Authors:
Avery Sapoznikow Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Okanagan, Kelowna, Canada

Search for other papers by Avery Sapoznikow in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Zachary Walsh Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Okanagan, Kelowna, Canada

Search for other papers by Zachary Walsh in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Kenneth W. Tupper School of Population and Public Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada

Search for other papers by Kenneth W. Tupper in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Earth Erowid Erowid Center, Grass Valley, CA, USA

Search for other papers by Earth Erowid in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Fire Erowid Erowid Center, Grass Valley, CA, USA

Search for other papers by Fire Erowid in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

Ayahuasca is a psychedelic decoction prepared from two (or more) plants containing monoamine oxidase inhibitors, N, N-dimethyltryptamine, and other chemicals. Ayahuasca has apparently been used for centuries in the Amazon basin of South America, and in recent years has increasingly been used internationally in diverse contexts. This study aims to elucidate differences between cross-cultural ceremonial and psychonautic contexts of ayahuasca use.

Methods

This study systematically examines subjective differences across contexts for contemporary ayahuasca-drinking practices. User reports of ayahuasca experiences were subjected to textual analysis to compare use in cross-culturalceremonial contexts that attempt to include elements of traditional Amazonian practices, with psychonautic use that does not formally integrate traditional ceremonial aspects. The experience reports were collected from an online database.

Results

The use of ayahuasca in a cross-cultural ceremonial context is associated with prominence of affective and motivational features, whereas psychonautic use was associated with an emphasis on cognitive processes.

Conclusions

The beneficial effects of ayahuasca may operate via affective processing and integration and as such, cross-cultural ceremonial use may have advantages relative to psychonautic use. Findings are considered in light of the importance of context on experiences with ayahuasca and other psychedelic substances.

Abstract

Background and aims

Ayahuasca is a psychedelic decoction prepared from two (or more) plants containing monoamine oxidase inhibitors, N, N-dimethyltryptamine, and other chemicals. Ayahuasca has apparently been used for centuries in the Amazon basin of South America, and in recent years has increasingly been used internationally in diverse contexts. This study aims to elucidate differences between cross-cultural ceremonial and psychonautic contexts of ayahuasca use.

Methods

This study systematically examines subjective differences across contexts for contemporary ayahuasca-drinking practices. User reports of ayahuasca experiences were subjected to textual analysis to compare use in cross-culturalceremonial contexts that attempt to include elements of traditional Amazonian practices, with psychonautic use that does not formally integrate traditional ceremonial aspects. The experience reports were collected from an online database.

Results

The use of ayahuasca in a cross-cultural ceremonial context is associated with prominence of affective and motivational features, whereas psychonautic use was associated with an emphasis on cognitive processes.

Conclusions

The beneficial effects of ayahuasca may operate via affective processing and integration and as such, cross-cultural ceremonial use may have advantages relative to psychonautic use. Findings are considered in light of the importance of context on experiences with ayahuasca and other psychedelic substances.

Introduction

Ayahuasca is a psychoactive decoction that produces cognitive shifts, emotional lability, and visionary experiences, and has a longstanding history of use in the Amazon basin region of South America (Grob et al., 1996; Rätsch, 2005). Traditional ayahuasca is made from the stalk of the vine Banisteriopsis caapi, a source of monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs), and typically the leaves of the Psychotria viridis shrub, a source of N,N-dimethyltryptamine (DMT). The sources and concentrations of MAOIs and DMT may vary based on phytogenetics, growing conditions, and tradition. Diverse botanical admixture plants may also be included in some ayahuasca brews. The combination of both an MAOI and DMT is required to produce the psychedelic effects, which result from MAOI inhibiting the metabolism of orally ingested DMT (McKenna, Towers, & Abbott, 1984; Riba et al., 2003). The psychoactive effects of DMT are comparable to those of other psychedelics that strongly affect the central nervous system (Hofmann, Ratsch, & Schultes, 1992; Pierce & Peroutka, 1989; Riba et al., 2003; Smith, Canton, Barrett, & Sanders-Bush, 1998; Waldmeier & Maître, 1977), and include profound changes in perception, affect, and cognition (Shanon, 2002) for the duration of the effects of the substance. The traditional indigenous Amazonian uses of ayahuasca involve an array of ceremonial practices (McKenna, 2006). In addition to these traditional indigenous modes of use, in the 20th century, new religious movements in Brazil incorporated ayahuasca as a sacrament into their rituals. Recent decades have witnessed the internationalization, or worldwide spread, of these kinds of ayahuasca-drinking practices, as well as novel kinds of use in contexts that diverge substantially from Amazonian traditions (Tupper, 2009).

Contemporary ayahuasca drinking can be broadly characterized by four categories: (a) Amazonian use that includes traditional or indigenous-style ceremonial elements and is directed by an expert leader (e.g., ayahuasquero, curandero, or shaman; Labate & Cavnar, 2014); (b) formalized use in predominantly South American religious organizations, such as the Santo Daime and União do Vegetal, with novel rituals for ayahuasca consumption developed since the mid-20th century (Coomber & South, 2004; Hofmann et al., 1992; Labate, Rose, & Dos Santos, 2009); (c) less-formalized ceremonial uses involving diverse practices that range from attempts to reproduce traditional Amazonian practices and constituents of admixtures, to modernized ceremonial practices that may incorporate popular music; (d) non-ceremonial use in which ayahuasca-like mixtures – sometimes referred to as pharmahuasca – are consumed individually for self-exploration (Tupper, 2009). The latter two categories, which we provisionally label as cross-cultural ceremonial and psychonautic respectively, are prominent modes of use outside of South America and are the focus of this study.

The internationalization of ayahuasca has been driven in part by the putative salutary effects of the brew, primarily for mental health and well-being. Indeed, a small but growing body of research suggests associations between ayahuasca use and reduced psychopathology and enhanced well-being (Barbosa, Giglio, & Dalgalarrondo, 2005; Grob et al., 1996; Palhano-Fontes et al., 2019; Sanches et al., 2016; Thomas, Lucas, Capler, Tupper, & Martin, 2013). However, research on the potential health effects of ayahuasca has focused almost exclusively on use of ayahuasca in ceremonial contexts related to formal ayahuasca religions. As such, the extent to which benefits may be expected to extend to the increasingly diverse contexts for contemporary ayahuasca drinking is unclear. In particular, the consequences of psychonautic use, which typically involve privately acquired plant materials for independent home preparation and ingestion, remain largely unexamined (Halpern & Pope, 2001; Ott, 1994).

