Abstract
As the changeful history of psychedelic research and therapy since the 1950s can be assumed to be well enough known to this journal's audience, it is only recalled briefly in this article. Rarely has it been addressed, though, that the discursive struggles through which this history has been spawned have antecedents in much older controversies in the 18th and 19th centuries. As knowledge of this prehistory may shed new light on the current state of affairs in psychedelic matters, this article starts with an account of the transformations that animal magnetism underwent during the so-called ‘long century of mediumship’, transformations by which it was ultimately tamed by science. Based on this historical sensitization, the text then turns to a recent strand of the scientific debate on psychedelic therapy – to seminal journal articles from the Johns Hopkins research group – and subjects these articles to a fine-grained discourse analysis. In doing so, the inner cohesion of the forces that are currently tugging at the psychedelic, mostly pushing aside its emancipatory, resistive, utopian, and ‘spiritual’ connotations, becomes more apparent.
Introduction
Since Western science had taken notice of psychedelics in mid-20th century, their therapeutic and societal potential has been valued quite differently. After a brief period in the 1950s and 1960s during which LSD, not subject to any specific restrictions at the time, was primarily used in psychiatric contexts—where it showed considerable promise and was discussed with little controversy—, it entered the ‘Flower Power’ and anti-war movements of the early 1960s and became a party and counterculture drug. In response, the US government under President Nixon declared drug abuse as public enemy number one and initiated a wave of repression, not least geared toward psychedelics, that quickly gained international traction. Scientific research came to a halt, as did public discourse on the potentials and applications of psychedelics. With few exceptions, the therapeutic use of psychedelics went underground. For the generation of therapists affected by this prohibition, this was tantamount to a clearcutting of their legal therapeutic options. All the more surprising was the “psychedelic renaissance” (Sessa, 2017) that began to emerge, first within scientific circles, at the turn of the millennium, then quickly accelerating and gaining popularity. It became widely recognized especially after Michael Pollan's bestselling book How to Change Your Mind (2018) and a Netflix documentary series of the same name, also masterminded by Pollan.
The conditions under which this psychedelic renaissance is taking place, however, are markedly different from those of the first psychedelic phase of the 1950s and 1960s, to which the term renaissance refers in this case. Pro-psychedelic activists no longer confront the powers that be head-on; instead, they have adapted their strategies to existing scientific and legal frameworks and to the neoliberal zeitgeist. Psychedelic therapy has by now become a well-nigh research hype, attracting substantial public and private funding. In Europe and especially in North America, it is drawing significant entrepreneurial interest. Some key substances, such as MDMA and psilocybin, appear to be on the verge of approval in the US and Europe for the treatment of major psychiatric conditions. There are already no legal barriers to the use of ketamine—a decades-old, medically approved anesthetic—for psychedelic therapy, at least under compassionate use and named patient use protocols. The psychedelic landscape, thus, has fundamentally changed within a few years. The countercultural, utopian, and spiritual ambitions characteristic of the 1960s are increasingly coming into tension with the functional logic of pharmacology and health care systems, and with entrepreneurial interests.
Before addressing these tensions, though, I look further back in time, as sensitizations for understanding the current shifts in the psychedelic field can be gained from examining a historical controversy that anteceded some of these dynamics: the debate over animal magnetism and mediumship (‘Mediumismus’) between approximately 1770 and 1900. During this period, mediumship emerged as a “boundary object” (Star & Griesemer, 1989) between secular and religious, scientific and magical, medical and aesthetic interests and groups; it was also in this context that our modern concept of the medium was formed in the first place (Hahn & Schüttpelz, 2009; Schüttpelz, 2012).
