Abstract
Empirical research supports the use of psychoactive drugs to augment psychotherapy for the treatment of mental illness. The use of such drugs, however, is still resisted by many in the medical community and this transfers to resistance in the wider community. The stigma of controversy surrounding the drugs, further enhanced by the stigma of mental illness, hinders community acceptance. We report an experiment to change community attitudes to endorse the benefits of such practice. Participants were given information about a client with post-traumatic stress disorder, with accompanying stigma of mental illness, or a physical condition (Parkinson's disease) with comorbid psychological symptoms, but without stigma. They received information about the diagnosis and the value of psychoactive drugs in the enhancement of therapy or this information with a manipulation to induce empathy. Analysis revealed higher empathy for the PTSD than for the Parkinson's patient. The elevation of empathy was further enhanced by the empathy manipulation. While there was higher agreement that PTSD clients were responsible for their own condition, there was a greater willingness to help, and this was further increased by empathy. Such conditions should be considered to encourage the general community to accept enhanced therapy for patients that can enhance endorsement by medical practitioners.
“The heresies of one period always become the orthodoxies of the next’. Margaret Thatcher.
Pollan (2018) and Read and Papaspyrou (2021) have demonstrated the utility of psychoactive drugs in the treatment of mental illness. While this research was curtailed several decades ago by scandals concerning the use of such drugs, there has recently been a resurgence of interest in their use in psychotherapy (Doblin, Christianson, Jermine, & Burgh, 2019). Psychological associations, for example, the Australian Association of Psychologists Inc (AAPi) has created an interest group to advocate for the support of such psychotherapy. This paper contributes to an understanding of a movement towards the use of psychoactive drugs in psychotherapy using social influence to alter negative attitudes in the community. The study is concerned with how social psychological processes can be used to change community attitudes towards controversial medical interventions, namely the evidence-based use of psychoactive drugs in psychotherapy that, while presently stigmatized, can facilitate the treatment of conditions that are a burden to mental health.
Mental health is defined as a sense of wellbeing and self-esteem that allows an individual to form positive relationships. Mental disorders make such achievements difficult, but public attitudes are not always supportive of intervention. However, a recent meta-analysis of attitudes towards treatments of mental illness (Angermeyer, van der Auwers, Carta, & Schomerus, 2017) has shown that the public is increasingly likely to support the use of psychotherapeutic treatment of mental disorders.
A continuing barrier to the implementation of therapy, however, is the existence of Mental Health Stigma (MHS) that prevent individuals from seeking help and the community from recognising that a person may be suffering from a mental illness. The concept of MHS is the labelling by an individual of their illness as a negative stereotype that leads an individual unwilling to adopt a mental health intervention over a physical health one. Three types of MHS have been identified. Social stigma is characterised by prejudicial attitudes of others towards the mentally ill, perceived stigma is characterised by the internalizing by the sufferer of their perceptions of discrimination (Jennings, Cheung, Jeffirs, Peasley, & Lee 2015; Link, Cullen, Struening, & Shrout, 1989) and self-stigma is associated with feelings of low self-worth as a function of seeking treatment (Skopp et al., 2012). MHS also exists within the medical profession (Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer, & Rowlands, 2000). An example of the obstacles that MHS places on individuals is that of military personnel returning from warzones, particularly with the rate of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD).
Post-traumatic stress disorder
Post-traumatic stress disorder is a debilitating anxiety disorder characterized by hyperarousal, intrusive re-experiencing of traumatic experiences and avoidance symptoms (emotional numbing and withdrawal). It can develop following exposure to a potentially traumatic event such as warfare, terrorism or other threats on a person's life and has been posited to be an outcome of exposure to the threat of COVID-19 (DSM-V). It is, typically, a chronic illness that is co-morbid with depression, anxiety, substance abuse and suicide (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, Merikangas, & Walters, 2005). Across a lifetime, it has been estimated that between 50 and 70% of people will experience a potentially traumatic event, and of this group, 15–25% will develop PTSD (Creamer, Burgess, & McFarlane, 2001).
