Authors:
Brianna R. Altman Department of Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY, USA

Search for other papers by Brianna R. Altman in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9254-2939
,
M. N. Mian Department of Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY, USA

Search for other papers by M. N. Mian in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
M. Earleywine Department of Psychology, University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY, USA

Search for other papers by M. Earleywine in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Abstract

Objective

Given the growing popularity of ayahuasca and other hallucinogens, we sought to identify related expectancies and their role in why people might use this psychoactive brew rather than classic hallucinogens like LSD or psilocybin.

Method

Experienced users (N = 139) completed an online survey about their use of hallucinogens (including ayahuasca), how their ayahuasca experiences differed from those with other hallucinogens, as well as their intentions to use hallucinogens (including ayahuasca) in the future.

Results

Participants expected ayahuasca effects to differ meaningfully from the effects of other hallucinogens in multiple domains. Expected differences involved positive connections to nature (and other people), dramatic (or terrifying) thoughts, high variability of effects, and physical reactions. Intentions to use ayahuasca in the future increased as expectancies for positive connections increased, but decreased as physical reactions increased. One version of the full Novelty of Ayahuasca scale significantly covaried with the likelihood of using ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future, while another version did not. This result calls for further work examining how users perceive certain ayahuasca-induced effects.

Conclusions

These findings reveal that expectancies for ayahuasca differ from those for other hallucinogens, and impact an individual's willingness to try the substance again. Understanding these differences might help those who are interested in the therapeutic potential of hallucinogens decide among those available. Future studies might examine how these expectancies predict intentions to use ayahuasca (and other hallucinogens) longitudinally. Comparable approaches could also identify unique expectancies for other hallucinogens to see if those predict a preference for one over others.

Abstract

Objective

Given the growing popularity of ayahuasca and other hallucinogens, we sought to identify related expectancies and their role in why people might use this psychoactive brew rather than classic hallucinogens like LSD or psilocybin.

Method

Experienced users (N = 139) completed an online survey about their use of hallucinogens (including ayahuasca), how their ayahuasca experiences differed from those with other hallucinogens, as well as their intentions to use hallucinogens (including ayahuasca) in the future.

Results

Participants expected ayahuasca effects to differ meaningfully from the effects of other hallucinogens in multiple domains. Expected differences involved positive connections to nature (and other people), dramatic (or terrifying) thoughts, high variability of effects, and physical reactions. Intentions to use ayahuasca in the future increased as expectancies for positive connections increased, but decreased as physical reactions increased. One version of the full Novelty of Ayahuasca scale significantly covaried with the likelihood of using ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future, while another version did not. This result calls for further work examining how users perceive certain ayahuasca-induced effects.

Conclusions

These findings reveal that expectancies for ayahuasca differ from those for other hallucinogens, and impact an individual's willingness to try the substance again. Understanding these differences might help those who are interested in the therapeutic potential of hallucinogens decide among those available. Future studies might examine how these expectancies predict intentions to use ayahuasca (and other hallucinogens) longitudinally. Comparable approaches could also identify unique expectancies for other hallucinogens to see if those predict a preference for one over others.

Introduction

Psychedelic substances, such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) and psilocybin, are well-known for their ability to alter states of consciousness through their interaction with serotonin neurotransmitter systems (Glennon, Titeler, & McKenney, 1984; Jacobs, 1987). These hallucinogens might induce heightened awareness of one's self and surroundings, increases in emotionality, and physiological changes, including pupil dilation, dizziness, and loss of appetite (Grinspoon & Bakalar, 1983). Not all users will experience the same effects, potentially due to the impact of set (aspects of the individual) and setting (aspects of the environment) (Leary, Litwin, & Metzner, 1963). As of 2010, approximately 32 million Americans used LSD, psilocybin, mescaline, or peyote in their lifetimes (Krebs & Johansen, 2013). Moreover, lifetime hallucinogen use in the United States has increased since 2001, possibly due to the amplified curiosity about this class of substances (Shalit, Rehm, & Lev-Ran, 2019). While many individuals use these substances recreationally, new research reveals clinical utility of these drugs in treating depression, anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and substance use disorders (Bogenschutz & Johnson, 2016; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Mithoefer et al., 2018; Ot'alora et al., 2018; Ross et al., 2016; Rucker, Jelen, Flynn, Frowde, & Young, 2016; dos Santos, Bouso, Alcázar-Córcoles, & Hallak, 2018; Morgan, McAndrew, Stevens, Nutt, & Lawn, 2017). These recent findings call for an in-depth exploration of individual substances within this class of drugs, and why people might choose one over another.

Ayahuasca

Ayahuasca is a tea with psychoactive properties derived from the plants Banisteriopsis caapi and Psychotria viridis. The main psychoactive properties in ayahuasca result from the combination of dimethyltryptamine, or DMT, and beta-carboline alkaloids, which allow for the oral consumption of DMT to be psychoactive (McKenna, 2004; McKenna, Towers, & Abbott, 1984; Riba, Valle, Urbano, Yritia, Morte, & Barbanoj, 2003). Many citizens of South America turn to ayahuasca to alleviate illness and promote spiritual or religious experiences, although use has become more widespread among individuals seeking personal growth or emotional healing (Labate & Cavnar, 2014; Schmid, 2012; Tupper, 2008; Winkelman, 2011). Ingestion typically occurs during ceremonies or retreats, often led by trained shamans (Grunwell, 1998; Metzner, 1999). Some ceremonies encourage socialization among groups, with singing and music, while others promote a quieter and more introspective experience (Harris & Gurel, 2012; Luna, 1986, 2011; Winkelman, 2001). The interaction of the shaman and the specifics of the ritual set the tone for the ingestion experience.

Ayahuasca produces a host of subjective effects, usually within an hour of ingestion (Riba et al., 2001). Users might experience intense, dream-like hallucinations or increased sensitivity to auditory stimuli (Domínguez-Clavé et al., 2016; Riba et al., 2001). Not all users experience these effects, perhaps suggesting an unpredictable nature of hallucinogens in general (Domínguez-Clavé et al., 2016; Swanson, 2018). Ayahuasca might also induce cognitive changes, producing intense emotional reactions and generating new perspectives (Frecska, Bokor, & Winkelman, 2016; Uthaug et al., 2018). Other users report experiencing frightening, paranoia-like states, which might instill regret about the user's choice to participate in the ceremony (Kjellgren, Eriksson, & Norlander, 2009). Typically, adverse reactions subside after short periods; verbal encouragement can help to provide relief in some circumstances (Riba et al., 2001). Additionally, many individuals experience nausea or vomiting after ingestion (Riba et al., 2001; Shanon, 2002). While these effects are discouraging to some, others cite purging as a necessary component of ayahuasca use, allowing for the release of psychological suffering and summoning a meaningful perspective on past experiences (Kavenská & Simonová, 2015; Loizaga-Velder, 2013). In spite of these physical short-term side effects, healthy users tolerate ayahuasca well, and long-term negative reactions are rare in laboratory settings (dos Santos, Balthazar, Bouso, & Hallak, 2016).

Although literature suggests ayahuasca's effects may parallel the impact of other psychedelics including LSD and mushrooms, some work suggests otherwise. One qualitative study suggests that individuals should be experienced with the effects of hallucinogens before trying ayahuasca (Wiecko, Thompson, & Parham, 2017). In fact, many ayahuasca users report experience with other psychedelics including LSD and mushrooms (Harris & Gurel, 2012). Individuals also rated the effects of ayahuasca as stronger than more typical hallucinogens (Lawn et al., 2017). Moreover, a recent study of “God encounter experiences” suggests that ayahuasca users reported more positive life outcomes after their God encounter, including enhanced social relationships and more positive self-views, compared to those who used LSD or psilocybin (Griffiths, Hurwitz, Davis, Johnson, & Jesse, 2019). Taken together, these results suggest discernible features of the ayahuasca experience potentially render it unique from more traditional psychedelics; however, given the paucity of research on this topic, more work appears justified. These variations in subjective effects might influence individuals' likelihood to use ayahuasca or other hallucinogens in the future. Should the effects prove discernably unique, participants might benefit from an education about the novel facets of the tea.