Sensitization: A look back on animal magnetism and mediumship
At the beginning of this long century of mediumship, tensions are already evident that resurface in the current debate over psychedelics. Perhaps few figures are more misrepresented by today's medical and psychological history of knowledge than Franz Anton Mesmer (1734–1815), who gained fame in the 1770s in Vienna for his practice of animal magnetism (Ellenberger, 1970). As a doctor, he had become familiar with magnetic treatments that were common at the time, but then he sensed—or believed he sensed—that the healing effect was not actually tied to the magnets themselves. Instead, he thought that the entire universe was filled with a ‘vital fire,’ which also extended into human and animal bodies (hence the term animal magnetism) and that people become ill when they are not sufficiently permeated by this universal energy (today, we might say: when the Chi no longer flows). Mesmer believed that he could channel this energy through his bare hands, without direct physical contact to his patients who frequently fell into states of ecstasy and felt spiritually cleansed after the treatment.
Mesmer entirely dispensed with the idea that he would exorcise evil spirits or the devil. The fact that his understanding of animal magnetism was almost antithetical to this becomes particularly evident in his conflict with Father Johann Josef Gaßner, a famous “devil banisher” in southern Germany at the time. Gaßner also elicited cathartic reactions during his exorcisms, but did so within the framework of Catholic exorcism rites and prayers—something that would never have occurred to Mesmer. When Father Gaßner's successes began to unsettle the Catholic authorities, the Elector of Bavaria appointed a commission in 1775 to investigate the events—and Mesmer as a member of it. Before this commission, Mesmer was able to demonstrate that he could induce healing crises just as effectively as Gaßner and that his explanation—attributing the effect to a natural physical force, namely his animal magnetism—was more plausible than attributing it to the exorcism ritual. In the discursive struggle over the causes of the magnetic catharsis, this marked a turning point where the religiously inclined among its proponents started to find themselves on the defensive.
By the late 1770s, Mesmer had become a full-fledged superstar in Paris. Contributing to his rise was his ability to expand his method to a group setting. Mesmer discovered—or believed he had discovered—that the energy he channeled could also be stored. To achieve this, he used a kind of wooden vat, in which he collected the animal magnetic “fluidum” in a mixture of water and iron filings. Wires or metallic rods protruded from the vat, allowing multiple people—sometimes dozens—to be connected to the fluid (Slunecko & Benetka, 2023, 125f). Among those gathered around Mesmer's baquet for séances who were magnetically infused together, utopian ideas of a life in freedom and social harmony occasionally emerged on the eve of the French Revolution—ideas that aligned with Mesmer's revolutionary outlook. However, the scientific community remained skeptical: A commission of the French Academy of Sciences appointed by King Louis XVI was tasked with evaluating Mesmer's magnetic therapy and Mesmer was ambitious or confident enough in his own abilities to agree to this examination. The key moment occurred in 1784 when Mesmer failed to convince the commission of the validity of animal magnetism. None of the Academy members gathered around the magnetized vat felt any effect. Even a patient who was convinced of the procedure couldn't distinguish Mesmer's fluid from ordinary water in a blind test (probably, the first documented in the history of science). Without direct rapport with the therapist, no effect occurred. It was only when the magnetist himself handed the cup with the fluid to the patient that the ecstatic healing trance set in (Schmidbauer, 2012).
From this failed demonstration, a question arises that seems essential for the discourse on psychedelics, too: How to account for effects that only unfold in particular situations and settings or within meaningful relationships? If such qualities are intrinsic to a procedure (be it animal magnetism or psychedelic therapy), its proponents must be utterly careful when facing demands for scientific validation, as long as such validation is equated—as it is the hallmark of experimental science—with decontextualization and impersonalization. Or better yet, as we will see later: proponents of such procedures better themselves take control of the verification process and, consequently, of the kind of decontextualization that goes along with it!