Current pharmacological treatment options include administration of antidepressants. Meta-analyses, however, show an insignificant response rate to the medications greater than placebo and complete remission of symptoms in only 30% of patients. This emphasizes the need for more effective agents for PTSD, including psychological therapies (Foa, Keane, Friedman, & Cohen, 2009). However, existing psychological therapies are ineffective for between 35% and 50% of patients who enrol in clinical trials. With current treatments lacking effectiveness there is a need for the development of new treatments (Cloitre, Stolbach, Herman, van der Kolk, Pynoose, Wang & Petkova, 2009). Pharmacologically assisted psychotherapy has been shown to be beneficial.
MDMA-assisted psychotherapy
The use of psychoactive drugs, in particular clinical grade 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), has been shown to be beneficial in comparison with therapy alone (Mithoefer et al., 2018; Schenberg, 2018; Schweiger, Kahl, Klein, Sipos, & Schweiger, 2019). MDMA may catalyse therapeutic processing by allowing participants to stay emotionally engaged while revisiting traumatic experiences (Stolaroff, 2004). Participants can experience and express fear, anger and grief as part of the therapeutic process, with less likelihood of feeling overwhelmed by these emotions. Mithoefer et al. (2018) found a significant lasting beneficial effect of the MDMA-assisted therapy for PTSD symptoms. Two months following final treatment, this reduction in PTSD symptoms remained significantly lower for the treatment group than the placebo group. The effect sizes were between 1.1 and 2.8 standard deviations of difference between groups.
However, this drug remains illegal across the world, with a Schedule 9: Prohibited substance scheduling in Australia, under the Standard for the Uniform Scheduling of Medicines and Poisons (SUSMP) legislation. The substance may only be used for research purposes, with no current accepted medical value. There is, therefore, a double stigmatization. First, the stigma of a patient presenting with a mental illness and second, the stigma of a treatment by the association of it with a negatively stereotyped drug.
Some studies do indicate adverse effects, with neuroimaging showing that regular use may alter the brain's serotonergic system in a dose-dependent manner (Reneman, de Win, van der Brink, Booij, & den Heeten, 2006; Roberts, Jones, & Montgomery, 2016). Szigeti, Winstock, Erritzoe, and Maier (2018), however, found that these studies focused on unusually heavy MDMA use. These may overestimate the extent of serotonergic alterations. Such contraindications, however, do not apply in the case of controlled therapeutic intervention with a trained therapist. Physiological effects of MDMA administration during therapy, including elevations of blood pressure, pulse, and body temperature all return to baseline at the end of session. Side effects are typically resolved over a period of hours or days, and there are no serious drug-related adverse effects. In addition, success depends on the sensitivity and talent of the therapist who employs it (Grinspoon & Doblin, 2001) and for an enduring change the psychotherapeutic element is essential (Parrott, 2013; Schenberg, 2018). There is still significant resistance within the medical profession. A recent official submission to the Therapeutic Goods Administration (TGA) in Australia by the Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Psychiatrists (RANZCP) has been criticised by members of the College for misrepresenting the quality of research into the use of psychoactive drugs.
The present study is concerned with an educational psycho-social intervention to induce empathy with the patient to highlight the benefits of MDMA assisted psychotherapy. Attitudinal change towards the benefit of MDMA-assisted therapy will be compared people with a physical disease (Parkinson's disease, which affects the functioning of the central nervous system and which can, in one third of sufferers, result in symptoms of depression and anxiety, but without the stigma of a mental illness) and people with a mental health diagnosis of PTSD. Due to the widespread negative stereotyping and prejudice towards mental health sufferers, it is predicted that there will be greater positive attitudinal support for the use of MDMA in therapy for the PTSD patient following the empathy induction intervention, compared with a physical-health target group (namely sufferers from Parkinson's disease).
Attitudes and empathy induction
Empathy is broadly defined as “the reactions of one individual to the observed experiences of another” (Davis, 1983). Research has shown the benefit of building empathy in changing an already established attitude (Samra, Griffiths, Cox, Conroy, & Knight, 2013). An increase in empathy can reduce stereotypes of stigmatized groups by reducing blame attributed to individuals (Batson & Ahmad, 2009), or by reducing the perceived differences between the self and other (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Galinsky, Wang, & Ku, 2008). Empathy has been shown to be positively associated with more favourable attitudes towards stigmatized individuals and groups (e.g. Batson, Chang, Orr, & Rowland, 2002; Hodson, Choma, & Costello, 2009). While recent research has questioned the benefits of emotional empathy (Bloom, 2016), this study will test the value of an empathy induction to engage a rational process to understand the perspective of another person, not an automatic emotional response. Participants read an account by a person diagnosed with the relevant condition, detailing the symptoms and the daily problems of living with the condition, designed to highlight insight into the personal experience. Recipients of the information are then encouraged to approve the use of a stigmatised drug to enhance therapy for a patient group also stigmatised by the label of having a mental illness.