The influence of expectancies on future hallucinogen use

Substance use expectancies are individuals' beliefs about how a substance might influence their subjective experience (McPherson & Harris, 2015). Positive expectancies typically covary with more frequent and heavy substance use, while negative expectancies often predict abstinence (Gaher & Simons, 2007). In both the cannabis and alcohol literature, expectancies about substance effects are associated with either drug-taking or avoidance behaviors (Christiansen, Smith, Roehling, and Goldman, 1989; Jones, Corbin, & Fromme, 2001; Metrik et al., 2009; Schafer & Brown, 1991). Very little work has directly examined the role of hallucinogen expectancies and their influence on users' subjective experiences and future ingestion behavior. Positive expectancies about the effects of psilocybin covary with more enjoyable ingestion experiences (Metzner, Litwin, & Weil, 1965). Previous experiences with classic hallucinogens might influence present psilocybin use (Studerus, Gamma, Kometer, & Vollenwider, 2012). To the author's knowledge, the impact of expectancies about ayahuasca and their role in determining future ayahuasca use remains unexamined. For this reason, an exploration of the role of expectancies and other predictors on the use of ayahuasca or other hallucinogens appears warranted.

The present study

Perhaps due to the sacred nature of the ayahuasca ceremony, research is lacking about individuals' experiences with the psychoactive tea and how it differs from other substances. Current research cannot identify who might seek out ayahuasca rather than other hallucinogens.

The present study queried users about their experience with ayahuasca (and whether it differed from other substances) in an effort to predict individual substance choices. We hypothesized that ayahuasca use would covary with a greater connection to nature than other psychedelic drugs. Ayahuasca might also produce more undesirable physical reactions, such as vomiting, nausea, and diarrhea than other hallucinogens, which might discourage later use. Alternatively, some individuals might cite these physical effects as meaningful components of the ayahuasca experience, which could positively relate to their future use. Additionally, we believed that the sum of these experiences would correlate with individuals' willingness to ingest ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future. Should these findings hold true, we might better understand the facets of the ayahuasca experience that compel individuals to choose ayahuasca over other psychedelics.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Participants were recruited via community Facebook pages pertaining to ayahuasca and other hallucinogen use. In these Facebook groups, we posted a SurveyMonkey (www.surveymonkey.com) link inviting interested individuals to share their thoughts about ayahuasca and other hallucinogens. SurveyMonkey is a secure, internet-based data collection tool that allows researchers to develop and distribute surveys to willing participants. Upon clicking the SurveyMonkey link, participants received information about the study's aims and were prompted to provide informed consent if they were interested in participating in our study. Only individuals who were at least 18 years old were invited to complete the survey. Other researchers have used similar methodology to recruit participants for studies on hallucinogens (Carbonaro et al., 2016).

Invited participants (N = 152) were then asked to complete an online survey assessing demographic variables, ayahuasca and other hallucinogen use, beliefs about ayahuasca's novelty compared to other hallucinogens, and future use intentions. For the purposes of this study, only individuals who used ayahuasca at least once (N = 139) were included in data analysis. All study procedures were completed in accordance with and approved by the local Institutional Review Board.

Measures

Demographics of study participants

The majority of the sample was male (54.3%) and reported an age between 40 and 49 (29.5%). Participants were predominantly white (87.1%) and highly educated, with 29% reporting holding a bachelor's degree and 29.7% reporting having a graduate degree.

Ayahuasca use

Participants reported on their lifetime use of ayahuasca (M = 43.96, Median = 9.00, SD = 139.28) and how many years (M = 0.58, Median = 0.00, SD = 1.838) and months (M = 2.77, Median = 2.00, SD = 2.77) had elapsed since their most recent ingestion.

Hallucinogen use

Participants also reported on their lifetime use of other LSD, mushrooms, DMT, and other substances, as seen in Table 1.

Table 1.

Lifetime use of hallucinogens

HallucinogenMean (SD)MedianRange
LSD27.51 (104.99)1.000–1,000
Mushrooms25.64 (94.08)5.000–1,000
MDMA19.55 (82.7)0.000–5
DMT3.66 (11.21)0.000–100
Peyote2.34 (17.37)0.000–200
Ketamine2.18 (7.80)0.000–800
Mescaline2.00 (6.317)0.000–60
Salvia1.58 (5.50)0.000–50
Psilocybin0.19 (1.027)0.000–10
Ibogaine0.17 (0.63)0.000–5
PCP0.10 (0.643)0.000–6
DXM0.10 (0.634)0.000–5
DOT0.01 (0.087)0.000–1

Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale

A separate sample of 19 participants helped generate items by responding to the prompt, “How is ayahuasca different from other hallucinogens like mushrooms or LSD?” in a group setting. Of these 19 participants, 11 were women, approximately 74% were Caucasian, their mean age was approximately 43, and they were also highly educated (∼95% hold a Bachelor's degree or higher). Each participant had used ayahuasca at least twice and used other hallucinogens, such as LSD and mushrooms, at least once. On average, endorsements of use were much higher than these selection criteria. Initial ideas led to 44 items, which were collapsed to 22 based on similar content. These 22 were then reviewed by two people (1 female, both Caucasian, Mage = 48.5, both hold graduate degrees) familiar with scale construction who had used ayahuasca, LSD, and mushrooms at least six times each. These individuals reduced the list to 19 items based on ambiguity or overlapping content. These 19 items were then reviewed by a shaman trainee (male, age = 45, Caucasian, Bachelor's degree) who had used ayahuasca approximately 700 times and mushrooms and LSD approximately 10 times each. For demographics of scale contributors, see Table 2. The final list of items used in this scale appears in Table 3 Participants were presented with the prompt, “Compared to ‘typical hallucinogens’, ayahuasca…” and were asked to respond to the items on a Likert scale from −2 (completely false) to 2 (completely true). Cronbach's alpha for these original 19 items was 0.784.

Table 2.

Demographics of contributors to scale items

ContributorsMean (SD)MedianRange
Initial contributors (N = 19)
Lifetime use of
 Ayahuasca42.58 (10.93)43.0026–61
 LSD17.11 (12.50)11.000–50
 Mushrooms26.79 (15.11)22.009–60
 MDMA12.26 (8.79)11.000–30
 DMT1.58 (2.36)0.000–8
 Peyote5.68 (6.15)4.000–27
 Ketamine2.52 (3.63)0.000–10
 Mescaline0.21 (0.63)0.000–2
 Salvia10.79 (8.89)10.000–30
Item reviewers (N = 2)
 Ayahuasca57.00 (12.73)57.0048–66
 LSD22.50 (14.85)22.5012–33
 Mushrooms23.50 (7.78)23.5018–29
 MDMA7.00 (4.24)7.004–10
 DMT1.00 (1.41)1.000–2
 Peyote4.50 (6.36)4.500–9
 Ketamine0.00 (0.00)0.000
 Mescaline0.00 (0.00)0.000
 Salvia18.50 (3.54)18.5016–21
Shaman trainee (N = 1)
 Ayahuasca∼700
 LSD∼100
 Mushrooms∼200
 MDMA∼50
 DMT5
 Peyote1
 Ketamine∼100
 Mescaline3
 Salvia∼100

Likelihood of future use

Participants reported their likelihood to ingest ayahuasca again in their lifetime, the next year, the next 3 months, and the next month, ranging from −3 = “Impossible” to 3 = “Definitely will happen”. Participants also reported on their likelihood to use other hallucinogens. We generated a difference variable by subtracting “likelihood to use other hallucinogens” from “likelihood to use ayahuasca,” indicating how likely participants were to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in their lifetime, the next year, the next 3 months, and the next month. We then combined these four variables (Cronbach's α = 0.897) to generate one composite variable, indicating participant likelihood of using ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future. Thus, this variable served as an index of the magnitude of intentions to use ayahuasca over and above the intentions to use other hallucinogens in all subsequent analyses.