Context of use, or set and setting, has been identified as an important determinant of the nature of user experiences with psychedelic substances (Zinberg, 1984), with set referring generally to individual attitudes, expectations, and mood going into the psychedelic experience, and setting pertaining to the environment in which the experience occurs. Context may be a particularly important influence in responses to psychedelics (Leary, Metzner, & Alpert, 1964; Studerus, Gamma, Kometer, & Vollenwieder, 2012; Talin & Sanabria, 2017), as reflected by the attention afforded to ritual steps and elements among longstanding cultural traditions of psychedelic plant use, exemplified by the incorporation of specific music and dance in the Huichol peyote ceremony and the singing of icaros in Shipibo ayahuasca ceremonies (Schultes, Hoffman, & Rätsch, 2001). Indeed, some of the discrepant findings of early psychedelic research have been proposed to reflect differences in set and setting, and the contemporary renaissance of interest in psychedelic therapies has led to a resurgence of attention to the influence of context on psychedelic experiences and their outcomes (Hartogsohn, 2016; Tupper, Wood, Yensen, & Johnson, 2015).

Edited databases of information, such as Erowid, as well as online fora and web-based data sets that are focused on the effects of psychoactive drugs, are increasingly used by researchers as a resource for examining the subjective effects of substances such as ayahuasca that have a relatively low prevalence of use (e.g., Hearne & Van Hout, 2016; Sanz & Tagliazucchi, 2018; Swogger et al., 2015). To the extent that these “experience reports” include information relevant to context, they allow for a cross-context comparison of the subjective effects of psychoactive drugs of interest. In this study, we use linguistic analysis of published texts to identify domains of experience related to reports of ayahuasca experiences and examine differences in the relative prevalence of these domains as a function of cross-cultural ceremonial versus psychonautic contexts for use.

Methods

Participants

The sample for this study comprised archival reports collected from Erowid.org (Erowid, 2017), a non-profit website dedicated to information on psychoactive substances, which publishes reports of user experiences known as “experience reports.” Erowid and Erowid (2006) selected and edited the submitted experience reports according to their guidelines for quality and publication. Several prior studies have drawn data from these reports (e.g., Spaderna, Addy, & D’Souza, 2013; Swogger et al., 2015). All published ayahuasca experience reports as of January 30, 2016 (n = 203) were downloaded from the website for consideration of analysis. All reports’ content was screened to exclude reports involving concurrent use of additional psychoactive substances such as cannabis (the exception was tobacco, which is commonly used concurrently with ayahuasca in traditional Amazonian ritual practices); reports that lack content relevant to an experience of drinking ayahuasca (e.g., an individual discussing events not directly related to the ayahuasca experience), recipes, or guidance on brewing ayahuasca; and reports of perspectives of individuals supervising another individual’s ayahuasca experience (i.e., not directly autobiographical reports). Based on these criteria, we removed 50 reports, leaving 153 reports matching inclusion criteria.

The reports were classified as “psychonautic” versus “cross-cultural ceremonial” based on several criteria: (a) location (personal residence vs. ceremonial space), (b) presence of expert guide, and (c) procedure of brew preparation and ingestion (i.e., self-made tea vs. a shamanically made and administered brew). Taking these criteria into account, the reports were read individually for eligibility in the study. Any reports of one experience split into multiple published reports were considered one report. Reports involving atypical circumstances, such as drinking ayahuasca during a music festival, were classified as psychonautic use. All reports contained sufficient detail to discern relevance to the three criteria and the most suitable category. Based on these criteria, 58 (38%) of the included reports described use in a cross-cultural ceremonial context and 95 (62%) described experiences in psychonautic contexts.

Analysis

Quantitative linguistic analysis of first-hand autobiographical texts is a data-reducing process that involves analysis of texts to identify the relative prominence of predefined categories of interest. We used the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software (LIWC; Pennebaker, Boyd, Jordan, & Blackburn, 2015; Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010), which outputs the proportions of psychologically meaningful information into 41 predetermined categories designed to capture temporal, emotional, and cognitive processes. For example, the phrase “I was seeing the colors shift and bend and twist in front of me” would increase the proportions of the domains Perception & Relativity along with the output domains for personal pronouns, verbs, etc. The phrase “It made me feel genuine sadness and fear; I thought I was lost in the dark spiral for eternity” would increase the proportions of the domains Affective Processes and Cognitive Processes. Based on previous research using LIWC, we constrained our consideration of categories to those that produced a mean output of 2% or greater (Beukeboom, Tanis, & Vermeulen, 2012; Carey et al., 2015; Cariola, 2014; Czechowski, Miranda, & Sylvestre, 2016; Hill, Ford, & Farreras, 2015; Isaac, Chertoff, Lee, & Carnes, 2011), indicating that at least 2% of the word content of the experience report loaded on the domain. Reports were edited manually based on the LIWC transcript protocol for non-fluencies, improper punctuation, and numerals. Furthermore, information pertaining to brew preparation or other elements not directly related to the psychedelic experience itself was removed to maintain consistency in analyzing content purely descriptive of the experiences. We conducted principal components analysis (PCA) of the domains identified using LIWC to explore the extent to which these categories could be reduced to a more parsimonious number of factors. The factor loading scores, reported in Table 1, are calculated using PCA and speak to the magnitude and direction of the relationship between the individual domains and the three different factors identified. Negative factor loading scores indicate a negative relationship between the individual domain and the factor. Potential scores range from −1 to 1, with 0 indicating a lack of a relationship. Factors were then compared across contexts using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).

Table 1.

Mean percentage of domain content and factor loading scores

Domain Mean % (SD) Factor 1 (Cognitive) Factor 2 (Motivational/Affective) Factor 3 (Perception)
Affective processes 4.39 (1.40) 0.339 0.699 0.374
Cognitive processes 11.99 (2.54) 0.889 −0.048 −0.127
Insight 3.34 (1.16) 0.783 −0.115 0.296
Perception 4.57 (1.19) 0.017 −0.113 0.538
Biological processes 3.35 (1.24) −0.128 0.142 0.746
Drives 5.26 (1.28) −0.107 0.851 0.019
Relativity 15.18 (2.31) −0.566 −0.095 0.318
Social processes 5.07 (2.48) −0.130 0.684 −0.281

Note. Bold values indicate highly loading domains on the extracted factors. This table represents the mean percentage of total word content that each domain held on our data set as well as the corresponding factor loading scores from the principal components analysis. “Mean %” column refers to the average percent of word content across all experiences reports from each domain. Factor loading scores indicate the magnitude and direction of the relationship (i.e., the larger the number, the greater the strength of the relationship and negative values indicate a negative relationship between the domain and factor).

