Author:
D. Liebenberg Cape Peninsula University of Technology

Search for other papers by D. Liebenberg in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Using Robin Dunbar’s theories about hominin social bonding and the evolution of music, dance and religion, this article argues that a proto-ritualistic collective song and dance performance established conditions that were ideal for the evolution of fundamental elements of human cognition, such as shared intentionality, joint attention, and the ability to form a mental template. Furthermore, it is argued that this dance routine would have been an effective breeding ground for the development of a psychological regulatory system that could powerfully enforce cooperation by collectively targeting individuals who do not or cannot conform to the melody or the beat – i.e. misfits who do not achieve synchrony with the other members of the dance group. Thus a hominin society consisting of highly cooperative individuals possessing the cognitive skills of shared intentionality, joint attention, and the ability to form mental templates – i.e. a society with all the necessary skills for the development of a mimetic form of communication, followed much later by spoken language – comes into being.

  • Aiello, L., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Neocortex size, group size and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology, 34, 184193.

  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, New Series, 211 (4489 ), 13901396.

  • Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. (2003). The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100 (6 ), 35313535.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 10031011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cross, I. (1999). Is music the most important thing we ever did? Music, development and evolution. In: Yi S (Ed.), Music, mind and science. Seoul: Seoul National University Press. 1039.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cross, I. (2001). Music, cognition, culture, and evolution. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 930, 2842.

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Donald, M. (1998). Mimesis and the executive suite: Missing links in language evolution. In: J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 4467.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortex size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681735.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber & Faber.

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2004). The human story. A new history of mankind’s evolution. London: Faber & Faber.

  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). Mind the bonding gap: Constraints on the evolution of hominin societies. In: S. Shennan (Ed.), Pattern and process in cultural evolution. Berkeley: University of California Press, 223234.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dunbar, R. I. M., Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I., & Barra, V. (2012). Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: Implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolutionary Psychology. www.epjournal.net – 2012. 10(4): 688702.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425 (23 ), 785791.

  • Freeman, W. J. (2000). A neurobiological role of music in social bonding. In: N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 411424.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 206, 169179.

  • Gintis, H., Henrich, J., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2008). Strong reciprocity and the roots of human morality. Social Justice Research DOI 10.1007/s11211-008-0067-y

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Girard, R. (1977). Violence and the sacred, transl. P. Gregory. Baltimore and London: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

  • Hampson, N. (1968). The enlightenment. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

  • Heinemann, P. P. (1972). Mobbning-Gruppvåld bland barn och vuxna. Stockholm: Natur & Kultur.

  • Honour, H. (1968). Neo-classicism. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

  • Honour, H. (1981). Romanticism. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

  • Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn., 27, 949961. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4- year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav., 31, 354364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.-2010.04.004.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Knight, C. (2007). Language co-evolved with the rule of law. Mind & Society DOI 10.1007/s11299-007-0039-1.

  • Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events. Psychol. Rev., 106, 119159. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.119

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leymann, H. (1990). Mobbing and psychological terror at workplaces. Violence and Victims, 5 (2 ), 119-126.

  • Lewis-Williams, D. (2002a). A cosmos in stone: Interpreting religion and society through rock art. New York: Altamira Press.

  • Lewis-Williams, D. (2002b). The mind in the cave. London: Thames and Hudson.

  • McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping together in time. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Merker, B. (2000). Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In: The origins of music, N. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 315328.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2009). The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchrony and social perception. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol., 45, 585589. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.- 2009.02.002

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind. London: Thames & Hudson.

  • Mithen, S. (2005). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, language, mind and body. London: Orion.

  • Power, C. (1998). Old wives’ tales: The gossip hypothesis and the reliability of cheap signals. In: J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 111129.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reddish, P., Fischer, R. & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality, and cooperation. PLoS ONE 8 (8 ), e71182. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071182

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roederer, J. (1984). The search for a survival value of music. Music Perception, 1, 350356.

  • Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R. & Bettinger, R. L. (2005). Evolution on a restless planet: Were environmental variability and environmental change major drivers of human evolution? In: Wuketits, F.M., & Ayala, F.J. (Eds.), Handbook of evolution, vol. 2. Weinheim, Germany: John Wiley & Sons, 223242.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sterelny, K. (2012). Language, gesture, skill: The co-evolutionary foundations of language. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 367 (1599 ), 21412151.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675735.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.

  • Ulbaek, I. (1998). The origin of language and cognition. In: Approaches to the evolution of language, J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy, & C. Knight (Eds.), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 3043.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Westhues, K. (1998). Eliminating professors: a guide to the dismissal process. Lewiston, New York: Edwin Mellen Press.

  • Westhues, K. (2007). Mobbing: a natural fact. Workplace Mobbing in Academe. http://www.kwesthues.com/graz0701.htm

  • Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci., 20, 15. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

  • Collapse
  • Expand

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: David P. Schmitt

Editor(s): Lisa DeBruine

Editor(s): Clark Barrett

Review editor(s): Sandra Virgo

Editorial Board

  • Oliver Curry
  • David Coall
  • Robin Dunbar
  • Andrew Dunn
  • Jiaqing O
  • Michael Price
  • Michel Raymond
  • Martin Tovee

 

Advisory Board

  • Tamas Bereczkei
  • Csaba Pleh
  • Rob Kurzban
  • Brooke Scelza
  • Steven Pinker
  • Bruce Ellis
  • Mark Flinn
  • Michael Tomasello
  • Daniel Fessler
  • James Chisholm

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Google Scholar
  • PsycINFO
  • SCOPUS

 

2019  
Scimago
H-index
21
Scimago
Journal Rank
0,118
Scimago
Quartile Score
Anthropology Q4
Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics Q4
Experimental and Cognitive Psychology Q4
Social Psychology Q4
Scopus
Cite Score
5/9=0,6
Scopus
Cite Score Rank
Anthropology 212/398 (Q3)
Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics 553/629 (Q4)
Experimental and Cognitive Psychology 130/145 (Q4)
Scopus
SNIP
0,362
Scopus
Cites
9
Scopus
Documents
5
Acceptance
Rate
31%

 

Evolution, Mind and Behaviour
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 700 EUR/article for authors with grant funding
400 EUR/article for PhD students or authors with no grant funding
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Editorial Board / Advisory Board members: 50%
Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%
Subscription Information Gold Open Access
Purchase per Title  

Evolution, Mind and Behaviour
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2002
Publication
Programme
2021 Volume 19
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
1
Founder Akadémiai Kiadó
Founder's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN (Print)
ISSN 2560-0982 (Online)

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Nov 2024 0 98 230
Dec 2024 0 159 238
Jan 2025 0 69 281
Feb 2025 0 61 179
Mar 2025 0 119 257
Apr 2025 0 36 148
May 2025 0 0 0