Method
Design
The study's design was a 2 (health condition) X 2 (informative intervention) between-subjects factorial design. The health condition had two levels, with the target condition being either PTSD or Parkinson's Disease (PD). The information intervention had two levels: Information alone (I) or a combination of Information with an Empathy manipulation (I+E). A control condition comprised a group who completed the dependent variable questionnaire for a client identified as having a broad stress related condition with no prior exposure to the independent information or empathy manipulations.
The dependent measures were attitudes and behavioural intention towards supporting the use of MDMA in an assisted psychotherapeutic intervention, as well as beliefs about Empathy, Social Distance, Personal Responsibility Beliefs, and Affective reactions (Fear, Pity, Anger, Helping Willingness, & Coercion Segregation) towards the target conditions.
Participants
People over the age of 18 were recruited online (Facebook and SONA) and randomly allocated to one of five conditions, four experimental conditions and a control group. Of the 82 participants who completed the questionnaire, 76 finished with complete data (93%). Four cases identified as influential outliers were removed, resulting in a final participant group of 72. Group compositions consisted of Control (n = 16), PTSD/I (n = 15), PTSD/I+E (n = 15), Parkinson's/I (n = 14), and Parkinson's/ I+E (n = 12).
Experimental conditions
Information
Participants were given a one-page essay defining and summarizing the health condition to which they had been randomly assigned. They were presented with an information sheet that outlined the potential benefits of MDMA-Assisted Psychotherapy for treatment-resistant PTSD based on recent study findings. The information sheet also listed potential negative side effects that these studies found, although these were described as mild and/or dissipated after a few days [again, based on recent study findings]. Importantly, the sheet also stated that we were referencing use of 'medical grade' MDMA administered by a trained therapist, and we were not advocating for personal use; noting that this is both dangerous to one-self and illegal in most countries around the world. The citation of peer reviewed articles was used to support each fact. The Information sheets for PTSD and Parkinson's were designed with Flesch-Kincaide readability grade levels of 9.5 and 9.6 respectively.
Empathy
The Empathy induction condition was adapted from a study testing the effects of narrative and evidence on individual attitudes towards target groups (Niederdeppe, Shapiro, & Porticella, 2011) designed to elicit cognitive empathy (i.e. knowing how someone else is thinking or feeling; Batson & Ahmad, 2009). Participants assigned to the empathy condition read a half page first-hand account of a sufferer recounting their condition, highlighting the struggles of everyday living and emphasizing the symptoms of the condition. These Empathy narrative accounts for PTSD and Parkinson's had readability grade levels of 4.5 and 5.8 respectively. Participants in this condition also read the one-page account of the potential benefits of MDMA-assisted therapy.
Measures
Participants were assessed on their attitudes towards drugs and drug usage using the questionnaire adapted from the Australian National Drug and Alcohol research centre's Review of Australian public opinion surveys on illicit drugs (2008).
Two scales were used to assess empathy. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1983) was used to assess participants' natural responsivity toward others as an individual difference measure of empathy and perspective-taking. Participants rated 14 adjective items describing their current emotional state, including five empathy items (i.e., sympathetic, soft-hearted, warm, compassionate, and tender) and eight distracters. These measures were used in previous studies (e.g. Gapinski, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2006).
To assess attitudinal social distance towards persons labelled with PTSD (mental disorder), or Parkinson's disease (Physical disorder), eight questions were used, adapted from Link, Phelan, Bresnahan, Stueve, and Pescosolido (1999). Questions included asking participants how willing they were to engage in various interactions with a person with PTSD or Parkinson's.
Affective reactions were measured through a questionnaire assessing participants' emotional reactions to an “ill person” (PTSD or Parkinson's), including both negative affect and sympathy/concern reactions, adapted from Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan, and Kubiak (2003).
A self-constructed questionnaire was used to measure attitudes towards the use of MDMA-assisted psychotherapy. Reliability analysis of the questionnaire revealed strong internal consistency with Cronbach's Alpha equal to 0.86.