Data analytic plan

First, we conducted a series of one-sample t-tests against 0 to examine whether participants believed that ayahuasca produced significantly different effects from other hallucinogens. For these 19 analyses, we considered a P-value of P < 0.001 (0.05/19) to be significant (Wilcox, 2013). Next, we performed an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale to identify factorable components of individuals' experiences with the substance. Additionally, we conducted a multiple regression to determine whether the factors yielded by the EFA accounted for a significant amount of unique variance in preference for ayahuasca over other hallucinogens, while controlling for the influence of gender and lifetime ayahuasca use. Finally, we examined correlations among the factors of the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale, the global scale after removing unfit items, and participant likelihood of using ayahuasca again over other hallucinogens.

Results

Ayahuasca versus other hallucinogens

One sample t-tests indicated that all ayahuasca experience variables were significantly different from 0, P < 0.001, (t(133–136) ranging from 6.212 to 27.658), except for “almost requires being out in nature” (t(136)=2.033, P = 0.044), “has the potential to lead to darker thoughts” (t(135)=1.459, P = 0.147) and “can create more negative thoughts or emotions” (t(134)= −2.276, P = 0.024; See Table 3). Cohen's d effect sizes ranged from small to very large (d = 0.14–2.18).

Table 3.

Results of one sample t-tests

VariableMean (SD)t-ValueAbs Value of Cohen's d
Can seem particularly great with the right facilitators/shaman1.51 (0.63)27.66*2.18
Could create strong visuals or none at all1.43 (0.76)22.17*1.80
Is less “recreational”1.47 (0.92)18.67*1.62
Is more of a medicine1.39 (0.80)20.50*1.61
Makes people throw up more often1.21 (0.74)18.85*1.59
Seems closer to other plants and the earth1.31 (0.83)18.13*1.55
Can cause more nausea1.20 (0.80)17.65*1.47
Creates results that are harder to predict1.20 (0.91)15.33*1.30
Has more of a connection to the earth1.11 (0.90)14.48*1.26
Is more likely to have no impact at all−1.20 (1.07)−13.10*1.17
Changes more with the shaman0.95 (0.83)13.31*1.13
Makes bigger demands of users0.80 (1.07)8.74*0.79
Leads to thoughts of being out in the open or in the wilderness0.60 (0.84)8.21*0.73
Depends more on songs and music0.62 (0.93)7.85*0.63
Leads to diarrhea more often0.63 (1.11)6.54*0.56
Can be more terrifying0.60 (1.11)6.21*0.55
Almost requires being in nature0.20 (1.18)2.030.22
Can create more negative thoughts or emotions−0.21 (1.06)−2.280.15
Has the potential to lead to darker thoughts0.14 (1.11)1.460.14

Prompt: How true is each of these items? “Compared to ‘typical hallucinogens’, ayahuasca…”.

Scale: −2 (completely false), −1 (mostly false), 0 (neither true nor false), 1 (mostly true), 2 (completely true).

Note: *P < 0.001.

Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale

We conducted an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) using a principal-axis factor extraction to establish the factor structure of 19 items related to participants' experiences using ayahuasca compared to other hallucinogens. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy (0.72) and Bartlett's test of sphericity (χ2 = 675.01, P < 0.001) demonstrated that these data were factorable. A visual inspection of the scree plot and examination of eigenvalues greater than 1.00, revealed that a three-factor solution was appropriate (eigenvalues = 4.03, 2.92, and 1.65). To interpret these three factors, we used a promax rotation. After running this preliminary 3-factor model, we noted that seven of the original variables did not map onto these three factors (“almost requires being out in nature,” “could create strong visuals or none at all,” “depends more on songs and music,” “is less recreational,” “is more likely to have no impact at all,” “makes bigger demands of users,” and “leads to thoughts of being out in the open or wilderness”). Removal of these seven items decreased Cronbach's alpha from 0.78 to 0.69. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of sampling adequacy remained the same (0.72) and Bartlett's test of sphericity remained significant (χ2 = 434.74, P < 0.001). An examination of extracted communalities suggested that these remaining items mapped onto the three factors well. A three-factor model still appeared justified (eigenvalues = 3.03, 2.62, 1.38), and accounted for 58.60% of the total variance.

Factor 1 (five items) represented positive aspects of users' ayahuasca experiences that increased their connection to nature and their shaman. Factor 2 (four items) signified aspects of users' experiences that led to more negative or unpredictable reactions. Factor 3 (three items) characterized physical reactions associated with ayahuasca use. The factors accounted for 22.25%, 21.85%, and 11.49% of the total scale variance respectively. Table 4 demonstrates observed factor loadings for each item. Indices of internal consistency for the three factors appeared to be within the acceptable range (Cronbach's α = 0.76, 0.74, and 0.70, respectively).

Table 4.

Factor analysis of ayahuasca experiences constructs

ItemFactor 1Factor 2Factor 3
Changes more with the shaman0.4570.036−0.130
Has more of a connection to the earth0.763−0.0020.037
Is more of a medicine0.751−0.0220.017
Seems closer to other plants and the earth0.5330.032−0.017
Can seem particularly great with the right facilitators or shaman0.705−0.1020.029
Creates results that are harder to predict0.3320.469−0.022
Can be more terrifying0.0180.5530.212
Has the potential to lead to darker thoughts−0.0090.831−0.058
Can create more negative thoughts or emotions−0.1420.677−0.029
Can cause more nausea−0.0100.0630.699
Leads to diarrhea more often−0.0610.0640.570
Makes people throw up more often0.012−0.0950.783
Eigenvalues3.032.621.38
% of Variance25.2521.8511.49

Prompt: How true is each of these items? “Compared to ‘typical hallucinogens’, ayahuasca…”.

Scale: −2 (completely false), −1 (mostly false), 0 (neither true nor false), 1 (mostly true), 2 (completely true).

Bolded values indicate factor loadings of items that comprise each factor.

Ayahuasca over other hallucinogens

We regressed preference for ayahuasca on the three factors of the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale while controlling for gender and lifetime ayahuasca use. After removal of seven outliers, the data met assumptions for linearity, normality, heteroscedasticity, and multicollinearity. The regression was significant (F(5,113)= 3.97, P < 0.01, adjusted R2 = 0.11). The adjusted R2 value suggests that approximately 11% of the variance in the dependent variable could be accounted for. Only factor 1 (B = 0.69, t = 2.99, P < 0.01) and factor 3 (B = −0.89, t = −2.68, P < 0.01) had significant main effects. The squared semi partial coefficients revealed that removal of factors 1 and 3 from the regression equation would yield a 7 and 6% decrease in predictive utility, respectively. Participant gender, lifetime ayahuasca use, and factor 2 were not significant (See Table 5).

Table 5.

Predicting preference for ayahuasca over other hallucinogens from gender, lifetime ayahuasca use, and factors 1, 2, and 3

PredictorPreference for AyahuascaBSEβCI (95%)sr2
Gender1.452.001.380.13−0.74, 4.730.02
Lifetime ayahuasca use1.240.030.020.11−0.02, 0.070.01
Factor 12.99*0.690.230.260.23, 1.140.07
Factor 20.460.100.230.04−0.34, 0.550.00
Factor 3−2.68*−0.890.33−0.25−1.54, −0.230.06
Intercept−2.583.05

Note: *P < 0.01.

Predicting ayahuasca and other substance use in the future

The total score on The Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale (sum of all 12 items with items from factors 2 and 3 reverse-scored; Cronbach's alpha = 0.661) significantly positively covaried with intentions to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future, and intentions to use ayahuasca again in the future, but negatively related to intentions to use other hallucinogens in the future. Factor 1 of the scale was positively related to individuals likelihood of future ayahuasca use over other hallucinogens and negatively related to intentions to use other hallucinogens in the future. Factor 1 did not correlate significantly with simple intentions to use ayahuasca. Factor 2 was not significantly related to any measure of intentions of future use. Factor 3 not only varied significantly (and inversely) with intentions to use ayahuasca in the future, it also covaried negatively with intentions to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens. Additionally, using the method for comparing correlated correlation coefficients proposed by Meng, Rosenthal, and Rubin (1992), we found that the relation between ayahuasca preference and factor 3 was significantly stronger than the association between ayahuasca preference and factor 1 (P < 0.001).