Ethics

Due to the archival nature of this study, a formal ethics review was not required by our institution. Once users submit their articles to the Erowid Center for publication, the reports undergo Erowid Center’s editorial procedures (Erowid & Erowid, 2006) and are published in the Erowid Experience Vaults (Erowid, 2017) under the copyright of Erowid. Individuals who submit their experience reports forfeit all rights to their report. After being granted permission to analyze the ayahuasca experiences hosted on the website, we began our analysis procedures. The authors have acknowledged the work of all individuals who have contributed to this study and report no conflicts of interest.

Results

The LIWC analysis produced eight categories based on a 2% minimum cut-off (Table 1). PCA identified three factors that retained eigenvalues greater than 1 and that each accounted for more than 15% of the total variance in our data (Table 1). Thinking [consisting of cognitive processes and insight (words describing thought and introspection), accounting for 23.74% of total variance], Feeling [consisting Affective Processes, Drives, and Social Processes (words describing emotion, motivations, and social interaction), accounting for 21.52% of total variance], and Perceiving [consisting of perception, biological processes, and relativity (words describing sensory information, bodily functions, and movement), accounting for 15.81% of total variance] together accounted for a total of 61.1% of the variance. A MANOVA testing differences between psychonautic and cross-cultural ceremonial context on the extracted factors identified a significant multivariate main effect, Wilks’ λ = 0.83, F(3, 149) = 10.25, p < .001, η2 = 0.17, and follow-up t-tests (Figure 1) identified differences on the Thinking factor, F(1, 151) = 7.75, p < .01, η2 = 0.04, indicating higher scores in the psychonautic relative to cross-cultural ceremonial context (M = 0.17, SD = 1.04 vs. M = −0.28, SD = 0.86). The cross-cultural ceremonial context produced higher scores on the Motivation/Affective [F(1, 151) = 21.04, p < .001, η2 = 0.122; M = 0.45, SD = 0.89 vs. M = −0.27, SD = 0.97]. No differences were identified on scores on the Perceiving Factor [F(1, 151) = 0.02, p = .89, η2 = 0.00; M = −0.01, SD = 0.85 vs. M = 0.01, SD = 1.08].

Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Differences between cross-cultural and psychonautic experiences on extracted factors. Standard deviations: Cross cultural: 0.86 (Cognitive); 0.89 (Motivational/Affective); 0.85 (Perceiving). Psychonautic: 1.04 (Cognitive); 0.97 (Motivational/Affective); 1.08 (Perceiving). A bar graph represents the differences between cross-cultural and psychonautic ayahuasca use in terms of factor loading scores for each of the extracted factors. This graph details the extent to which each factor is associated with either type of ayahuasca use. Significant differences found for the Cognitive and Motivational/Affective factors

Citation: Journal of Psychedelic Studies 3, 3; 10.1556/2054.2019.028

Supplementary analyses examined differences on each of the eight LIWC domains, indicating a multivariate effect, Wilks’ λ = 0.763, F(8, 144) = 5.60, p < .001, η2 = 0.237. Follow-up ANOVA identified higher scores for cross-cultural ceremonial contexts for Affective Processes, Drives, and Social Processes, whereas psychonautic contexts produced higher scores for Cognitive Processes and Insight (Table 2).

Table 2.

Bivariate follow-up ANOVAs for the significant MANOVA of LIWC domains

Domain Setting Mean (SD) F value p value
Affective processes Psychonautic 4.14 (1.24) 8.41 .004
Cross-cultural 4.80 (1.55)
Cognitive processes Psychonautic 12.51 (2.72) 11.08 .001
Cross-cultural 11.15 (1.95)
Insight Psychonautic 3.55 (1.26) 8.29 .005
Cross-cultural 3.01 (0.87)
Perception Psychonautic 4.50 (1.18) 0.90 .344
Cross-cultural 4.69 (1.22)
Biological processes Psychonautic 3.37 (1.30) 0.03 .866
Cross-cultural 3.33 (1.14)
Drives Psychonautic 5.03 (1.32) 8.48 .004
Cross-cultural 5.63 (1.12)
Relativity Psychonautic 15.08 (2.34) 0.48 .490
Cross-cultural 15.35 (2.27)
Social processes Psychonautic 4.40 (2.34) 20.57 <.001
Cross-cultural 6.17 (2.33)

Note. Bold values represent significant results for Affective, Cognitive, and Social Processes, Insight, and Drives. SD: standard deviation; ANOVA: analysis of variance; MANOVA: multivariate analysis of variance; LIWC: Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count.

Discussion

We identified differences between reports of psychonautic and cross-cultural ceremonial contexts for ayahuasca drinking such that descriptions of cross-cultural ceremonial uses were characterized by a higher proportion of affective (mood-related) and motivational content as well as a lower proportion of cognitive processes. Broadly stated, cross-cultural ceremonial contexts appear to be associated with relatively more feeling and less thinking. These findings are based on the assumptions that through linguistic analysis, themes associated with affective and cognitive processes are directly related to the set, and perceptual themes are related to setting. Although both set and setting are correlated, the distinction between cognition and emotion versus perception allows for a comparison between the two aspects of set to be made.

These findings have the potential to inform future research and practice related to the therapeutic uses of ayahuasca. Specifically, to the extent that the salutary effects of ayahuasca, and psychedelic psychotherapy more broadly, operate via the facilitation of affective processing and integration (Loizaga-Velder, 2014; Mithoefer, Grob, & Brewerton, 2016), these conclusions suggest that, relative to a psychonautic context, the cross-cultural ceremonial context may have salutary advantages. Indeed, ceremonial use of ayahuasca has been associated with reductions in symptoms of depression, anxiety, and trauma (Dos Santos et al., 2016; Palhano-Fontes et al., 2019; Sanches et al., 2016; Talin & Sanabria, 2017); each of which has been proposed to include dysregulation of emotions and motivation in their respective etiologies (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The preponderance of affective content may be particularly salient in the case of trauma-related psychopathologies, such as post-traumatic stress disorder, in which the reprocessing of affectively laden material has been proposed to play a central therapeutic role (Monson et al., 2006; Resick, Nishith, Weaver, Millie, & Feurer, 2010). Future research may confirm the therapeutic advantages of the cross-cultural ceremonial context, as it borrows elements from long standing therapeutic technologies; ayahuasca has traditionally been used in the Amazon basin for the treatment of psychological (or psychospiritual) maladies, and practitioners of ayahuasca-based therapies have developed effective and sophisticated approaches to maximizing the therapeutic impact of ayahuasca.

