.8.2019.54.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Borghuis, J., Denissen, J. J., Oberski, D., Sijtsma, K., Meeus, W. H., Branje, S., & Bleidorn, W. (2017). Big Five personality stability, change, and codevelopment across adolescence and early adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(4), 641657. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000138.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brand, M., Wegmann, E., Stark, R., Müller, A., Wölfling, K., Robbins, T. W., & Potenza, M. N. (2019). The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 104, 110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brown, R. I. F. (1991). Gaming, gambling and other addictive play. In J. H. Kerr, & M. J. Apter (Eds.), Adult place: A reversal theory approach (pp. 101118). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Calleja, G. (2010). Digital games and escapism. Games and Culture: A Journal of Interactive Media, 5(4), 335353. https://doi.org/10.1177/1555412009360412.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chan, G., Huo, Y., Kelly, S., Leung, J., Tisdale, C., & Gullo, M. (2022). The impact of eSports and online video gaming on lifestyle behaviours in youth: A systematic review. Computers in Human Behavior, 126, 106974. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106974.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chew, P. K. (2022). A meta-analytic review of Internet gaming disorder and the Big Five personality factors. Addictive Behaviors, 126, 107193. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107193.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chumbley, J., & Griffiths, M. (2006). Affect and the computer game player: The effect of gender, personality, and game reinforcement structure on affective responses to computer game-play. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(3), 308316. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.308.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curran, P. J., & Bollen, K. A. (2001). The best of both worlds: Combining autoregressive and latent curve models. In L. M. Collins, & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 105136). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Curran, P. J., Howard, A. L., Bainter, S. A., Lane, S. T., & McGinley, J. S. (2014). The separation of between-person and within-person components of individual change over time: A latent curve model with structured residuals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82(5), 879894. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035297.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de Hesselle, L. C., Rozgonjuk, D., Sindermann, C., Pontes, H. M., & Montag, C. (2021). The associations between Big Five personality traits, gaming motives, and self-reported time spent gaming. Personality and Individual Differences, 171, 110483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110483.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Deng, Y., Chang, L., Yang, M., Huo, M., & Zhou, R. (2016). Gender differences in emotional response: Inconsistency between experience and expressivity. Plos One, 11(6), e0158666. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158666.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Desai, N., Zhao, R., & Szafron, D. (2016). Effects of gender on perception and interpretation of video game character behavior and emotion. IEEE Transactions on Computational Intelligence and AI in Games, 9(4), 333341. https://doi.org/10.1109/TCIAIG.2016.2570006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dieris-Hirche, J., Pape, M., te Wildt, B. T., Kehyayan, A., Esch, M., Aicha, S., … Bottel, L. (2020). Problematic gaming behavior and the personality traits of video gamers: A cross-sectional survey. Computers in Human Behavior, 106, 106272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106272.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dong, G. H., & Potenza, M. N. (2022). Considering gender differences in the study and treatment of internet gaming disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 153, 2529. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.06.057.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Elmer, T., & Stadtfeld, C. (2020). Depressive symptoms are associated with social isolation in face-to-face interaction networks. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1444. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-58297-9.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Erevik, E. K., Landrø, H., Mattson, Å. L., Kristensen, J. H., Kaur, P., & Pallesen, S. (2022). Problem gaming and suicidality: A systematic literature review. Addictive behaviors reports, 15, 100419. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.abrep.2022.100419.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Evans, B. E., Stam, J., Huizink, A. C., Willemen, A. M., Westenberg, P. M., Branje, S., & van Lier, P. A. (2016). Neuroticism and extraversion in relation to physiological stress reactivity during adolescence. Biological Psychology, 117, 6779. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.03.002.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eysenck, H. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 244276). The Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fagot, B. I., Rodgers, C. S., & Leinbach, M. D. (2000). Theories of gender socialization. In T. Eckes, & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 6589). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410605245-10.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fam, J. Y. (2018). Prevalence of internet gaming disorder in adolescents: A meta‐analysis across three decades. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(5), 524531. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12459.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Feng, W., Ramo, D. E., Chan, S. R., & Bourgeois, J. A. (2017). Internet gaming disorder: Trends in prevalence 1998–2016. Addictive Behaviors, 75, 1724. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.06.010.