Alternatively, two astute and anonymous reviewers pointed out that factor 3 might not necessarily be interpreted as negative by all users; thus, we recalculated the global scale with only items from factor 2 reverse-scored (Cronbach's alpha = 0.404). As expected, all three factors significantly positively covaried with this global score. However, this global score did not significantly covary with any of the three future use variables. Correlations among study variables appear in Table 6.

Table 6.

Table of correlations

VariablesMean (SD)1234567
1. Factor 16.27 (2.88)
2. Factor 21.69 (3.17)0.110
3. Factor 33.02 (2.12)−0.0060.341**
4. Global Scale (with F2 and F3 reverse scored)1.52 (5.06)0.457**−0.685**−0.654**
5. Global Scale (with F2 reverse scored)11.14 (5.43)0.594**0.780**0.586**−0.396**
6. Intentions to use aya in the future5.26 (5.37)0.095−0.100−0.251**0.136−0.093
7. Intentions to use other halls in the future2.31 (6.67)−0.207*−0.0220.148−0.193*−0.0610.139
8. Intentions to use aya over other halls in the future2.95 (7.98)0.242**−0.048−0.294**0.258**−0.0060.557**−0.745**

Note: *P < 0.05, **P < 0.05.

Discussion

Few studies address differences among hallucinogens and their role in intentions to use them. The present study sought to determine if novel aspects of the ayahuasca experience contributed to hypothetical decisions to use it again and to use it again rather than other hallucinogens. This information can help researchers focus on ways to minimize harm and let users make informed decisions about their use. Overall, our findings confirm that expectancies about ayahuasca's effects predict intentions to use the psychoactive brew again, much like research has revealed for other substances (Goldman, Brown, & Christiansen, 1987; Hayaki, Hagerty, Herman, de Dios, Anderson, & Stein, 2010; Metrik et al., 2009; Metzner et al., 1965). Moreover, these expectancies also predict intentions to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens.

Respondents reported striking differences between ayahuasca and typical hallucinogens. Two items that specifically mention that role of the shaman (“changes more with the shaman” and “can seem particularly great with the right facilitators or shaman”) showed large effects (Cohen's d = 1.13 and 2.18, respectively), confirming previous work (Kavenská & Simonová, 2015). Participants also believed ayahuasca “is more of a medicine” (Cohen's d = 1.61) and “is less ‘recreational’” (Cohen's d = 1.62) than other hallucinogens, as other work has shown (Harris & Gurel, 2012). Users also believed that ayahuasca increased their connectivity to earth and nature more so than other hallucinogens (Cohen's d = 1.55 and 1.26, respectively), perhaps because of its plant origin. The sum of these items created factor 1 of the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale and covaried positively with intentions to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future.

Participants also reported that the psychoactive brew is unpredictable, more likely to “create results that are harder to predict”, and “can be more terrifying” than other hallucinogens (Cohen's d = 1.30 and 0.55, respectively). These variables and two others addressing the negative cognitions produced by ayahuasca (“has the potential to lead to darker thoughts” and “can create more negative thoughts or emotions”) comprised factor 2. Factor 2 did not significantly relate to ayahuasca preference in our bivariate correlations or multiple regressions. Perhaps users consider these variable effects in combination with the perceived benefits of the psychoactive tea and they cancel each other out. Alternatively, individuals might recognize the potential for negative cognitions with all substances within this class; thus, it might not be predictive of whether or not someone uses ayahuasca again or over other hallucinogens.

Respondents also experience more nausea, vomiting, and diarrhea while using ayahuasca compared to other psychedelics (Cohen's d = 1.47, 1.59, and 0.56, respectively). These gastro-intestinal symptoms, which constitute factor 3, are inversely related to individuals' intentions to use ayahuasca over other hallucinogens in the future. Although some users might cite these experiences as necessary and meaningful components of the ayahuasca ceremony, in our work these physical side effects inhibit a preference for ayahuasca.

In combining these three factors, we sought to generate a scale which might educate interested individuals about the unique effects of ayahuasca compared to other hallucinogens. While one version of the global scale (with both factors 2 and 3 reverse-scored) significantly predicts who might choose ayahuasca over other hallucinogens, another version (with only factor 2 reverse scored) did not significantly relate to any future use variables. Relatedly, when we used this alternative calculation, the internal reliability dropped drastically. Future work should gather more information about how users perceive the ayahuasca-induced physical effects (e.g., vomiting, diarrhea) to determine whether these items should be reverse-scored before being aggregated into the global score. Nevertheless, the three unique factors of the scale have important relations to future use variables; individuals' responses to these items might help us predict what hallucinogen they are most inclined to use in the future.

Three items did not fit prevalent stereotypes about ayahuasca. Two items related to negative, dark, or gloomy cognitions failed to differ from zero on their endorsements (“has the potential to lead to darker thoughts” and “can create more negative thoughts or emotions”). This result contrasts with previous papers where users reported greater negative effects after ingesting ayahuasca than when using LSD or mushrooms (Lawn et al., 2017). Future work might benefit from more specific answer options to allow individuals to report nuances about their ayahuasca experiences. Furthermore, the only other item that did not differ significantly from 0 (“almost requires being out in nature”), might suggest a common feature of hallucinogen use — that an outdoor environment might enhance experience with any of them (Shanon, 2002). Although we are reluctant to argue from a null result, this result might reassure those who are contemplating ayahuasca consumption if they are concerned about a more aversive or gloomy experience than that created by other hallucinogens.

Potential contributors to item selection

A number of items from the original Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale did not map onto the three factors in our EFA. Further examination of these items yielded several hypotheses about the meaning of their exclusion. Some items which were excluded may have been too similar to items that were included. For instance, the correlation between “better with the right shaman” and “can seem particularly great with the right shaman” is r = 0.425. Future work should examine nuances between similar items to consider if they should be modified to gain a more detailed picture about the ayahuasca experience.

Three items related to set and setting (“almost requires being out in nature”, “depends more on songs and music”, and “leads to thoughts of being out in the open or wilderness”) did not map onto any of the observed factors in the EFA. While two of these three items yielded significant t-values in our independent t-tests, “almost requires being out in nature” did not (t = 2.03, P = 0.044). It is possible that these items did not uniquely relate to individuals' ayahuasca experiences and more broadly represent important attributes of psychedelic substances. Similarly, the item “could create strong visuals or none at all” yielded a significant t-value with a large effect size (t = 22.17, Cohen's d = 1.80), but did not map onto any of the observed EFA factors. This item may also not be specific enough to ayahuasca to warrant inclusion in the final Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale, as many psychedelics produce visual hallucinations. A more in-depth exploration of facets of the hallucinogen experience that are unique to ayahuasca appears warranted.

Potential therapeutic benefits of ayahuasca

Many users might seek ayahuasca for its purported medicinal and healing effects, thus an examination of the literature on clinical applications of ayahuasca is necessitated. Across numerous randomized control and open label trials, ayahuasca demonstrates strong antidepressant and anxiolytic effects (Osório et al., 2015; Palhano-Fontes et al., 2019; Sanches et al., 2016). Preliminary evidence suggests ayahuasca might help to inhibit problematic substance use, especially when used in conjunction with psychotherapy (Hamill, Hallak, Dursun, & Baker, 2018; Loizaga-Velder & Pazzi, 2014; Thomas, Lucas, Capler, Tupper, & Martin, 2013). Current work indicates ayahuasca poses limited risk to healthy adults across acute and long-term administration; future work should examine ayahuasca's safety among pregnant women, those with health problems, and those consuming other drugs or supplements (dos Santos, 2013; dos Santos et al., 2016). Despite being well-tolerated overall, reported adverse effects during consumption in clinical settings include anxiety, nausea, and vomiting (dos Santos et al., 2016; Osório et al., 2015). Individuals should weigh these potential outcomes against the expected benefits to make an informed decision about personal ayahuasca consumption.