On the contrary, our findings tentatively suggest that psychonautic use of ayahuasca may not confer the same emotional benefits that are associated with cross-cultural ceremonial ayahuasca drinking, which tends to involve formalized drinking practices and guidance, and as such, may allow for more emotionally focused experiences through this guidance. Given the expansion of psychedelic drug use for diverse purposes (Fadiman, 2011), and the relative dearth of guidance regarding optimization of psychedelic use for specified ends, preliminary findings such as these may be informative. In addition, it is important to note that it is the conflict between our results and recent literature suggesting that ayahuasca modulates cognitive dimensions through mindfulness and increases in divergent versus convergent thinking (Kuypers et al., 2016; Sampedro et al., 2017; Soler et al., 2016, 2018; Uthang et al., 2018). This conflict suggests that a dynamic interaction occurs involving both cognitive and affective processes, which in turn impacts the user.

Our conclusions are limited by the nature of the available textual material we analyzed. Specifically, the reliability and validity of the reports are unknown, and it is possible that they may include experiences that are not directly the result of ayahuasca, and may be flawed or skewed by poor recollection of the experiences by the writers. Furthermore, due to reliance on anonymous authors, there is no way to establish the accuracy of the representation of the ayahuasca experiences. When considering intentions and/or goals for ayahuasca use versus those in psychotherapy, the goals one may strive for are far less defined when taking ayahuasca than a formal treatment setting. Therefore, potential positive or negative impact on mental state is more difficult to prove. In addition, the motivations of the individuals who submitted the analyzed experience reports vary and therefore some reports may be less impactful than described. This study considers less-formalized ceremonial use and non-ceremonial use for analysis due to the lack of indigenous and religious use reported in the collected experience reports. This limits the ability to draw conclusions regarding formal ceremonial ayahuasca use in indigenous and religious contexts. This study is limited by the manual editing of all reports. All reports were edited by the same individual to match the analysis software’s textual requirements (e.g., sentences containing numerical symbols such as, “I drank the ayahuasca 4 hours ago” would be edited to “I drank the ayahuasca four hours ago”) and information that was irrelevant to the analysis of the experience itself, such as recipes, prologues, and epilogues to experiences, etc., was removed. The intention of this practice was to isolate the ayahuasca experience itself from the entire report to allow for an analysis of only the experiences; however, there is a possibility that some relevant information was removed. Furthermore, the selection/editing criteria applied by the editors of Erowid significantly differ from our editing process. Therefore, some of the excluded reports may have influenced our results. It should also be noted that the results and interpretations of this study may be negatively skewed because all writers live in North America. We have many different cultural and ethnic differences, which influence our thinking with regard to ayahuasca, which likely differ to that of the traditional indigenous peoples of the Amazon. These limitations are shared with other research that has drawn from experience reports, and it is our belief that despite the clear limitations of these data, they nonetheless contain information that is potentially valuable, particularly in areas where empirical research is currently scant and subject to substantial barriers.

Beyond the limitations, inherent on reliance on experience reports are factors that covary with context and thus complicate inferences related to locus of effects. At the level of the brew itself, it is possible that the ayahuasca consumed in the cross-cultural ceremonial context may differ in content and production style, and as such the observed differences may be attributable to differences in the ayahuasca decoctions ingested rather than to the context per se. Differences may also be attributable to the set that is influenced by the chosen context in which the ayahuasca will be used. In addition, demographic factors such as age and socioeconomic status may also be pertinent with regard to the chosen setting for users; ceremonial contexts may be more likely to involve a financial commitment and thus may limit the ability of younger or less wealthy individuals to partake in traditional ayahuasca ceremonies. Or there might be other underlying differences in personality, diction, or style of experience reporting between psychonauts who decide to make and/or use homemade, self-administered ayahuasca, and those who use ayahuasca in cross-cultural ceremonial contexts. Moreover, the extent to which apparent contextual differences reflect set, setting, or both elements is difficult to parse as differences in setting may serve as a marker for differences in set; the intention and attitudes of respondents who sought cross-cultural ceremonial settings may have differed in meaningful ways from the set of those who drank ayahuasca in psychonautic settings. The design of this study does not allow for the careful parsing of the extent to which differences in the content of ayahuasca experiences reflect the pharmacology of the brew, the set of participants, and the setting of the experiences, and these factors and others likely interact in complex ways. Interestingly, recent studies that have administered ayahuasca in a clinical setting have reported anti-depressant effects (Palhano-Fontes et al., 2019; Sanches et al., 2016), which suggest potential for salutary mental health effects outside of a ceremonial context. Future clinical research is required to more definitively address the many interesting and consequential gaps in our understanding of psychedelic medicines. Pending such investigations, and with the above limitations in mind, this study may help to provide future researchers with a foundation for investigating the influence of context on the effects of ayahuasca and psychedelic medicines more generally

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Matthew Baggott, PhD, for contributing in editing of the manuscript prior to submission.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barbosa, P. C. , Giglio, J. S., & Dalgalarrondo, P. (2005). Altered states of consciousness and short-term psychological after-effects induced by the first time ritual use of ayahuasca in an urban context in Brazil. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 37(2), 193201. doi:10.1080/02791072.2005.10399801

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beukeboom, C. J. , Tanis, M., & Vermeulen, I. E. (2012). The language of extraversion: Extraverted people talk more abstractly, introverts are more concrete. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32(2), 191201. doi:10.1177/0261927X12460844

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carey, A. L. , Brucks, M. S. , Küfner, A. C. , Holtzman, N. S. , Große Deters, F. , Back, M. D. , Donnellan, M. B. , Pennebaker, J. W., & Mehl, M. R. (2015). Narcissism and the use of personal pronouns revisted. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 109(3), e1e15. doi:10.1037/pspp0000029

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cariola, L. A. (2014). Lexical tendencies of high and low barrier personalities in narratives of everyday and dream memories. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 34(2), 133161. doi:10.2190/IC.34.2.d

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coomber, R., & South, N. (Eds.). (2004). Drug use and cultural contexts ‘Beyond the West’: Tradition, change and post-colonialism. London, UK: Free Association Books.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Czechowski, K. , Miranda, D., & Sylvestre, J. (2016). Like a rolling stone: A mixed-methods approach to linguistic analysis of Bob Dylan’s lyrics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(1), 99113. doi:10.1037/aca0000045

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dos Santos, R. G. , Osório, F. L. , Crippa, J. A. S. , Riba, J. , Zuardi, A. W., & Hallak, J. E. C. (2016). Antidepressive, anxiolytic, and antiaddictive effects of ayahuasca, psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD): A systematic review of clinical trials published in the last 25 years. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 193213. doi:10.1177/2045125316638008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Erowid, E. (2017). Erowid experience vaults. Retrieved from http://www.erowid.org/exp