Procedure
Participants were randomly assigned to a health condition. Each group was further randomly assigned to an intervention condition, Information or Information + Empathy. Participants in the control condition were only assigned to a generic ‘Stress’ based health condition, with no intervention. Participants in each condition were asked for demographic information, familiarity with mental & chronic physical illness, and were measured for their attitudes towards drug use.
Control group members were then measured for their empathy, attitudinal social distance, affective reactions, and their attitudes toward the use of MDMA-assisted therapy for a general ‘stress’ condition. Following this stage, participants in the experimental conditions received their intervention, either ‘information’ or ‘information + empathy”. Participants’ empathy, attitudinal social distance, affective reactions, and their attitudes toward the use of MDMA-assisted therapy were then assessed for their randomly assigned health condition.
All participants were then provided with an outline of the aims of the study, with information regarding the legality and dangers of use surrounding MDMA outside a controlled clinical setting without a trained therapist.
Results
Eight cases were discarded due to incomplete data and a further four outlying cases were discarded due to their influence on the model. Thirty-one per cent of the sample (N = 72), identified as male, 68% identified as female, while the remaining 1% identified as other (intersex). The mean age was 30.69 years (SD = 8.32).
Normality
Normality tests resulted in 6 cases being removed due to suspicious patterns of answering and due to outlier significance. Eight outliers remained for the dependant variable (MDMA Therapy Support), according to case wise diagnostics, but were deemed non-influential and kept in the data set.
Only one variable significantly correlated with MDMA therapy support; Approval of regular substance use positively correlated with MDMA therapy support (r = 0.59, P < 0.001).
Analysis of variance
Although normality assumptions were violated between variables, we proceeded with analysis of variance analyses due to their robust nature and to the power of ANOVA to detect differences. ANOVA also allows for the assessment and interpretation of interactions between variables. Interpretative caution was accounted for by setting a non-directional hypothesis level of significance at the P = 0.05 level and effect sizes are reported for each significant outcome report. A series of 2x2 Factorial ANOVAs tested for group differences between the four experimental conditions on the nine outcome variables (see Table 1).
Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for Each Group
Characteristic | MDMA Therapy Support | Empathy | Fear | PRB | Helping | Social Distance | Pity | Anger | CoSeg | n |
Control | 44.86 (11.74) | 16.00 (3.72) | 5.50 (2.76) | 8.13 (2.90) | 14.06 (2.41) | 17.81 (4.13) | 10.00 (2.34) | 4.56 (1.93) | 4.81 (2.23) | 16 |
PTSD I | 47.27 (7.87) | 17.20 (4.68) | 6.60 (1.92) | 4.53 (1.41) | 14.93 (3.31) | 17.13 (4.85) | 10.60 (2.56) | 3.93 (1.44) | 4.40 (0.63) | 15 |
PTSD I+E | 44.47 (9.50) | 19.13 (3.56) | 7.07 (2.43) | 6.40 (2.53) | 22.07 (4.38) | 14.47 (5.38) | 11.07 (2.60) | 4.07 (1.28) | 6.00 (3.46) | 15 |
Park I | 49.36 (3.95) | 14.79 (3.87) | 5.43 (3.65) | 3.79 (1.31) | 15.36 (3.08) | 15.57 (4.91) | 10.00 (2.96) | 3.93 (1.86) | 4.93 (2.43) | 14 |
Park I+E | 45.92 (6.68) | 10.67 (4.87) | 4.67 (0.89) | 4.42 (1.31) | 14.42 (4.68) | 14.25 (4.35) | 11.59 (2.50) | 3.50 (1.00) | 4.75 (1.29) | 12 |
Total Mean | 46.33 (8.49) | 15.78 (4.86) | 5.90 (2.61) | 5.56 (2.59) | 16.22 (4.65) | 15.94 (4.83) | 10.61 (2.59) | 4.03 (1.56) | 4.99 (2.27) | 72 |
R 2 | 0.045 | 0.315 | 0.107 | 0.402 | 0.431 | 0.088 | 0.054 | 0.048 | 0.060 |
N = 72.
Note: Measures are scored such that higher numbers indicate more of each construct. Reported R 2 represents the univariate ANOVA of each condition for each outcome measure. PRB = Personal Responsibility Beliefs; CoSeg = Coercion Segregation. PTSD = Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Park = Parkinson's disease.