Limitations

Like many computer-administered, cross-sectional, self-report studies of illicit psychoactive substance use and relevant correlates, these data require cautious interpretation. Our predominantly Caucasian, middle-aged, highly educated sample responded via computer on a single occasion long after subjective effects of ayahuasca had likely dissipated. Despite our concerted efforts to assure anonymity, legal sanctions against hallucinogens and imperfections in recall might alter participant reports. A large, diverse sample who could complete measures without access to computers could help reveal if the new scale's factor structure and the relations with intentions of future use applies to a wider range of people. A longitudinal design that permitted follow-ups of extended duration and confirmation of subsequent use of ayahuasca or other hallucinogens would certainly provide an improved test of these hypotheses. Additionally, qualitative reports could help clarify the nature of differences between various types of hallucinogens. Due to the importance of the highly subjective experience of these substances, future studies might employ interview formats to gain a more nuanced perspective about the unique qualities of specific drugs.

In addition, although individuals within this sample were relatively experienced with hallucinogens, few endorsed lifetime use of psilocybin, DXM, ibogaine, and DOT. Participants were most experienced with LSD, mushrooms, and MDMA; thus, our results are mostly generalizable to those substances. Future work might target individuals who have used ayahuasca in addition to other less popular substances to confirm whether these findings apply to those drugs as well. Relatedly, ayahuasca was the most frequently used hallucinogen among this sample, suggesting the possibility that individuals exaggerated the positive effects of this substance compared to other hallucinogens, due to its familiarity. Likewise, our work may be subject to selection bias in that only individuals with strongly positive or strongly negative attitudes toward ayahuasca might have self-selected to enroll in our study. To strengthen future attempts at replication, researchers might survey all willing participants after an ayahuasca ceremony to gain a more objective view of all individuals' experiences with the psychoactive tea.

Conclusion

Taken together, factor 3 (physical reactions) negatively covaries with individuals' intentions to use ayahuasca in the future. Ayahuasca-induced gastro-intestinal effects were associated with a decreased desire to use the substance in the future. Alternatively, factor 1 (positive connection to nature and other people) was negatively associated with a desire to use other hallucinogens in the future. This suggests that individuals' positive experiences with ayahuasca might decrease their desire for other hallucinogens. Finally, looking at users' intentions to seek out ayahuasca over more traditional psychedelic substances, it appears that factor 1 relates positively while factor 3 correlates negatively. Ayahuasca consumers, who experience more positive effects and increased connectiveness to the earth, might prefer to use the psychoactive tea again over other hallucinogens in their lifetime. Conversely, individuals who do not tolerate nausea, vomiting, or diarrhea well might be more inclined to use other hallucinogenic substances. Moreover, factor 3 was more strongly related to ayahuasca preference than factor 1. This finding intimates that the negative side effects of using ayahuasca might outweigh the positives when individuals consider using the substance again. Factor 2 (unpredictable or negative cognitive responses) did not significantly relate to individuals' proclivities toward ayahuasca or other hallucinogens, perhaps because it underscores the variable nature of all hallucinogenic substances, which might generate apprehension in users. These results intimate that we might be able to predict which hallucinogenic substance individuals will prefer in the future based on their previous experiences with ayahuasca.

In summary, the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale reveals distinct ways in which users' experiences with ayahuasca differ from more traditional hallucinogens. Participant reports mapped onto three main factors: a positive connection to nature and other people, unpredictable or negative cognitive responses, and physical reactions. These unique aspects of the ayahuasca experience map onto individuals' willingness to use the psychoactive tea again in the future over other hallucinogens. Future work with a more diverse sample, collateral reporters, and longitudinal follow-up could confirm these findings and add to their generalizability and validity. The creation of this scale might aid some users in determining whether they should try ayahuasca based on the experiences of others. For individuals who might seek out ayahuasca for medicinal, recreational, or perspective-raising purposes, the Novelty of Ayahuasca Scale might serve as an educational resource to help them decide if the psychoactive tea is the right substance for their intentions. Especially among those seeking relief from depression, anxiety, and trauma, the scale, in combination with the existent clinical literature, can allow these individuals to make an informed choice about the perceived pros and cons of using ayahuasca as a healing substance.

References

  • Bogenschutz, M. P., & Johnson, M. W. (2016). Classic hallucinogens in the treatment of addictions. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 64, 250258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carbonaro, T. M., Bradstreet, M. P., Barrett, F. S., MacLean, K. A., Jesse, R., Johnson, M. W., et al. (2016). Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: Acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12681278. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116662634.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Rucker, J., Day, C. M. J., Erritzoe, D., Kaelen, M., et al. (2016). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: An open-label feasibility study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(7), 619627. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30065-7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Christiansen, B. A., Smith, G. T., Roehling, P. V., & Goldman, M. S. (1989). Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 9399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.93.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Domínguez-Clavé, E., Soler, J., Elices, M., Pascual, J. C., Álvarez, E., de la Fuente Revenga, M., et al. (2016). Ayahuasca: Pharmacology, neuroscience, and therapeutic potential. Brain Research Bulletin, 126(1), 89101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2016.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frecska, E., Bokor, P., & Winkelman, M. (2016). The therapeutic potentials of ayahuasca: Possible effects against various diseases of civilization. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 7, 35. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2016.00035.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaher, R. M., & Simons, J. S. (2007). Evaluations and expectancies of alcohol and marijuana problems among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(4), 545. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.545.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glennon, R. A., Titeler, M., & McKenney, J. D. (1984). Evidence for 5-HT2 involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents. Life Sciences, 35(25), 25052511. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(84)90436-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldman, M. S., Brown, S. A., & Christiansen, B. A. (1987). Expectancy theory-thinking and drinking. In K. E. Leonard, & H. T. Blane (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcohol (181-226). New York: The Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, R. R., Hurwitz, E. S., Davis, A. K., Johnson, M. W., & Jesse, R. (2019). Survey of subjective “God encounter experiences”: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PLoS One, 14(4), e0214377. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214377.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grinspoon, L., & Bakalar, J. B. (1983). Psychedelic reflections. New York: Human Sciences Press.

  • Grunwell, J. N. (1998). Ayahuasca tourism in South America. Written for Anthropology of Tourism, 8(3), 5962. Found on MAPS Website: maps-fourm@maps.org.

  • Hamill, J., Hallak, J., Dursun, S. M., & Baker, G. (2018). Ayahuasca: Psychological and physiologic effects, pharmacology, and potential uses in addiction and mental illness. Current Neuropharmacology, 16(2), 108182. https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159X16666180125095902.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harris, R., & Gurel, L. (2012). A study of ayahuasca use in North America. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 44(3), 209215. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2012.703100.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayaki, J., Hagerty, C. E., Herman, D. S., de Dios, M. A., Anderson, B. J., & Stein, M. D. (2010). Expectancies and marijuana use frequency and severity among young females. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 9951000. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.017.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jacobs, B. L. (1987). How hallucinogen drugs work: Hallucinogen drugs appear to exert their effects by acting on a specific type of serotonin receptor in the brain. American Scientist, 75(4), 386392.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones, B. T., Corbin, W., & Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96, 5772. https://doi.org/10.1080/09652140020016969.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kavenská, V., & Simonová, H. (2015). Ayahuasca tourism: Participants in shamanic rituals and their personality styles, motivation, benefits and risks. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47(5), 351359. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2015.1094590.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kjellgren, A., Eriksson, A., & Norlander, T. (2009). Experiences of encounters with ayahuasca – “The vine of the soul”. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 41(4), 309315. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2009.10399767.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krebs, T. S., & Johansen, P. O. (2013). Over 30 million psychedelic users in the United States. F1000Research, 2, 98. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.2-98.v1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Labate, B. C., & Cavnar, C. (Eds.). (2014). Ayahuasca shamanism in the Amazon and beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Lawn, W., Hallak, J. E., Crippa, J. A., dos Santos, R., Porffy, L., Barratt, M. J., et al. (2017). Well-being, problematic alcohol consumption and acute subjective drug effects in past-year ayahuasca users: A large, international, self-selecting online survey. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1470014706. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14700-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, T., Litwin, G. H., & Metzner, R. (1963). Reactions to psilocybin administered in a supportive environment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 137, 561573.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. (2013). A psychotherapeutic view on therapeutic effects of ritual ayahuasca use in the treatment of addiction. MAPS Bulletin, 23(1), 3640.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. & Pazzi, A. L. (2014). Therapist and patient perspectives on ayahuasca- assisted treatment for substance dependence. In B. C. Labate, & C. Cavanar (Eds.), The therapeutic use of ayahuasca (133-152). Springer.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luna, L. E. (1986). Vegetalismo: Shamanism among the mestizo population of the Peruvian Amazon. Stockholm: University of Stockholm.