  • Erowid, E., & Erowid, F. (2006). The value of experience. Erowid Extracts, 10, 1419. Retrieved from https://erowid.org/experiences/exp_info3.shtml

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fadiman, J. (2011). The psychedelic explorer’s guide: Safe, therapeutic, and sacred journeys. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

  • Grob, C. S. , McKenna, D. J. , Callaway, J. C. , Brito, G. S. , Neves, E. S. , Oberlaender, G. , Saide, O. L. , Labigalini, E. , Tacla, C. , Miranda, C. T. , Strassman, R. J., & Boone, K. B. (1996). Human psychopharmacology of hoasca, a plant hallucinogen used in ritual context in Brazil. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184(2), 8694. doi:10.1097/00005053-199602000-00004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Halpern, J. H., & Pope, H. G. (2001). Hallucinogens on the Internet: A vast new source of underground drug information. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 481483. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.481

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hartogsohn, I. (2016). Set and setting, psychedelics and the placebo response: An extra-pharmacological perspective on psychophamacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12591267. doi:10.1177/0269881116677852

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hearne, E., & Van Hout, M. C. (2016). “Trip-Sitting” in the black hole: A netnographic study of dissociation and indigenous harm reduction. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 48(4), 233242. doi:10.1080/02791072.2016.1207827

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hill, J. , Ford, W. R., & Farreras, I. G. (2015). Real conversations with artificial intelligence: A comparison between human-human online conversations and human-chatbot conversations. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 245250. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.026

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hofmann, A., Ratsch, C., & Schultes, R. (1992). Plants of the Gods: Their Sacred, Healing, and Hallucinogenic Powers. Rochester, NY: Healing Arts Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Isaac, C. , Chertoff, J. , Lee, B., & Carnes, M. (2011). Do students’ and authors’ genders affect evaluations? A linguistic analysis of medical student performance evaluations. Academic Medicine, 86(1), 5966. doi:10.1097/ACM.0b013e318200561d

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuypers, K. P. , Riba, J. , de la Fuente Revenga, M. , Barker, S. , Theunissen, E. L., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2016). Ayahuasca enhances creative divergent thinking while decreasing conventional convergent thinking. Psychopharmacology, 233(18), 33953403. doi:10.1007/s00213-016-4377-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Labate, B. C., & Cavnar, C. (Eds.). (2014). The therapeutic use of Ayahuasca. Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.

  • Labate, B. C. , Rose, I. S., & Dos Santos, R. G. (2009). Ayahuasca religions: A comprehensive bibliography and critical essays. Santa Cruz, CA: Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, T. , Metzner, R., & Alpert, R. (1964). The psychedelic experience: A manual based on the Tibetan book of the dead. New Hyde Park, NY: University Books.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. (2014). Therapeutic effects of ritual ayahuasca use in the treatment of substance dependence – Qualitative results. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 46(1), 6372. doi:10.1080/02791072.2013.873157

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. (2006). Ayahuasca an ethnopharmacologic history. In R. Metzner (Eds.), Sacred vine of spirits: Ayahuasca (pp. 4062). Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. , Towers, G. H. N., & Abbott, F. S. (1984). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors in South American hallucinogenic plants: Tryptamine and β-carboline constituents of ayahuasca. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 10(2), 195223. doi:10.1016/0378-8741(84)90003-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mithoefer, M. C. , Grob, C. S., & Brewerton, T. D. (2016). Novel psychopharmacological therapies for psychiatric disorders: Psilocybin and MDMA. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(5), 481488. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00576-3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Monson, C. M. , Schnurr, P. P. , Resick, P. A. , Friedman, M. J. , Young-Xu, Y., & Stevens, S. P. (2006). Cognitive processing therapy for veterans with military-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(5), 898907. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.898

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ott, J. (1994). Ayahuasca analogues: Pangæan entheogens. Kennewick, WA: Natural Products Company.

  • Palhano-Fontes, F. , Barreto, D. , Onias, H. , Andrade, K. C. , Novaes, M. M. , Pessla, J. A. , Mota-Rolim, S. A. , Osório, F. L. , Sanches, R. , Dos Santos, R. G. , Tófoli, L. F. , de Oliveira Silveira, G. , Yonamine, M. , Riba, J. , Santos, F. R. , Silva-Junior, A. A. , Alchieri, J. C. , Galvão-Coelho, N. L. , Lobão-Soares, B. , Hallak, J. E. C. , Arcoverde, E. , Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., & Araújo, D. B. (2019). Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 655663. doi:10.1017/S0033291718001356

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pennebaker, J. W. , Boyd, R. L. , Jordan, K., & Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pierce, P. A., & Peroutka, S. J. (1989). Hallucinogenic drug interactions with neurotransmitter receptor binding sites in human cortex. Psychopharmacology, 97(1), 118122. doi:10.1007/BF00443425

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rätsch, C. (2005). The Encyclopedia of psychoactive plants: Ethnopharmacology and its applications. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

  • Resick, P. A. , Nishith, P. , Weaver, T. L. , Millie, C. A., & Feurer, C. A. (2010). A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(4), 867879. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.4.867

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J. , Valle, M. , Urbano, G. , Yritia, M. , Morte, A., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2003). Human pharmacology of ayahuasca: Subjective and cardiovascular effects, monoamine metabolite excretion, and pharmacokinetics. Journal of Pharmacology, 306, 7383. doi:10.1124/jpet.103.049882

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sampedro, F. , de la Fuente Revenga, M. , Valle, M. , Roberto, N. , Domínguez-Clavé, E. , Elices, M. , Luna, L. E. , Crippa, J. A. S. , Hallak, J. E. C. , de Araujo, D. B. , Friedlander, P. , Barker, S. A. , Álvarez, E. , Soler, J. , Pascual, J. C. , Feilding, A., & Riba, J. (2017). Assessing the psychedelic “After-Glow” in ayahuasca users: Post-acute neurometabolic and functional connectivity changes are associated with enhanced mindfulness capacities. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(9), 698711. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyx036