In the figure of Mesmer, we can also feel other tensions that are crucial for understanding the current development of psychedelic therapy. Can this therapy align itself with the “powers that be,” particularly those in science? And related to this question: What is its take on religion? Is the psychedelic perhaps the seed of a (renewed) religion or metaphysics? Mesmer's stance towards religion was, as mentioned, strictly antithetical: He saw himself as an Enlightenment thinker and understood animal magnetism as a natural force, but this is not true for some of his followers, who— like Swedenborg—transformed animal magnetism into a kind of mystical religion. And finally: Does the healing success rest on figments of imagination—on suggestion—, as the commission of the French Academy, without questioning the healing success itself, concluded regarding Mesmer's animal magnetism? In other words, is the subjective component the central agent, or is it a negligible, if not undesirable, epiphenomenon?
The negative verdict of the French Academy of Sciences regarding Mesmer's explanatory model did not hinder the long-term spread of his ideas. His most prominent successor, the Marquis de Puységur, discovered—or believed he discovered—that trees could also be charged, or were already charged, with magnetism and could distribute this life energy. With Puységur, a certain taming of the practice began, replacing the dramatic, ecstatic catharses with “calm crises” or “magnetic sleep.” From there, it is only a short step to hypnosis—a term that may seem to come directly from antiquity but actually first appeared in the mid-19th century with James Braid (1795–1860); Mesmer and Puységur had not yet used it. The central role of rapport in hypnosis, that is, the state of nonverbal connection between the hypnotist and the hypnotized, and the induction of “somnambulistic” states of consciousness for healing purposes are direct inheritances from Mesmer's approach. However, the astrological references that were still present in Mesmer's work were discarded in the development of hypnosis.
Freud encountered hypnosis already in a domesticated form—grounded in psychophysiology (monotonous sensory stimulation causes a lowering of the level of consciousness and, consequently, a reduction in the resistance of the ego-consciousness to suggestions), entirely focused on individuals, stripped of social-utopian connotations, teachable and learnable (i.e., not reliant on charisma)—through Hippolyte Bernheim (1840–1919), whose book on suggestion (Bernheim, 1889) he translated. During his study stay in Paris in 1885/86, Freud also witnessed the hypnotic trances induced by Jean-Martin Charcot (1825–1893). Eventually, however, Freud turned away from hypnosis which he initially had practiced, opting for a more civilized form, a form better suited to his bourgeois practice, but still involving non-ordinary and at times even trance-like states of consciousness: the transference relationship and its analysis (Slunecko & Benetka, 2023, 134f.). This, too, as evidenced by the centrality of the rapport between analyst and analysand, is still a descendant of Mesmer's practice.
It is thus a relay of techniques, terminologies, and testing events that accompanied animal magnetism and mediumship in the 18th and 19th centuries. In this process, variants of it were developed that could be lastingly institutionalized and, like psychoanalysis, have become and remained part of our therapeutic world. The development trajectory of animal magnetism and mediumship, only roughly and somewhat pointillistically sketched here (for more details, see Schüttpelz, 2012), however, offers more than a general sense of the historical depth from which discourses are charged. As epistemic objects, magnetism and mediumship are close relatives of psychedelic approaches, such that some of the struggles that surround them—the relationship to the religious, the tendency towards decontextualization under testing conditions, the move towards demystification and individualization, and the question of the status of the patient's experience—also sensitize us to the current discoursive battle over psychedelics.
Zooming in
Against this background, I will now subject a segment of the scientific discourse on psychedelics to a much more fine-grained analysis. I thereby draw on material that is more extensively documented in Grzegorczyk (2022) and has also been accessed by Slunecko (in press). The material consists of a cohesive series of highly influential scientific studies from one of the world's leading contemporary research groups on psychedelic therapy at Johns Hopkins University in Baltimore, led by the late psychopharmacologist Roland Griffiths. Starting in the late 1990s, Griffiths here initiated a continuously productive research program to study the effects of psilocybin (and later, other psychedelic substances), which has since garnered global attention. At that time, Griffiths was already an internationally recognized expert in the field of psychopharmacology, serving as a consultant to the WHO and the American NIH, with a distinguished track record, particularly in studies on the effects and withdrawal of caffeine, nicotine, and alcohol.