  • Luna, L. E. (2011). Indigenous and mestizo use of ayahuasca. An overview. In R. G. dos Santos (Ed.), The ethnopharmacology of ayahuasca (1-21). Kerala, India: Transworld Research Network.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. (2004). Clinical investigations of the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: Rationale and regulatory challenges. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 102, 111129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2004.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J., Towers, G. H., &Abbott, F. (1984). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors in South American hallucinogenic plants: Tryptamine and beta-carboline constituents of ayahuasca. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 10(2), 195223. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-841(84)90003-5.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McPherson, A., & Harris, L. M. (2015). Mindfulness, explicit and implicit expectancies in alcohol dependent and non-alcohol dependent adults. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 13(4), 457469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-014-9538-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 172175. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.172.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metrik, J., Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., McGeary, J., Cook, T. A. R., & de Wit, H., et al. (2009). Effectiveness of a marijuana expectancy manipulation: Piloting the balanced-placebo design for marijuana. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17(4), 217225. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016502.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metzner, R. (1999). Ayahuasca: Human consciousness and the spirits of nature. New York: Thunder's Mouth Press.

  • Metzner, R., Litwin, G. H., & Weil, G. M. (1965). The relation of expectation and mood to psilocybin reactions: A questionnaire study. Psychedelic Review, 5, 339.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mithoefer, M. C., Mithoefer, A. T., Feduccia, A. A., Jerome, L., Wagner, M., Wymer, J., et al. (2018). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) – assisted psychotherapy for post-traumatic stress disorder in military veterans, firefighters, and police officers: A randomized, double-blind, dose-response, phase 2 clinical trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(6), 486497. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30135-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morgan, C., McAndrew, A., Stevens, T., Nutt, D., & Lawn, W. (2017). Tripping up addiction: The use of psychedelic drugs in the treatment of problematic drug and alcohol use. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 13, 7176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.10.009.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Osório, F. L., Sanches, R. F., Macedo, L. R., dos Santos, R. G., Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2015). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A preliminary report. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 37(1), 1320. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2014-1496.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ot'alora, G. M., Grigsby, J., Poulter, B., Van Derveer, J. W., Giron, S. G., Jerome, L., et al. (2018). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy for treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized phase 2 controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(12), 12951307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881118806297.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Palhano-Fontes, F., Barreto, D., Onias, H., Andrade, K. C., Novaes, M. M., Pessoa, J. A., et al. (2019). Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 655663. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001356.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Urbano, G., Morte, A., Antonijoan, R., Montero, M., et al. (2001). Subjective effects and tolerability of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 154, 8595. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000606.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J., Valle, M., Urbano, G., Yritia, M., Morte, A., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2003). Human pharmacology of ayahuasca: Subjective and cardiovascular effects, monoamine metabolite excretion, and pharmacokinetics. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 306(1), 7383. https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.103.049882.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ross, S., Bossis, A., Guss, J., Agin-Liebes, G., Malone, T., Cohen, B., et al. (2016). Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 11651180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675512.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rucker, J. H., Jelen, L. A., Flynn, S., Frowde, K. D., & Young, A. H. (2016). Psychedelics in the treatment of unipolar mood disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12201229. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116679368.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanches, R. F., de Lima Osório, F., dos Santos, R. G., Macedo, L., Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2016). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A SPECT study. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 36(1), 7781. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G. (2013). Safety and side effects of ayahuasca in humans—An overview focusing on developmental toxicology. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 45(1), 6878. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2013.763564.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G., Balthazar, F. M., Bouso, J. C., & Hallak, E. C. (2016). The current state of research on ayahuasca: A systematic review of human studies assessing psychiatric symptoms, neuropsychological functioning, and neuroimaging. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12301247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116652578.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G., Bouso, J. C., Alcázar-Córcoles, M. A., & Hallak, J. E. C. (2018). Efficacy, tolerability, and safety of serotonergic psychedelics for the management of mood, anxiety, and substance-use disorders: A systematic review of systematic reviews. Expert Review of Clinical Pharmacology, 9, 888902. https://doi.org/10.1080/17512433.2018.1511424.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schafer, J. & Brown, S. A. (1991). Marijuana and cocaine effect expectancies and drug use patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(4), 558565.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmid, J. T. (2012). The myth of ayahuasca. The International Journal of Religion and Spirituality in Society, 3, 257.

  • Shalit, N., Rehm, J., & Lev-Ran, S. (2019). Epidemiology of hallucinogen use in the U.S. results from the national epidemiologic survey of alcohol and related conditions III. Addictive Behaviors, 89, 3543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.09.020.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shanon, B. (Ed.). (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the ayahuasca experience. New York: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Studerus, E., Gamma, A., Kometer, M., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PloS One, 7(2), e30800. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030800.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swanson, L. R. (2018). Unifying theories of psychedelic drug effects. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 172. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2018.00172.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thomas, G., Lucas, P., Capler, N. R., Tupper, K. W., & Martin, G. (2013). Ayahuasca-assisted therapy for addiction: Results from a preliminary observational study in Canada. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 6(1), 3042. https://doi.org/10.2174/15733998113099990003.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. (2008). The globalization of ayahuasca: Harm reduction or benefit maximization?. International Journal of Drug Policy, 19(4), 297303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2006.11.001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uthaug, M. V., van Oosouw, K., Kuypers, K. P. C., van Boxtel, M., Broers, N. J., Mason, N. L., et al. (2018). Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 235(10), 29792989. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wiecko, F. M., Thompson, W. E., & Parham, B. P. (2017). A high by any other name: Exploring the motivations for consumption of “legal highs”. Deviant Behavior, 38(5), 549560. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2016.1197034.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wilcox, R. R. (2013). New statistical procedures for the social sciences: Modern solutions to basic problems. Florence, KY: Psychology Press.

  • Winkelman, M. (2001). Alternative and traditional medicine approaches for substance abuse programs: A shamanic perspective. International Journal of Drug Policy, 12, 337351. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0955-3959(01)00100-1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Winkelman, M. (2011). Drug tourism or spiritual healing? Ayahuasca seekers in Amazonia. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 37(2), 209218. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2005.10399803.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bogenschutz, M. P., & Johnson, M. W. (2016). Classic hallucinogens in the treatment of addictions. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 64, 250258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carbonaro, T. M., Bradstreet, M. P., Barrett, F. S., MacLean, K. A., Jesse, R., Johnson, M. W., et al. (2016). Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: Acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12681278. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116662634.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Rucker, J., Day, C. M. J., Erritzoe, D., Kaelen, M., et al. (2016). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: An open-label feasibility study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(7), 619627. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30065-7.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Christiansen, B. A., Smith, G. T., Roehling, P. V., & Goldman, M. S. (1989). Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 9399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.93.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Domínguez-Clavé, E., Soler, J., Elices, M., Pascual, J. C., Álvarez, E., de la Fuente Revenga, M., et al. (2016). Ayahuasca: Pharmacology, neuroscience, and therapeutic potential. Brain Research Bulletin, 126(1), 89101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2016.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frecska, E., Bokor, P., & Winkelman, M. (2016). The therapeutic potentials of ayahuasca: Possible effects against various diseases of civilization. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 7, 35. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2016.00035.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gaher, R. M., & Simons, J. S. (2007). Evaluations and expectancies of alcohol and marijuana problems among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(4), 545. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.545.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Glennon, R. A., Titeler, M., & McKenney, J. D. (1984). Evidence for 5-HT2 involvement in the mechanism of action of hallucinogenic agents. Life Sciences, 35(25), 25052511. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(84)90436-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goldman, M. S., Brown, S. A., & Christiansen, B. A. (1987). Expectancy theory-thinking and drinking. In K. E. Leonard, & H. T. Blane (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alcohol (181-226). New York: The Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Griffiths, R. R., Hurwitz, E. S., Davis, A. K., Johnson, M. W., & Jesse, R. (2019). Survey of subjective “God encounter experiences”: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PLoS One, 14(4), e0214377. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214377.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grinspoon, L., & Bakalar, J. B. (1983). Psychedelic reflections. New York: Human Sciences Press.