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanches, R. F. , Osório, F. L. , dos Santos, R. G. , Macedo, L. R. H. , Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P. , Wichert-Ana, L. , de Araujo, D. B. , Riba, J. , Crippa, J. A. S., & Hallak, J. E. (2016). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A SPECT study. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 36(1), 7781. doi:10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanz, C., & Tagliazucchi, E. (2018). The experience elicited by hallucinogens presents the highest similarity to dreaming within a large database of psychoactive substances reports. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 7. doi:10.3389/fnins.2018.00007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schultes, R. E. , Hofmann, A., & Rätsch, C. (2001). Plant of the gods: Their sacred, healing, and hallucinogenic powers. Rochester, VT: Healing Arts Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shanon, B. (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the ayahuasca experience. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smith, R. , Canton, H. , Barrett, R. J., & Sanders-Bush, E. (1998). Agonist properties of N, N-dimethyltryptamine at serotonin 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 61(3), 323330. doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00110-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soler, J. , Elices, M. , Domínguez-Clavé, E , Pascual, J. C. , Feilding, A. , Navarro-Gil, M. , García-Campayo, J., & Riba, J. (2018). Four weekly ayahuasca sessions lead to increases in “Acceptance” capacities: A comparison study with a standard 8-week mindfulness training program. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 224. doi:10.3389/fphar.2018.00224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soler, J. , Elices, M. , Franguesa, A. , Barker, S. , Friedlander, P. , Feilding, A. , Pascual, J. C., & Riba, J. (2016). Exploring the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: Acute intake increases mindfulness-related capacities. Psychopharmacology, 233(5), 823829. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4162-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spaderna, M. , Addy, P. H., & D’souza, D. C. (2013). Spicing things up: Synthetic cannabinoids. Psychopharmacology, 228(4), 525540. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3188-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Studerus, E. , Gamma, A. , Kometer, M., & Vollenwieder, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PLoS One, 7(2), e30800. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030800

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swogger, M. T. , Hart, E. , Erowid, F. , Erowid, E. , Trabold, N. , Yee, K. , Parkhurst, K. A. , Priddy, B. M., & Walsh, Z. (2015). Experience of Kratom users: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47(5), 360367. doi:10.1080/02791072.2015.1096434

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Talin, P., & Sanabria, E. (2017). Ayahuasca’s entwined efficacy: An ethnographic study of ritual healing from ‘addiction’. The International Journal of Drug Policy, 44, 2330. doi:10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.02.017

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words; LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 2454. doi:10.1177/0261927X09351676

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thomas, G. , Lucas, P. , Capler, N. R. , Tupper, K. W., & Martin, G. (2013). Ayahuasca-assisted therapy for addiction: Results from a preliminary observational study in Canada. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 6(1), 3042. doi:10.2174/15733998113099990003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. (2009). Ayahuasca healing beyond the Amazon: The globalization of a traditional indigenous entheogenic practice. Global Networks: A Journal of Transnational Affairs, 9(1), 117136. doi:10.1111/j.1471-0374.2009.00245.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. , Wood, E. , Yensen, R., & Johnson, M. W. (2015). Psychedelic medicine: A re-emerging therapy. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 187(14), 10541059. doi:10.1503/cmaj.141124

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uthang, M. V. , van Oorsouw, K. , Kuypers, K. P. C. , van Boxtel, M. , Broers, N. J. , Mason, N. L. , Toennes, S. W. , Riba, J., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2018). Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology, 235, 29792989. doi:10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Waldmeier, P. C., & Maître, L. (1977). Neurochemical investigations of the interaction of N, N-dimethyltryptamine with the dopaminergic system in rat brain. Psychopharmacology, 52(2), 137144. doi:10.1007/BF00439100

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zinberg, N. E. (1984). Drug, set, and setting: The basis for controlled intoxicant use. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Barbosa, P. C. , Giglio, J. S., & Dalgalarrondo, P. (2005). Altered states of consciousness and short-term psychological after-effects induced by the first time ritual use of ayahuasca in an urban context in Brazil. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 37(2), 193201. doi:10.1080/02791072.2005.10399801

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Beukeboom, C. J. , Tanis, M., & Vermeulen, I. E. (2012). The language of extraversion: Extraverted people talk more abstractly, introverts are more concrete. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32(2), 191201. doi:10.1177/0261927X12460844

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carey, A. L. , Brucks, M. S. , Küfner, A. C. , Holtzman, N. S. , Große Deters, F. , Back, M. D. , Donnellan, M. B. , Pennebaker, J. W., & Mehl, M. R. (2015). Narcissism and the use of personal pronouns revisted. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 109(3), e1e15. doi:10.1037/pspp0000029

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cariola, L. A. (2014). Lexical tendencies of high and low barrier personalities in narratives of everyday and dream memories. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 34(2), 133161. doi:10.2190/IC.34.2.d

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coomber, R., & South, N. (Eds.). (2004). Drug use and cultural contexts ‘Beyond the West’: Tradition, change and post-colonialism. London, UK: Free Association Books.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Czechowski, K. , Miranda, D., & Sylvestre, J. (2016). Like a rolling stone: A mixed-methods approach to linguistic analysis of Bob Dylan’s lyrics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(1), 99113. doi:10.1037/aca0000045

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dos Santos, R. G. , Osório, F. L. , Crippa, J. A. S. , Riba, J. , Zuardi, A. W., & Hallak, J. E. C. (2016). Antidepressive, anxiolytic, and antiaddictive effects of ayahuasca, psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD): A systematic review of clinical trials published in the last 25 years. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 6(3), 193213. doi:10.1177/2045125316638008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Erowid, E. (2017). Erowid experience vaults. Retrieved from http://www.erowid.org/exp

  • Erowid, E., & Erowid, F. (2006). The value of experience. Erowid Extracts, 10, 1419. Retrieved from https://erowid.org/experiences/exp_info3.shtml

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fadiman, J. (2011). The psychedelic explorer’s guide: Safe, therapeutic, and sacred journeys. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

  • Grob, C. S. , McKenna, D. J. , Callaway, J. C. , Brito, G. S. , Neves, E. S. , Oberlaender, G. , Saide, O. L. , Labigalini, E. , Tacla, C. , Miranda, C. T. , Strassman, R. J., & Boone, K. B. (1996). Human psychopharmacology of hoasca, a plant hallucinogen used in ritual context in Brazil. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 184(2), 8694. doi:10.1097/00005053-199602000-00004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Halpern, J. H., & Pope, H. G. (2001). Hallucinogens on the Internet: A vast new source of underground drug information. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 481483. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.481

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hartogsohn, I. (2016). Set and setting, psychedelics and the placebo response: An extra-pharmacological perspective on psychophamacology. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12591267. doi:10.1177/0269881116677852

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hearne, E., & Van Hout, M. C. (2016). “Trip-Sitting” in the black hole: A netnographic study of dissociation and indigenous harm reduction. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 48(4), 233242. doi:10.1080/02791072.2016.1207827

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hill, J. , Ford, W. R., & Farreras, I. G. (2015). Real conversations with artificial intelligence: A comparison between human-human online conversations and human-chatbot conversations. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 245250. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.026

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hofmann, A., Ratsch, C., & Schultes, R. (1992). Plants of the Gods: Their Sacred, Healing, and Hallucinogenic Powers. Rochester, NY: Healing Arts Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Isaac, C. , Chertoff, J. , Lee, B., & Carnes, M. (2011). Do students’ and authors’ genders affect evaluations? A linguistic analysis of medical student performance evaluations. Academic Medicine, 86(1), 5966. doi:10.1097/ACM.0b013e318200561d

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kuypers, K. P. , Riba, J. , de la Fuente Revenga, M. , Barker, S. , Theunissen, E. L., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2016). Ayahuasca enhances creative divergent thinking while decreasing conventional convergent thinking. Psychopharmacology, 233(18), 33953403. doi:10.1007/s00213-016-4377-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Labate, B. C., & Cavnar, C. (Eds.). (2014). The therapeutic use of Ayahuasca. Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-Verlag.