MDMA therapy support
The main effect of Health condition on MDMA Therapy Support was non-significant, F (1, 52) = 0.80, P = 0.375, as were the main effect of Intervention, F (1, 52) = 2.49, P = 0.12 and the interaction, F (1, 52) = 0.03, P = 0.87. There were no significant differences between any of the experimental group means and the mean for the control condition. Overall, therefore, the interventions had no positive or negative impact on attitude to the use of psychoactive drugs in therapy. Analysis of the personal reactions to the interventions, however, does show meaningful variation in such reactions.
Empathy
The empathy manipulation was effective in the case of the PTSD group, with main effect of F (1, 52) = 22.75, P < 0.001, η2 p = 0.30 with empathy significantly higher for the PTSD group (M = 18.17, SD = 4.20) than for the Parkinson's group (M = 12.88, SD = 4.75). The interaction effect was significant, F (1, 52) = 7.04, P = 0.011, η2 p= 0.12. The PTSD Information + Empathy condition was significantly higher than control, which in turn was significantly greater than the mean for Parkinson's Information + Empathy. The effect sizes for the significant outcomes were all more than 0.1 and considered worthy of interpretation (Cohen, 1988).
Fear
There was a main effect of Health condition on Fear towards sufferers, F (1, 52) = 7.27, P = 0.009, η2 p = 0.12; fear of PTSD sufferers was significantly higher (M = 6.83, SD = 2.17) than for Parkinson's sufferers (M = 5.08, SD = 2.73). The effect of Intervention, F (1, 52) = 0.05, P = 0.82 and the interaction effect F (1, 52) = 0.86, P =0.36 were not significant.
Personal responsibility beliefs
All four experimental conditions significantly reduced the attribution of personal responsibility compared with the control condition. Within the experimental conditions there was a main effect of health condition on Personal Responsibility Beliefs, F (1, 52) = 8.49, P = 0.005, η2 p = 0.14; attribution of personal blame for their condition to the sufferer was significantly higher for the PTSD group (M = 5.47, SD = 2.22) than for the Parkinson's group (M = 4.08, SD = 1.32). The main effect of Intervention was significant, F (1, 52) = 7.10, P = 0.010, η2 p = 0.12. Attribution of personal responsibility for a sufferer's condition was significantly higher with the Empathy intervention (M = 5.52, SD = 2.28) than for Information alone (M = 4.17, SD = 1.39). The interaction effect was non-significant, F (1, 52) = 1.74, P = 0.193. The effect sizes are large enough to have clinical significance.
Helping
There was a main effect of Health condition on general helping intentions, F (1, 52) = 11.99, P = 0.001, η2 p = 0.19; willingness to help was higher for a PTSD sufferer (M = 18.50, SD = 5.26) than for a Parkinson's sufferer (M = 14.92, SD = 3.85). The main effect of Intervention was significant (F (1, 52) = 8.807, P = 0.005, η2 p = 0.15) with willingness to help a sufferer significantly higher with the empathy manipulation (M = 18.67, SD = 5.88) than information alone (M = 15.14, SD = 3.15). This effect was, however, modified by the significant interaction effect, F (1, 52) = 14.97, P < 0.001, η2 p = 0.22). For PTSD, Info + Empathy led to higher willingness to helping compared with all other experimental conditions. Note the large effect size reported for the interaction effect.
Discussion
Health condition or intervention type did not influence support for MDMA assisted therapy; there was a non-significant difference in MDMA assisted therapy support between the control condition and the four experimental conditions. However, ‘regular substance use approval’ was strongly associated with support for MDMA assisted therapy. This suggests the generalised acceptance of use of an already approved substance to another, especially if the use is beneficial.
The induction of empathy significantly affected reactions to the client in several ways. Respondents exposed to the PTSD condition, with the induction of empathy, displayed greater fear of and higher individual blame for the client's condition compared with the Parkinson's condition. These results support previous MHS studies, which have shown that personal responsibility and fear are attributed to sufferers of mental health (Crisp et al., 2000; Jennings et al., 2015; Link et al., 1989, 1999). Evoking empathy can emphasize the negative aspects and increase bias towards targets or groups (Batson & Ahmad, 2009). Alternatively, Galinsky, Ku, and Wang (2005) have suggested that empathic perspective-taking can increase stereotypes by association.