  • Grunwell, J. N. (1998). Ayahuasca tourism in South America. Written for Anthropology of Tourism, 8(3), 5962. Found on MAPS Website: .

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hamill, J., Hallak, J., Dursun, S. M., & Baker, G. (2018). Ayahuasca: Psychological and physiologic effects, pharmacology, and potential uses in addiction and mental illness. Current Neuropharmacology, 16(2), 108182. https://doi.org/10.2174/1570159X16666180125095902.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harris, R., & Gurel, L. (2012). A study of ayahuasca use in North America. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 44(3), 209215. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2012.703100.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayaki, J., Hagerty, C. E., Herman, D. S., de Dios, M. A., Anderson, B. J., & Stein, M. D. (2010). Expectancies and marijuana use frequency and severity among young females. Addictive Behaviors, 35, 9951000. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.017.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jacobs, B. L. (1987). How hallucinogen drugs work: Hallucinogen drugs appear to exert their effects by acting on a specific type of serotonin receptor in the brain. American Scientist, 75(4), 386392.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones, B. T., Corbin, W., & Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96, 5772. https://doi.org/10.1080/09652140020016969.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kavenská, V., & Simonová, H. (2015). Ayahuasca tourism: Participants in shamanic rituals and their personality styles, motivation, benefits and risks. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47(5), 351359. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2015.1094590.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kjellgren, A., Eriksson, A., & Norlander, T. (2009). Experiences of encounters with ayahuasca – “The vine of the soul”. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 41(4), 309315. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2009.10399767.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Krebs, T. S., & Johansen, P. O. (2013). Over 30 million psychedelic users in the United States. F1000Research, 2, 98. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.2-98.v1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Labate, B. C., & Cavnar, C. (Eds.). (2014). Ayahuasca shamanism in the Amazon and beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Lawn, W., Hallak, J. E., Crippa, J. A., dos Santos, R., Porffy, L., Barratt, M. J., et al. (2017). Well-being, problematic alcohol consumption and acute subjective drug effects in past-year ayahuasca users: A large, international, self-selecting online survey. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1470014706. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14700-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, T., Litwin, G. H., & Metzner, R. (1963). Reactions to psilocybin administered in a supportive environment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 137, 561573.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. (2013). A psychotherapeutic view on therapeutic effects of ritual ayahuasca use in the treatment of addiction. MAPS Bulletin, 23(1), 3640.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Loizaga-Velder, A. & Pazzi, A. L. (2014). Therapist and patient perspectives on ayahuasca- assisted treatment for substance dependence. In B. C. Labate, & C. Cavanar (Eds.), The therapeutic use of ayahuasca (133-152). Springer.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Luna, L. E. (1986). Vegetalismo: Shamanism among the mestizo population of the Peruvian Amazon. Stockholm: University of Stockholm.

  • Luna, L. E. (2011). Indigenous and mestizo use of ayahuasca. An overview. In R. G. dos Santos (Ed.), The ethnopharmacology of ayahuasca (1-21). Kerala, India: Transworld Research Network.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J. (2004). Clinical investigations of the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: Rationale and regulatory challenges. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 102, 111129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2004.03.002.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McKenna, D. J., Towers, G. H., &Abbott, F. (1984). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors in South American hallucinogenic plants: Tryptamine and beta-carboline constituents of ayahuasca. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 10(2), 195223. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-841(84)90003-5.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McPherson, A., & Harris, L. M. (2015). Mindfulness, explicit and implicit expectancies in alcohol dependent and non-alcohol dependent adults. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 13(4), 457469. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-014-9538-6.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Meng, X. L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 172175. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.172.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metrik, J., Rohsenow, D. J., Monti, P. M., McGeary, J., Cook, T. A. R., & de Wit, H., et al. (2009). Effectiveness of a marijuana expectancy manipulation: Piloting the balanced-placebo design for marijuana. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17(4), 217225. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016502.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Metzner, R. (1999). Ayahuasca: Human consciousness and the spirits of nature. New York: Thunder's Mouth Press.

  • Metzner, R., Litwin, G. H., & Weil, G. M. (1965). The relation of expectation and mood to psilocybin reactions: A questionnaire study. Psychedelic Review, 5, 339.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mithoefer, M. C., Mithoefer, A. T., Feduccia, A. A., Jerome, L., Wagner, M., Wymer, J., et al. (2018). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) – assisted psychotherapy for post-traumatic stress disorder in military veterans, firefighters, and police officers: A randomized, double-blind, dose-response, phase 2 clinical trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(6), 486497. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30135-4.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morgan, C., McAndrew, A., Stevens, T., Nutt, D., & Lawn, W. (2017). Tripping up addiction: The use of psychedelic drugs in the treatment of problematic drug and alcohol use. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 13, 7176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.10.009.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Osório, F. L., Sanches, R. F., Macedo, L. R., dos Santos, R. G., Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2015). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A preliminary report. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 37(1), 1320. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2014-1496.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ot'alora, G. M., Grigsby, J., Poulter, B., Van Derveer, J. W., Giron, S. G., Jerome, L., et al. (2018). 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine-assisted psychotherapy for treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized phase 2 controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(12), 12951307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881118806297.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Palhano-Fontes, F., Barreto, D., Onias, H., Andrade, K. C., Novaes, M. M., Pessoa, J. A., et al. (2019). Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 49(4), 655663. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718001356.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Urbano, G., Morte, A., Antonijoan, R., Montero, M., et al. (2001). Subjective effects and tolerability of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology, 154, 8595. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002130000606.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riba, J., Valle, M., Urbano, G., Yritia, M., Morte, A., & Barbanoj, M. J. (2003). Human pharmacology of ayahuasca: Subjective and cardiovascular effects, monoamine metabolite excretion, and pharmacokinetics. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 306(1), 7383. https://doi.org/10.1124/jpet.103.049882.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ross, S., Bossis, A., Guss, J., Agin-Liebes, G., Malone, T., Cohen, B., et al. (2016). Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 11651180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675512.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rucker, J. H., Jelen, L. A., Flynn, S., Frowde, K. D., & Young, A. H. (2016). Psychedelics in the treatment of unipolar mood disorders: A systematic review. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12201229. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116679368.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sanches, R. F., de Lima Osório, F., dos Santos, R. G., Macedo, L., Maia-de-Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2016). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A SPECT study. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 36(1), 7781. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G. (2013). Safety and side effects of ayahuasca in humans—An overview focusing on developmental toxicology. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 45(1), 6878. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2013.763564.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G., Balthazar, F. M., Bouso, J. C., & Hallak, E. C. (2016). The current state of research on ayahuasca: A systematic review of human studies assessing psychiatric symptoms, neuropsychological functioning, and neuroimaging. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 12301247. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116652578.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • dos Santos, R. G., Bouso, J. C., Alcázar-Córcoles, M. A., & Hallak, J. E. C. (2018). Efficacy, tolerability, and safety of serotonergic psychedelics for the management of mood, anxiety, and substance-use disorders: A systematic review of systematic reviews. Expert Review of Clinical Pharmacology, 9, 888902. https://doi.org/10.1080/17512433.2018.1511424.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schafer, J. & Brown, S. A. (1991). Marijuana and cocaine effect expectancies and drug use patterns. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(4), 558565.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmid, J. T. (2012). The myth of ayahuasca. The International Journal of Religion and Spirituality in Society, 3, 257.