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(12), 15731578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.005.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fernandes, B., Maia, B. R., & Pontes, H. M. (2019). Internet addiction or problematic internet use? Which term should be used? Psicologia USP, 30, e190020. https://doi.org/10.1590/0103-6564E190020.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Francis, L. J. (1996). The development of an abbreviated form of the revised Junior Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (JEPQR-A) among 13–15 year olds. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 835844. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00159-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gao, X., Zhang, M., Yang, Z., Wen, M., Huang, H., Zheng, R., & Zhang, Y. (2021). Structural and functional brain abnormalities in internet gaming disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 679437. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.679437.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological video–game use among youth ages 8 to 18: A national-study. Psychological Science, 20(5), 594602. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02340.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gervasi, A. M., La Marca, L., Costanzo, A., Pace, U., Guglielmucci, F., & Schimmenti, A. (2017). Personality and internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of recent literature. Current Addiction Reports, 4, 293307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-017-0159-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Giardina, A., Schimmenti, A., Starcevic, V., King, D. L., Di Blasi, M., & Billieux, J. (2024). Problematic gaming, social withdrawal, and escapism: The compensatory-dissociative online gaming (C-DOG) model. Computers in Human Behavior, 155, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2024.108187.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gomez, R., Holmberg, K., Bounds, J., Fullarton, C., & Gomez, A. (1999). Neuroticism and extraversion as predictors of coping styles during early adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(1), 317. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00224-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. P. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hanley, A. W., de Vibe, M., Solhaug, I., Gonzalez-Pons, K., & Garland, E. L. (2019). Mindfulness training reduces neuroticism over a 6-year longitudinal randomized control trial in Norwegian medical and psychology students. Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103859. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2019.103859.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hygen, B. W., Belsky, J., Stenseng, F., Skalicka, V., Kvande, M. N., Zahl‐Thanem, T., & Wichstrøm, L. (2020). Time spent gaming and social competence in children: Reciprocal effects across childhood. Child Development, 91(3), 861875. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13243.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kenny, D. A., & Zautra, A. (2001). Trait-state models for longitudinal data. In L. M. Collins, & A. G. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 243263). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, D., Nam, J. K., & Keum, C. (2022). Adolescent Internet gaming addiction and personality characteristics by game genre. Plos One, 17(2), e0263645. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263645.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kim, H. S., Son, G., Roh, E. B., Ahn, W. Y., Kim, J., Shin, S. H., & Choi, K. H. (2022). Prevalence of gaming disorder: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 126, 107183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107183.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2014). The cognitive psychology of Internet gaming disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(4), 298308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.006.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2018). Internet gaming disorder: Theory, assessment, treatment, and prevention. Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/C2016-0-04107-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klimstra, T. A., Akse, J., Hale III, W. W., Raaijmakers, Q. A., & Meeus, W. H. (2010). Longitudinal associations between personality traits and problem behavior symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(2), 273284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2010.02.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kristensen, J. H., Pallesen, S., King, D. L., Hysing, M., & Erevik, E. K. (2021). Problematic gaming and sleep: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 675237. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.675237.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, J., Lee, S., Chun, J. W., Cho, H., Kim, D. J., & Jung, Y. C. (2015). Compromised prefrontal cognitive control over emotional interference in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18(11), 661668. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0231.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liang, L., Zhou, D., Yuan, C., Shao, A., & Bian, Y. (2016). Gender differences in the relationship between internet addiction and depression: A cross-lagged study in Chinese adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 463470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.04.043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Liao, Z., Chen, X., Huang, Q., & Shen, H. (2022). Prevalence of gaming disorder in East Asia: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 11(3), 727738. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2022.00050.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lippa, R. A. (2010). Gender differences in personality and interests: When, where, and why? Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 10981110. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Little, R. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data. New Jersey: Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119013563.