Griffiths, Richards, McCann, and Jesse (2006)
Based on this reputation, his credibility, and likely not least due to his encompassing friendliness, Griffiths successfully reintroduced psychedelic research—and with it, talk of mystical experiences—into key publications within his field, seemingly without encountering significant resistance. The title of the first major article by Griffiths et al. (2006), published in the journal Psychopharmacology and immediately recognized as a “watershed event” (Schuster, 2006, 289) in psychedelic research, already encapsulates the defining interest of the Baltimore research group: “Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance.” Mystical experiences—to be precise, the claim is somewhat tempered with the term “mystical-type”—and their personal and spiritual significance are the central focus of interest. While the research, in principle, follows the pharmacological template of a double-blind study, pharmacological or natural-scientific parameters interestingly play almost no role in it. The study does include information on the dosage of psilocybin administered, and blood pressure and heart rate are measured during the sessions—likely as part of a safety monitoring protocol—, but these parameters are not further reported on. The focus is placed elsewhere: the behavior (e.g., laughing, crying, yawning, spontaneous movements, dancing, responsiveness to questions, etc.) and mood of the participants during the sessions are recorded and rated by the session guides. Additionally, so-called community observers—individuals from the participants' real-life environments—evaluate behavioral and attitudinal changes in the participants over the two months following the psilocybin administration. The most important tools, however, are questionnaires that participants complete immediately after the psychedelic session and again two months later: the Pahnke–Richards Mystical Experience Questionnaire, the Mysticism Scale, and the Spiritual Transcendence Scale. Using these questionnaires, Griffiths et al. (2006) document the occurrence of “mystical-type experiences” during the sessions and a measurable increase in spirituality even in the medium term. Setting aside the fundamental paradox of this research strategy (that questionnaires necessarily address the ego as the evaluating entity, i.e., the very ‘strong subject’ that is supposedly ‘dissolved’ in the mystical experience), these questionnaires, some of which date back to the pioneering psychedelic research of the 1960s, come with a crucial ‘advantage’ from a research economy perspective: They allow to operationalize and quantify such experiences with minimal effort, as they eliminate the researcher's need to interpret each time what experience qualifies as a mystical one, or to reflect on one's own understanding of such experience. Instead, within a short period, a numerical value—a score—is generated based on what a participant checks off on a questionnaire. Mystical experience, thus, is what questionnaires for mystical experience measure. In this way, the subjective experience disappears behind a curtain, only to reemerge in a mathematized form that, if not outright mistaken for nature, can at least be easily correlated with naturalistic parameters (e.g., dosage information) or similarly ‘naturalized’ metrics, such as the anxiety or depression scores discussed later.
The focus on mystical and spiritual experiences, as well as the shift of epistemic authority regarding psychedelic effects from researchers to participants, session guides, and individuals from their social environment, is not typical in a journal of psychopharmacology. However, it is introduced by a recognized key figure in the field and is presented in a form that is quantifiable and thus compatible with natural science. This self-imposed naturalization and the associated decontextualization of experience seem to have been sufficient to make “mystical-type experiences” acceptable within the realm of psychopharmacology.
Griffiths et al. (2016)
While the participants in the 2006 study were ‘healthy’ volunteers (with a religious background), in the study by Griffiths et al. (2016) the focus shifts to whether psilocybin can reduce depression and anxiety in cancer patients. This transition—from healthy volunteers in everyday settings to patients in hospital settings and, along with it, the adoption of psychiatric diagnoses such as anxiety and depression—reflects a central trend in psychedelic research: its progressive alignment with the healthcare system and its dispositifs and devices. Patients with life-threatening or terminal cancer, like those in Griffiths et al. (2016), represent a particularly significant population from both an ethical and strategic research perspective. With them, under the principle of ‘compassionate use,’ new treatments can be more easily explored when all existing options fall short.