  • Shalit, N., Rehm, J., & Lev-Ran, S. (2019). Epidemiology of hallucinogen use in the U.S. results from the national epidemiologic survey of alcohol and related conditions III. Addictive Behaviors, 89, 3543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.09.020.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shanon, B. (Ed.). (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the ayahuasca experience. New York: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Studerus, E., Gamma, A., Kometer, M., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PloS One, 7(2), e30800. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030800.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Swanson, L. R. (2018). Unifying theories of psychedelic drug effects. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 172. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2018.00172.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thomas, G., Lucas, P., Capler, N. R., Tupper, K. W., & Martin, G. (2013). Ayahuasca-assisted therapy for addiction: Results from a preliminary observational study in Canada. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 6(1), 3042. https://doi.org/10.2174/15733998113099990003.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tupper, K. W. (2008). The globalization of ayahuasca: Harm reduction or benefit maximization?. International Journal of Drug Policy, 19(4), 297303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2006.11.001.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Uthaug, M. V., van Oosouw, K., Kuypers, K. P. C., van Boxtel, M., Broers, N. J., Mason, N. L., et al. (2018). Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology (Berlin), 235(10), 29792989. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wiecko, F. M., Thompson, W. E., & Parham, B. P. (2017). A high by any other name: Exploring the motivations for consumption of “legal highs”. Deviant Behavior, 38(5), 549560. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2016.1197034.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wilcox, R. R. (2013). New statistical procedures for the social sciences: Modern solutions to basic problems. Florence, KY: Psychology Press.

  • Winkelman, M. (2001). Alternative and traditional medicine approaches for substance abuse programs: A shamanic perspective. International Journal of Drug Policy, 12, 337351. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0955-3959(01)00100-1.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Winkelman, M. (2011). Drug tourism or spiritual healing? Ayahuasca seekers in Amazonia. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 37(2), 209218. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2005.10399803.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand

Editor-in-Chief:

Attila Szabo - University of Oslo

E-mail address: attilasci@gmail.com

Managing Editor:

Zsófia Földvári, Oslo University Hospital

 

Associate Editors:

  • Alexander De Foe, School of Educational Psychology and Counselling, Monash University, Australia
  • Zsolt Demetrovics - Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary
  • Ede Frecska, founding Editor-in-Chief - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • David Luke - University of Greenwich, London, UK
  • Dennis J. McKenna- Heffter Research Institute, St. Paul, USA
  • Jeremy Narby - Swiss NGO Nouvelle Planète, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • Rick Strassman - University of New Mexico School of Medicine, Albuquerque, NM, USA
  • Enzo Tagliazucchi - Latin American Brain Health Institute, Santiago, Chile, and University of Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • Michael Winkelman - Retired from Arizona State University, Tempe, USA 

Book Reviews Editor:

Michael Winkelman - Retired from Arizona State University, Tempe, USA

Editorial Board

  • Gábor Andrássy - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • Paulo Barbosa - State University of Santa Cruz, Bahia, Brazil
  • Michael Bogenschutz - New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
  • Petra Bokor - University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
  • Jose Bouso - Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • Zoltán Brys - Multidisciplinary Soc. for the Research of Psychedelics, Budapest, Hungary
  • Susana Bustos - California Institute of Integral Studies San Francisco, USA
  • Robin Carhart-Harris - Imperial College, London, UK
  • Per Carlbring - Stockholm University, Sweden
  • Valerie Curran - University College London, London, UK
  • Alicia Danforth - Harbor-UCLA Medical Center, Los Angeles, USA
  • Alan K. Davis - The Ohio State University & Johns Hopkins University, USA
  • Rick Doblin - Boston, USA
  • Tra-ill Dowie - Ikon Institute of Australia, Australia
  • Rafael G. dos Santos - University of Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Brazil
  • Genis Ona Esteve - Rovira i Virgili University, Spain
  • Silvia Fernandez-Campos
  • Zsófia Földvári - Oslo University Hospital, Oslo, Norway
  • Andrew Gallimore - University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
  • Neal Goldsmith - private practice, New York, NY, USA
  • Charles Grob - Harbor-UCLA Medical Center, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • Stanislav Grof - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • Karen Grue - private practice, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • Jiri Horacek - Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
  • Lajos Horváth - University of Debrecen, Debrecen, Hungary
  • Robert Jesse - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Matthew Johnson - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Eli Kolp - Kolp Institute New, Port Richey, FL, USA
  • Stanley Krippner - Saybrook University, Oakland, CA, USA
  • Evgeny Krupitsky - St. Petersburg State Pavlov Medical University, St. Petersburg, Russia
  • Rafael Lancelotta - Innate Path, Lakewood, CO, USA
  • Anja Loizaga-Velder - National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
  • Luis Luna - Wasiwaska Research Center, Florianópolis, Brazil
  • Katherine MacClean - Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Deborah Mash - University of Miami School of Medicine, Miami, USA
  • Friedericke Meckel - private practice, Zurich, Switzerland
  • Ralph Metzner - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • Michael Mithoefer - private practice, Charleston, SC, USA
  • Levente Móró - University of Turku, Turku, Finland
  • David Nichols - Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
  • David Nutt - Imperial College, London, UK
  • Torsten Passie - Hannover Medical School, Hannover, Germany
  • Janis Phelps - California Institute of Integral Studies, San Francisco, CA, USA
  • József Rácz - Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary
  • Christian Rätsch - University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
  • Sidarta Ribeiro - Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
  • William Richards - Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA
  • Stephen Ross - New York University, New York, NY, USA
  • Brian Rush - University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
  • Eduardo Schenberg - Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • Ben Sessa - Cardiff University School of Medicine, Cardiff, UK
  • Lowan H. Stewart - Santa Fe Ketamine Clinic, NM, USA (Medical Director)
  • Rebecca Stone - Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • Csaba Szummer - Károli Gáspár University of the Reformed Church, Budapest, Hungary
  • Julien Tempone-Wiltshire - Australian College of Applied Psychology, Australia
  • Manuel Torres - Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA
  • Luís Fernando Tófoli - University of Campinas, Campinas, Brazil State
  • Malin Uthaug - Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
  • Julian Vayne - Norwich, UK
  • Nikki Wyrd - Norwich, UK

Attila Szabo
University of Oslo

E-mail address: attilasci@gmail.com

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science ESCI
  • Biological Abstracts
  • BIOSIS Previews
  • APA PsycInfo
  • DOAJ
  • Scopus
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2024  
Scopus  
CiteScore  
CiteScore rank  
SNIP  
Scimago  
SJR index 0.54
SJR Q rank Q1

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 2.2
Rank by Impact Factor Q2 (Psychology, Multidisciplinary)
Journal Citation Indicator 0.89
Scopus  
CiteScore 2.5
CiteScore rank Q1 (Anthropology)
SNIP 0.553
Scimago  
SJR index 0.503
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Psychedelic Studies
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge €990
Subscription Information Gold Open Access
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%. 
   

Journal of Psychedelic Studies
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2016
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year

4

Founder Akadémiai Kiadó
Debreceni Egyetem
Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Károli Gáspár Református Egyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
H-4032 Debrecen, Hungary Egyetem tér 1.
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
H-1091 Budapest, Hungary Kálvin tér 9.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2559-9283 (Online)

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Dec 2024 0 81 14
Jan 2025 0 137 7
Feb 2025 0 164 18
Mar 2025 0 145 9
Apr 2025 0 62 14
May 2025 0 13 9
Jun 2025 0 0 0