However, the finding that the empathy manipulation significantly increased the willingness of participants to help PTSD sufferers compared with Parkinson's sufferers was particularly interesting. Previous studies would suggest that MHS would have negative impact upon any attitudes towards suffers of mental health (Crisp et al., 2000; Jennings et al., 2015; Link et al., 1989, 1999), and predict a greater empathy effect for Parkinson's over PTSD. This willingness occurred regardless of the heightened negativity towards PTSD sufferers, measured in reactions of fear and blame. This finding is contrary to any previous findings on the effects of MHS (Crisp et al., 2000; Jennings et al., 2015; Link et al., 1989, 1999). The association of the use of drugs with psychotherapy together with the induction of empathy creates a context whereby the use of such drugs is perceived by the participants to be of potential benefit to clients.
A final note relates to the use of participants recruited to complete the study online. This methodology has become much more prevalent in recent years and the research which has been conducted to compare online participation compared to face-to-face has demonstrated that any differences in the outcomes that are assessed are small and insignificant (Batterham, 2014). Online recruitment has the additional benefit of enabling access to broader sections of the population which is especially beneficial in studies such as then present one that are concerned with the assessment of attitudes within the broader community than may be achievable in laboratory academic environments.
Summary
Attitudes towards MDMA assisted therapy are positive in the present study, regardless of the health condition being treated. Negative affective reactions towards patients previously associated with stigma (mental health patients) were reversed by the induction of cognitive empathy with an associated increase in willingness to help such patients. The use of empathy to influence community attitudes to the use of psychoactive drugs in the treatment of mental illness compared to conditions not associated with mental illness, would therefore seem indicated. This reinforces a case for inducing a rational form of empathic concern in health-related matters (Bloom, 2016). This research should be viewed as conducive to the facilitation of change in policy through community action within an emerging debate about the use of psycho-active drugs (Pollan, 2018). The debate within medical circles may also be affected by an emphasis upon an understanding of the perspectives of the patients through the manipulation of empathy in the training of medical practitioners.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare there are no conflicts of interest and no financial or sponsorship links with any organisation.
References
Angermeyer, M. C. , van der Auwera, S. , Carta, M. G. , & Schomerus, G. (2017). Public attitudes towards psychiatry and psychiatric treatment at the beginning of the 21st century: A systematic review and meta-analysis of population surveys. World Psychiatry, 16(1), 50–61.
Batson, C. D. , & Ahmad, N. Y. (2009). Using empathy to improve intergroup attitudes and relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 3, 141–177.
Batson, C. D. , Chang, J. , Orr, R. , & Rowland, J. (2002). Empathy, attitudes, and action: Can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group motivate one to help the group? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(12), 1656–1666.
Batterham, P. J. (2014). Recruitment of mental health survey participants using Internet advertising: Contents, characteristics and cost effectiveness. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 23(2), 184–191.
Bloom, P. (2016). Against empathy: The case for rational compassion. New York: Harpercollins.
Cloitre, M. , Stolbach, B. C. , Herman, J. L. , van der Kolk, B. , Pynoos, R. , Wang, J. , & Petkova, E. (2009). A developmental approach to complex PTSD. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22(5), 399–408.
Cohen, J. W. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Rutgers: Erlbaum.
Corrigan, P. , Markowitz, F. E. , Watson, A. , Rowan, D. , & Kubiak, M. A. (2003). An attribution model of public discrimination towards persons with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44, 162–179.
Creamer, M. , Burgess, P. , & McFarlane, A. C. (2001). Post-traumatic stress disorder: Findings from the Australian national survey of mental health and well-being. Psychological Medicine, 31(07), 1237–1247.
Crisp, A. H. , Gelder, M. G. , Rix, S. , Meltzer, H. I. , & Rowlands, O. J. (2000). Stigmatisation of people with mental illness. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(1), 4–7.
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.
Doblin, R. E. , Christianson, M. , Jermine, L. , & Burgh, B. (2019). The past and future of psychedelic science: An introduction to this issue. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 51, 93–97.