Griffiths et al. (2016) once again adhere to the pharmacological gold standard of the double-blind study. However, this time they employ a cross-over design, meaning that all participants receive the full or high dose at some point during the study, with half receiving it only in the second session. To minimize expectancy effects, the placebo is replaced with a minimal dose of psilocybin. Changes in heart rate and blood pressure are documented much more precisely than in Griffiths et al. (2006), further aligning the study with conventional medical presentation standards. As in the previously referenced study, both session guides and community observers (such as family members, friends, or colleagues) assess aspects like inner peace, patience, mental flexibility, and optimism in the participants; all these parameters show a positive impact.
Once again, questionnaire-based self-assessments demonstrate an increase in spirituality or maturity (sic!) of faith, as well as the occurrence of mystical experiences. The occurrence of these mystical experiences during a psilocybin session, in turn, is as a predictor of long-term positive changes in attitudes, mood, various aspects of psychosocial functioning, and spirituality. New to this study, compared to the one from 2006, are the instruments used to measure depression and anxiety. These are assessed through clinical standard measures that have been in use for decades: the Hamilton Scales for Depression and Anxiety. In this case, these scales demonstrate a significant and lasting antidepressant and anxiolytic effect from the psilocybin sessions, persisting for several months.
The article approaches the crucial question of causal relationships only at the end, and with caution, after presenting an impressive array of tables and significance calculations. This heavy reliance on statistical data makes the text, unlike the 2006 study, challenging for readers who are not used to such medical or natural-scientifically presentation format. The results would anyway suggest that the occurrence of mystical experiences is crucial for the success of the treatment: The more intensely someone has mystical experiences under psilocybin, i.e., reports them on the corresponding questionnaire, the greater the likelihood that their anxiety and depression scores will decrease—effects that remain detectable even months after the session. The crucial question here, as always with correlational relationships, is: Are these mystical experiences causally responsible for the desired therapeutic outcomes, or are they merely epiphenomena that accompany the actual pharmacological effects of psilocybin—side effects that (currently) cannot be avoided? At this point, Griffiths et al. (2016) propose a model in which mystical experience acts as a mediating variable between psilocybin administration and improved mood, reduced anxiety, etc. They hypothesize, and propose to statistically test—the study design of Griffiths et al. (2016) does not yet allow for this testing—, how much of the causation of the desired therapeutic effects can be attributed to mystical experiences. This represents a significant innovation for psychopharmacology with strong implications for the further use of psilocybin, as such use would need to focus on creating conditions that facilitate mystical-type experiences.
Davis et al. (2021)
Let's now consider some of the shifts evident in the study by Davis et al. (2021) in comparison. In this study, Griffiths is not the first author but the last, which, according to contemporary conventions in the natural sciences, indicates that the study originates from his research group, i.e., from the Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research at the Johns Hopkins School of Medicine in Baltimore which Griffiths was leading at the time and to which all the other authors of the study are affiliated. The fact that Griffiths is listed alongside Davis as one of the two corresponding authors further underscores his central role and justifies to draw on this study for further comparative analysis. While the study by Griffiths et al. (2016) did not mention therapy, the focus in Davis et al. (2021), as indicated by the title, is on the “effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder.” This suggests that psilocybin would assist some kind of therapy, with the therapy itself being the primary or leading element. However, what exactly is meant by therapy remains unclear. At one point, the text refers to “psychological support,” and at another, to “supportive psychotherapy.” However, calling it psychotherapy might be an overstatement. What actually happens? Each participant receives a total of eight hours of preparatory meetings with the two individuals who will accompany them during the two psilocybin sessions, conducted two months apart. Additionally, there are two to three hours of follow-up meetings, known as “postsession integration meetings,” held after each psilocybin session. Regarding the number of hours and the configuration (preparation, psychedelic sessions, integration meetings), the therapeutic design closely resembles a treatment template already widely used at the time for MDMA-assisted psychotherapy with post-traumatic stress disorder (Mithoefer, 2013). The accompanying individuals, called facilitators, are identified as a mix of students, psychologists, psychiatrists, and social workers. However, there is no information provided about their specific psychotherapeutic competencies, nor is there a detailed description of the procedures followed during the preparation and integration meetings. The only reported aspect is the encouragement given by the facilitators during the preparation sessions that participants should focus on their inner experiences and stay with whatever experience would arise during the session—a therapeutic intervention that, if anything, is quite non-specific. Thus, the therapeutic aspect in this context does neither seem to refer to specific professional qualifications of the session facilitators, nor is it associated with any particular psychotherapeutic model. At the very end of their paper, however, Davis et al. (2021, 487) do suggest that the nature of the therapy and the characteristics of the therapists should be the focus of future studies. The question of how, and whether at all, psychedelic treatment should be integrated with psychotherapy is indeed a pressing issue (Gründer et al., 2024) and one that will not be decided by scientific findings alone. Rather, considerable economic interests are already at play, particularly from institutions that have positioned themselves as early movers by offering what is potentially a lucrative training for future psychedelic therapists.