  • Liu, Y., Wang, X., Zhai, J., Luo, F., & Xin, T. (2023). The effect of neuroticism on problematic smartphone use: A mediation model of self-control for males and females. Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace, 17, 1. https://doi.org/10.5817/CP2023-1-1.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Löckenhoff, C. E., Chan, W., McCrae, R. R., De Fruyt, F., Jussim, L., De Bolle, M., & Terracciano, A. (2014). Gender stereotypes of personality: Universal and accurate? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(5), 675694. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022113520075.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Long, J., Liu, T., Liu, Y., Hao, W., Maurage, P., & Billieux, J. (2018). Prevalence and correlates of problematic online gaming: A systematic review of the evidence published in Chinese. Current Addiction Reports, 5, 359371. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-018-0219-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lopez-Fernandez, O., Williams, A. J., Griffiths, M. D., & Kuss, D. J. (2019). Female gaming, gaming addiction, and the role of women within gaming culture: A narrative literature review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 449322. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00454.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lu, L., & Hu, C. H. (2005). Personality, leisure experiences and happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies: An Interdisciplinary Forum on Subjective Well-Being, 6(3), 325342. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-005-8628-3.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Macur, M., & Pontes, H. M. (2021). Internet gaming disorder in adolescence: Investigating profiles and associated risk factors. BMC Public Health, 21, 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-11394-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mahalik, J. R., Burns, S. M., & Syzdek, M. (2007). Masculinity and perceived normative health behaviors as predictors of men's health behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 64(11), 22012209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.035.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mahalik, J. R., Morray, E. B., Coonerty-Femiano, A., Ludlow, L. H., Slattery, S. M., & Smiler, A. (2005). Development of the conformity to feminine norms inventory. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 52(7–8), 417435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-3709-7.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Männikkö, N., Ruotsalainen, H., Miettunen, J., Pontes, H. M., & Kääriäinen, M. (2020). Problematic gaming behaviour and health-related outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 25(1), 6781. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105317740414.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 11(3), 320341. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328007sem1103_2.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 577605. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McArdle, J. J., & Hamagami, F. (2001). Latent difference score structural models for linear dynamic analyses with incomplete longitudinal data. In L. Collins, & A. Sayer (Eds.), New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 137175). American Psychological Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meiklejohn, J., Phillips, C., Freedman, M. L., Griffin, M. L., Biegel, G., Roach, A., & Saltzman, A. (2012). Integrating mindfulness training into K-12 education: Fostering the resilience of teachers and students. Mindfulness, 3, 291307. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0094-5.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morken, I. S., Wichstrøm, L., Steinsbekk, S., & Viddal, K. R. (2024). Depression and personality traits across adolescence—within-person analyses of a birth cohort. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-024-01188-8.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2021). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(4), 638648. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nawaz, M. W., Nadeem, T., Rao, S. L., Fatima, T., & Shoaib, S. (2020). Impact of PUBG game addiction on social isolation and narcissistic tendencies among gamers. Asian Journal of Social Sciences and Management Studies, 7(3), 166172. https://doi.org/10.20448/journal.500.2020.73.166.172.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nesi, J., Rothenberg, W. A., Bettis, A. H., Massing-Schaffer, M., Fox, K. A., Telzer, E. H., & Prinstein, M. J. (2022). Emotional responses to social media experiences among adolescents: Longitudinal associations with depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 51(6), 907922. https://doi.org/10.1037/t87255-000.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nicholson, J. S., Deboeck, P. R., & Howard, W. (2017). Attrition in developmental psychology: A review of modern missing data reporting and practices. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41, 143153. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415618275.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nielsen, P., Favez, N., & Rigter, H. (2020). Parental and family factors associated with problematic gaming and problematic internet use in adolescents: A systematic literature review. Current Addiction Reports, 7, 365386. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-020-00320-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ojha, S. P., Shaikh, M., Gupta, M., Shinde, A., Waghmare, S., & Shukla, S. (2023, January). Study the effects of online gaming on different emotional behavior of the users. In International Conference on Research into design (pp. 445457). Singapore: Springer Nature Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-0293-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Orth, U., Clark, D. A., Donnellan, M. B., & Robins, R. W. (2021). Testing prospective effects in longitudinal research: Comparing seven competing cross-lagged models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 10131034. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000358.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pan, P. Y., & Yeh, C. B. (2018). Internet addiction among adolescents may predict self-harm/suicidal behavior: A prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 197, 262267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2018.01.046.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Ko, C. H., & O’Brien, C. P. (2015). Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17, 72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0610-0.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Phan, M. H., Jardina, J. R., Hoyle, S., & Chaparro, B. S. (2012, September). Examining the role of gender in video game usage, preference, and behavior. In Proceedings of the human factors and ergonomics society annual meeting (Vol. 56, No. 1, pp. 14961500). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: Sage Publications.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pornnoppadol, C., Ratta-apha, W., Chanpen, S., Wattananond, S., Dumrongrungruang, N., Thongchoi, K., & Vasupanrajit, A. (2020). A comparative study of psychosocial interventions for internet gaming disorder among adolescents aged 13–17 years. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 18, 932948. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-9995-4.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Przybylski, A. K., Deci, E. L., Rigby, C. S., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Competence-impeding electronic games and players’ aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(3), 441457. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034820.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Purwaningsih, E., & Nurmala, I. (2021). The impact of online game addiction on adolescent mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Open Access Macedonian Journal of Medical Sciences (OAMJMS), 9(F), 260274. https://doi.org/10.3889/oamjms.2021.6234.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raiha, S., Yang, G., Wang, L., Dai, W., Wu, H., Meng, G., … Liu, X. (2020). Altered reward processing system in internet gaming disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, Article 599141. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.599141.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rosendo-Rios, V., Trott, S., & Shukla, P. (2022). Systematic literature review online gaming addiction among children and young adults: A framework and research agenda. Addictive Behaviors, 129, 107238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2022.107238.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Şalvarlı, Ş. İ., & Griffiths, M. D. (2021). Internet gaming disorder and its associated personality traits: A systematic review using PRISMA guidelines. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 19, 14201442. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00081-6.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2010). Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika, 75, 243. https://doi:10.1007/s11336-009-9135-y.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Savitz, D. A., & Wellenius, G. A. (2023). Can cross-sectional studies contribute to causal inference? It depends. American Journal of Epidemiology, 192(4), 514516. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwac037.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schrock, D., & Schwalbe, M. (2009). Men, masculinity, and manhood acts. Annual Review of Sociology, 35, 277295. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115933.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sharma, M. K., Bhargav, H., Kumar, A., Digambhar, V., & Mani, T. A. (2021). Mindfulness-based interventions: Potentials for management of internet gaming disorder. International Journal of Yoga, 14(3), 244247. https://doi.org/10.4103/ijoy.ijoy_46_21.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sims, C. M. (2017). Do the big-five personality traits predict empathic listening and assertive communication? International Journal of Listening, 31(3), 163188. https://doi.org/10.1080/10904018.2016.1202770.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Slobodskaya, H. R., & Kornienko, O. S. (2021). Age and gender differences in personality traits from early childhood through adolescence. Journal of Personality, 89(5), 933950. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12624.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Snodgrass, J. G., Lacy, M. G., Dengah II, H. F., Eisenhauer, S., Batchelder, G., & Cookson, R. J. (2014). A vacation from your mind: Problematic online gaming is a stress response. Computers in Human Behavior, 38, 248260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.06.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stevens, M. W., Dorstyn, D., Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2021). Global prevalence of gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 55(6), 553568. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867420962851.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Su, W., Han, X., Yu, H., Wu, Y., & Potenza, M. N. (2020). Do men become addicted to internet gaming and women to social media? A meta-analysis examining gender-related differences in specific internet addiction. Computers in Human Behavior, 113, 106480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106480.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swingle, M. K. (2016). i-Minds: How cell phones, computers, gaming, and social media are changing our brains, our behavior, and the evolution of our species. Gabriola Island, BC Canada: New Society Publishers.