Foa, E. B. , Keane, T. M. , Friedman, M. J. , & Cohen, J. A. (2009). Effective treatments for PTSD, practice guidelines from the international society for traumatic stress studies (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Galinsky, A. D. , Ku, G. , & Wang, C. S. (2005). Perspective-taking and self-other overlap: Fostering social bonds and facilitating social coordination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8, 109–124.
Galinsky, A. D. , & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 708.
Galinsky, A. D. , Wang, C. S. , & Ku, G. (2008). Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(2), 404.
Gapinski, K. D. , Schwartz, M. B. , & Brownell, K. D. (2006). Can television change anti-fat attitudes and behavior? Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 11, 1–28.
Grinspoon, L. , & Doblin, R. (2001). Psychedelics as catalysts of insight-oriented psychotherapy. Social Research, 68(3), 67–95.
Hodson, G. , Choma, B. L. , & Costello, K. (2009). Experiencing alien-nation: Effects of a simulation intervention on attitudes toward homosexuals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 974–978.
Jennings, K. S. , Cheung, J. H. , Britt, T. W. , Goguen, K. N. , Jeffirs, S. M. , Peasley, A. L. , & Lee, A. C. (2015). How are perceived stigma, self-stigma, and self-reliance related to treatment-seeking? A three-path model. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 38(2), 109–116.
Kessler, R. C. , Chiu, W. T. , Demler, O. , Merikangas, K. R. , & Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSMIV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 617–627.
Link, B. G. , Cullen, F. T. , Struening, E. , & Shrout, P. E. (1989). A modified labelling theory approach to mental disorders: An empirical assessment. American Sociological Review, 54(3), 400–423.
Link, B. G. , Phelan, J. C. , Bresnahan, M. , Stueve, A. , & Pescosolido, B. A. (1999). Public conceptions of mental illness: Labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1328–1333.
Mithoefer, M. C. , Mithoefer, A. T. , Feduccia, A. A. , Wagner, M. , Wymer, J. , Holland, J. , et al. (2018). 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetmine (MDMA)-assisted psychotherapy for post-traumatic stress disorder in military veterans, firefighters, and police officers: A randomised, double-blind, dose-response phase 2 clinical trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 5(6), 486–497. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30135-4.
Niederdeppe, J. , Shapiro, M. A. , & Porticella, N. (2011). Attributions of responsibility for obesity: Narrative communication reduces reactive counterarguing among liberals. Human Communication Research, 37(3), 295–323.
Parrott, A. C. (2013). Human psychobiology of MDMA or ‘ecstasy’: An overview of 25 years of empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology, 28(4), 289–307.
Pollan, M. (2018). How to change your mind: The new science of psychedelics. London: Allen Lane.
Read, T. , & Papaspyrou, M. (2021). Psychedelics and psychotherapy: The healing potential of expanded states. Park Street Press.
Reneman, L. , de Win, M. M. , van den Brink, W. , Booij, J. , & den Heeten, G. J. (2006). Neuroimaging findings with MDMA/ecstasy. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 20, 164–175.
Roberts, C. A. , Jones, A. , & Montgomery, C. (2016). Meta-analysis of molecular imaging of serotonin transporters in ecstasy/polydrug users. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 63, 158–167.
Samra, R. , Griffiths, A. , Cox, T. , Conroy, S. , & Knight, A. (2013). Changes in medical student and doctor attitudes toward older adults after an intervention. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 61, 1188–1196.
Schenberg, E. E. (2018). Psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy: A paradigm shift in psychiatric research and development. Frontiers in Pharmacology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2018.00733.
Schweiger, J. I. , Kahl, K. G. , Klein, J. P. , Sipos, V. , & Schweiger, U. (2019). Innovation in psychotherapy, challenges, and opportunities: An opinion paper. Frontiers in Psychology 19 March 2019. https://frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00495/full.
Skopp, N. A. , Bush, N. E. , Vogel, D. L. , Wade, N. G. , Sirotin, A. P. , McCann, R. A. , & Metzger‐Abamukong, M. J. (2012). Development and initial testing of a measure of public and self‐stigma in the military. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(9), 1036–1047.
Stolaroff, M. (2004). The secret chief Revealed: Conversations with a pioneer of the underground therapy movement. Sarasota, FL: Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.
Szigeti, B. , Winstock, A. R. , Erritzoe, D. , & Maier, L. J. (2018). Are ecstasy induced serotonergic alterations overestimated for the majority of users? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(7), 741–748.