  • Labate, B. C. , Rose, I. S., & Dos Santos, R. G. (2009). Ayahuasca religions: A comprehensive bibliography and critical essays. Santa Cruz, CA: Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, T. , Metzner, R., & Alpert, R. (1964). The psychedelic experience: A manual based on the Tibetan book of the dead. New Hyde Park, NY: University Books.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. (2014). Therapeutic effects of ritual ayahuasca use in the treatment of substance dependence – Qualitative results. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 46(1), 6372. doi:10.1080/02791072.2013.873157

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. (2006). Ayahuasca an ethnopharmacologic history. In R. Metzner (Eds.), Sacred vine of spirits: Ayahuasca (pp. 4062). Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. , Towers, G. H. N., & Abbott, F. S. (1984). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors in South American hallucinogenic plants: Tryptamine and β-carboline constituents of ayahuasca. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 10(2), 195223. doi:10.1016/0378-8741(84)90003-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mithoefer, M. C. , Grob, C. S., & Brewerton, T. D. (2016). Novel psychopharmacological therapies for psychiatric disorders: Psilocybin and MDMA. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(5), 481488. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00576-3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Monson, C. M. , Schnurr, P. P. , Resick, P. A. , Friedman, M. J. , Young-Xu, Y., & Stevens, S. P. (2006). Cognitive processing therapy for veterans with military-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(5), 898907. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.898

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ott, J. (1994). Ayahuasca analogues: Pangæan entheogens. Kennewick, WA: Natural Products Company.

  • Palhano-Fontes, F. , Barreto, D. , Onias, H. , Andrade, K. C. , Novaes, M. M. , Pessla, J. A. , Mota-Rolim, S. A. , Osório, F. L. , Sanches, R. , Dos Santos, R. G. , Tófoli, L. F. , de Oliveira Silveira, G. , Yonamine, M. , Riba, J. , Santos, F. R. , Silva-Junior, A. A. , Alchieri, J. C. , Galvão-Coelho, N. L. , Lobão-Soares, B. , Hallak, J. E. C. , Arcoverde, E. , Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., & Araújo, D. B. (2019). Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 655663. doi:10.1017/S0033291718001356

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pennebaker, J. W. , Boyd, R. L. , Jordan, K., & Blackburn, K. (2015). The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2015. Austin, TX: University of Texas at Austin.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pierce, P. A., & Peroutka, S. J. (1989). Hallucinogenic drug interactions with neurotransmitter receptor binding sites in human cortex. Psychopharmacology, 97(1), 118122. doi:10.1007/BF00443425

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rätsch, C. (2005). The Encyclopedia of psychoactive plants: Ethnopharmacology and its applications. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.

  • Resick, P. A. , Nishith, P. , Weaver, T. L. , Millie, C. A., & Feurer, C. A. (2010). A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(4), 867879. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.70.4.867

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J. , Valle, M. , Urbano, G. , Yritia, M. , Morte, A., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2003). Human pharmacology of ayahuasca: Subjective and cardiovascular effects, monoamine metabolite excretion, and pharmacokinetics. Journal of Pharmacology, 306, 7383. doi:10.1124/jpet.103.049882

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sampedro, F. , de la Fuente Revenga, M. , Valle, M. , Roberto, N. , Domínguez-Clavé, E. , Elices, M. , Luna, L. E. , Crippa, J. A. S. , Hallak, J. E. C. , de Araujo, D. B. , Friedlander, P. , Barker, S. A. , Álvarez, E. , Soler, J. , Pascual, J. C. , Feilding, A., & Riba, J. (2017). Assessing the psychedelic “After-Glow” in ayahuasca users: Post-acute neurometabolic and functional connectivity changes are associated with enhanced mindfulness capacities. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(9), 698711. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyx036

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanches, R. F. , Osório, F. L. , dos Santos, R. G. , Macedo, L. R. H. , Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P. , Wichert-Ana, L. , de Araujo, D. B. , Riba, J. , Crippa, J. A. S., & Hallak, J. E. (2016). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A SPECT study. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 36(1), 7781. doi:10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanz, C., & Tagliazucchi, E. (2018). The experience elicited by hallucinogens presents the highest similarity to dreaming within a large database of psychoactive substances reports. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 12, 7. doi:10.3389/fnins.2018.00007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schultes, R. E. , Hofmann, A., & Rätsch, C. (2001). Plant of the gods: Their sacred, healing, and hallucinogenic powers. Rochester, VT: Healing Arts Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shanon, B. (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the ayahuasca experience. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smith, R. , Canton, H. , Barrett, R. J., & Sanders-Bush, E. (1998). Agonist properties of N, N-dimethyltryptamine at serotonin 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 61(3), 323330. doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00110-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soler, J. , Elices, M. , Domínguez-Clavé, E , Pascual, J. C. , Feilding, A. , Navarro-Gil, M. , García-Campayo, J., & Riba, J. (2018). Four weekly ayahuasca sessions lead to increases in “Acceptance” capacities: A comparison study with a standard 8-week mindfulness training program. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 224. doi:10.3389/fphar.2018.00224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soler, J. , Elices, M. , Franguesa, A. , Barker, S. , Friedlander, P. , Feilding, A. , Pascual, J. C., & Riba, J. (2016). Exploring the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: Acute intake increases mindfulness-related capacities. Psychopharmacology, 233(5), 823829. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4162-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Spaderna, M. , Addy, P. H., & D’souza, D. C. (2013). Spicing things up: Synthetic cannabinoids. Psychopharmacology, 228(4), 525540. doi:10.1007/s00213-013-3188-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Studerus, E. , Gamma, A. , Kometer, M., & Vollenwieder, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PLoS One, 7(2), e30800. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030800

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swogger, M. T. , Hart, E. , Erowid, F. , Erowid, E. , Trabold, N. , Yee, K. , Parkhurst, K. A. , Priddy, B. M., & Walsh, Z. (2015). Experience of Kratom users: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47(5), 360367. doi:10.1080/02791072.2015.1096434