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Syed, M., & Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2013). Personality development from adolescence to emerging adulthood: Linking trajectories of ego development to the family context and identity formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 371384. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030070.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thulin, E., Vilhelmson, B., & Schwanen, T. (2020). Absent friends? Smartphones, mediated presence, and the recoupling of online social contact in everyday life. Annals of the American Association of Geographers, 110(1), 166183. https://doi.org/10.1080/24694452.2019.1629868.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tian, L., Jiang, S., & Huebner, E. S. (2019). The big two personality traits and adolescents’ complete mental health: The mediation role of perceived school stress. School Psychology, 34, 3242. https://doi.org/10.1037/spq0000257.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tseng, M. C. (2024). Fitting cross-lagged panel models with the residual structural equations approach. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 31(5), 923931. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2023.2296862.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tullett-Prado, D., Stavropoulos, V., Mueller, K., Sharples, J., & Footitt, T. A. (2021). Internet gaming disorder profiles and their associations with social engagement behaviours. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 138, 393403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.04.037.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uz, C., & Cagiltay, K. (2015). Social interactions and games. Digital Education Review, 27, 112. https://doi.org/10.1344/der.2015.27.1-12.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Villani, D., Carissoli, C., Triberti, S., Marchetti, A., Gilli, G., & Riva, G. (2018). Videogames for emotion regulation: A systematic review. Games for Health Journal, 7(2), 8599. https://doi.org/10.1089/g4h.2017.0108.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Walther, B., Hanewinkel, R., & Morgenstern, M. (2014). Effects of a brief school-based media literacy intervention on digital media use in adolescents: Cluster randomized controlled trial. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(9), 616623. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0173.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, Z. L., Song, K. R., Zhou, N., Potenza, M. N., Zhang, J. T., & Dong, G. H. (2022). Gender-related differences in involvement of addiction brain networks in internet gaming disorder: Relationships with craving and emotional regulation. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 118, 110574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2022.110574.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, C. Y., Wu, Y. C., Su, C. H., Lin, P. C., Ko, C. H., & Yen, J. Y. (2017). Association between Internet gaming disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 564571. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.6.2017.088.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weinstein, A., Livny, A., & Weizman, A. (2017). New developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 75, 314330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.040.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Weisberg, Y. J., DeYoung, C. G., & Hirsh, J. B. (2011). Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the Big Five. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 178. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00178.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • William, T., Ling, N. Z., & Woon, L. H. (2016). Extraversion and agreeableness: Divergent routes to daily satisfaction with social relationships. Journal of Personality, 84(1), 121134. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12146.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization (WHO) (2019). ICD-11 mortality andmorbidity statistics. Mental, behavioural or neurodevelopmental disorders. Available at: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhao, H., Li, X., Zhou, J., Nie, Q., & Zhou, J. (2020). The relationship between bullying victimization and online game addiction among Chinese early adolescents: The potential role of meaning in life and gender differences. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, 105261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105261.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zhou, J., Zhang, L., & Gong, X. (2023). Longitudinal network relations between symptoms of problematic internet game use and internalizing and externalizing problems among Chinese early adolescents. Social Science & Medicine, 333, 116162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2023.116162.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zyphur, M. J., Allison, P. D., Tay, L., Voelkle, M. C., Preacher, K. J., Zhang, Z., … Diener, E. (2020). From data to causes I: Building a general cross-lagged panel model (GCLM). Organizational Research Methods, 23(4), 651687. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428119847278.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article
Effective from  1st Feb 2025:
1400 EUR/article
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): 

Csilla ÁGOSTON

Dana KATZ

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)
  • Ruth J. VAN HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)
  • Ágnes ZSILA (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Oct 2024 0 0 0
Nov 2024 0 0 0
Dec 2024 0 0 0
Jan 2025 0 0 0
Feb 2025 0 6990 194
Mar 2025 0 1376 83
Apr 2025 0 0 0