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Talin, P., & Sanabria, E. (2017). Ayahuasca’s entwined efficacy: An ethnographic study of ritual healing from ‘addiction’. The International Journal of Drug Policy, 44, 2330. doi:10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.02.017

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words; LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 2454. doi:10.1177/0261927X09351676

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thomas, G. , Lucas, P. , Capler, N. R. , Tupper, K. W., & Martin, G. (2013). Ayahuasca-assisted therapy for addiction: Results from a preliminary observational study in Canada. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 6(1), 3042. doi:10.2174/15733998113099990003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. (2009). Ayahuasca healing beyond the Amazon: The globalization of a traditional indigenous entheogenic practice. Global Networks: A Journal of Transnational Affairs, 9(1), 117136. doi:10.1111/j.1471-0374.2009.00245.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. , Wood, E. , Yensen, R., & Johnson, M. W. (2015). Psychedelic medicine: A re-emerging therapy. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 187(14), 10541059. doi:10.1503/cmaj.141124

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uthang, M. V. , van Oorsouw, K. , Kuypers, K. P. C. , van Boxtel, M. , Broers, N. J. , Mason, N. L. , Toennes, S. W. , Riba, J., & Ramaekers, J. G. (2018). Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology, 235, 29792989. doi:10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Waldmeier, P. C., & Maître, L. (1977). Neurochemical investigations of the interaction of N, N-dimethyltryptamine with the dopaminergic system in rat brain. Psychopharmacology, 52(2), 137144. doi:10.1007/BF00439100

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zinberg, N. E. (1984). Drug, set, and setting: The basis for controlled intoxicant use. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  • Collapse
  • Expand

Editor-in-Chief:

Attila Szabo - University of Oslo

E-mail address: attilasci@gmail.com

Managing Editor:

Zsófia Földvári, Oslo University Hospital

 

Associate Editors:

  • Alexander De Foe, School of Educational Psychology and Counselling, Monash University, Australia
  • Zsolt Demetrovics - Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • Ede Frecska, founding Editor-in-Chief - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • David Luke - University of Greenwich, London, UK
  • Dennis J. McKenna- Heffter Research Institute, St. Paul, USA
  • Jeremy Narby - Swiss NGO Nouvelle Planète, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • Stephen Szára - Retired from National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda, USA
  • Enzo Tagliazucchi - Latin American Brain Health Institute, Santiago, Chile, and University of Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • Michael Winkelman - Retired from Arizona State University, Tempe, USA 

Book Reviews Editor:

Michael Winkelman - Retired from Arizona State University, Tempe, USA

Editorial Board

  • Gábor Andrássy - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • Paulo Barbosa - State University of Santa Cruz, Bahia, Brazil
  • Michael Bogenschutz - New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
  • Petra Bokor - University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • Jose Bouso - Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • Zoltán Brys - Multidisciplinary Soc. for the Research of Psychedelics, Budapest, Hungary
  • Susana Bustos - California Institute of Integral Studies San Francisco, USA
  • Robin Carhart-Harris - Imperial College, London, UK
  • Per Carlbring - Stockholm University, Sweden
  • Valerie Curran - University College London, London, UK
  • Alicia Danforth - Harbor-UCLA Medical Center, Los Angeles, USA
  • Alan K. Davis - The Ohio State University & Johns Hopkins University, USA
  • Rick Doblin - Boston, USA
  • Rafael G. dos Santos - University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil
  • Genis Ona Esteve - Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
  • Silvia Fernandez-Campos
  • Zsófia Földvári - Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
  • Andrew Gallimore - University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • Neal Goldsmith - private practice, New York, NY, USA
  • Charles Grob - Harbor-UCLA Medical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • Stanislav Grof - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • Karen Grue - private practice, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • Jiri Horacek - Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
  • Lajos Horváth - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • Robert Jesse - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Matthew Johnson - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Eli Kolp - Kolp Institute New, Port Richey, FL, USA
  • Stanley Krippner - Saybrook University, Oakland, CA, USA
  • Evgeny Krupitsky - St. Petersburg State Pavlov Medical University, St. Petersburg, Russia
  • Rafael Lancelotta - Innate Path, Lakewood, CO, USA
  • Anja Loizaga-Velder - National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
  • Luis Luna - Wasiwaska Research Center, Florianópolis, Brazil
  • Katherine MacClean - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Deborah Mash - University of Miami School of Medicine, Miami, USA
  • Friedericke Meckel - private practice, Zurich, Switzerland
  • Ralph Metzner - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • Michael Mithoefer - private practice, Charleston, SC, USA
  • Levente Móró - University of Turku, Turku, Finland
  • David Nichols - Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
  • David Nutt - Imperial College, London, UK
  • Torsten Passie - Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany
  • Janis Phelps - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • József Rácz - Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary
  • Christian Rätsch - University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • Sidarta Ribeiro - Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
  • William Richards - Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Stephen Ross - New York University, New York, NY, USA
  • Brian Rush - University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
  • Eduardo Schenberg - Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • Ben Sessa - Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
  • Lowan H. Stewart - Santa Fe Ketamine Clinic, NM, USA (Medical Director)
  • Rebecca Stone - Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • Rick Strassman - University of New Mexico School of Medicine, Albuquerque, NM, USA
  • Csaba Szummer - Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church, Budapest, Hungary
  • Manuel Torres - Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
  • Luís Fernando Tófoli - University of Campinas, Campinas, Brazil State
  • Malin Uthaug - Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
  • Julian Vayne - Norwich, UK
  • Nikki Wyrd - Norwich, UK

Attila Szabo
University of Oslo

E-mail address: attilasci@gmail.com

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science ESCI
  • Biological Abstracts
  • BIOSIS Previews
  • APA PsycInfo
  • DOAJ
  • Scopus
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 2.2
Rank by Impact Factor Q2 (Psychology, Multidisciplinary)
Journal Citation Indicator 0.89
Scopus  
CiteScore 2.5
CiteScore rank Q1 (Anthropology)
SNIP 0.553
Scimago  
SJR index 0.503
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Psychedelic Studies
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge €990
Subscription Information Gold Open Access
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%. 
   

Journal of Psychedelic Studies
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2016
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
3
Founder Akadémiai Kiadó
Debreceni Egyetem
Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Károli Gáspár Református Egyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
H-4032 Debrecen, Hungary Egyetem tér 1.
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
H-1091 Budapest, Hungary Kálvin tér 9.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2559-9283 (Online)

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Jun 2024 0 102 17
Jul 2024 0 43 17
Aug 2024 0 46 18
Sep 2024 0 73 18
Oct 2024 0 189 31
Nov 2024 0 114 28
Dec 2024 0 7 1