In the impact model of the Baltimore research group, the experiences initially described as “mystical-type,” but more recently (Davis et al., 2021) almost exclusively framed as “personally meaningful,” consistently play a significant role alongside pharmacological effects. It is a genuinely integrative approach that also references (ibid.) that the combination of pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy leads to better outcomes than just one of these treatment modes. Throughout Davis et al. (2021), there is consistently talk of “psilocybin-assisted therapy.” I.e., psilocybin is incorporated into therapy and its beneficial effects are attributed not only to the substance but to the overall therapeutic concept. This integration of psilocybin within therapy is the central novelty compared to the previously described studies—and probably no coincidence. Rather, it documents the fact that the psychedelic has to arrange itself with another, quite powerful, player within the health system: psychotherapy. In this light, it makes sense to embed the administration of the substance within a therapeutic framework. This shift towards the therapeutic also brings into play the question of the economic viability of psilocybin therapy. While the production of the substance for pharmaceutical purposes is still quite complex, considering the small amounts needed and the fact that, unlike the current pharmacological treatment of depression, it is not a long-term continuous medication but rather involves only a few single doses, the expected revenues from the new medication are likely to be lower compared to those from current depression medications (not to mention that some of the profits will go to different producers). However, if psilocybin is integrated into even a ten-hour therapeutic process, it opens up further economic opportunities—such as multi-day to multi-week stays in specialized clinics with corresponding costs. This scenario is not found in Davis et al. (2021), but unlike in the previously cited studies, health-economic arguments are present: The article even begins by noting that Major Depressive Disorder represents an estimated annual economic burden of $210 billion for the USA.
In addition to the described shift towards the therapeutic, another significant development is already reflected in the title of Davis et al. (2021): the expansion (which, incidentally, was prompted by the FDA) of the indication from (depressed) cancer patients to Major Depressive Disorder, i.e., to the pathologizing (‘disorder’) official psychiatric designation of that widespread, often chronic, and therefore extremely lucrative, yet poorly treatable condition, commonly known as depression. The shift to a predominantly clinical-pathological discourse, already observed in Griffiths et al. (2016), thus continues in Davis et al. (2021). “Mystical-type experiences” are mentioned here only in the concluding discussion and mainly in retrospect to earlier studies. There is only a very subtle reference to the part of the study aimed at these experiences; indeed, also in this study, participants were asked the day after the psilocybin sessions to assess the personal meaning, spiritual significance, psychological challenge, and psychological insight they associated with the session, and they were given the Mystical-Experience Questionnaire. But these data are only found in the usually seldom-read supplement to the article. In the article itself, the “mystical-type experiences” are only mentioned at the very end, and without reference to data, and they are also paraphrased with terms like “psychologically insightful experiences” or “personally meaningful experiences” (ibid.: 487), i.e., with ‘spiritually de-emphasized’ terms, thereby shifting them into an (individual) psychological frame of reference. In a study published around the same time, Yaden and Griffiths (2020) also avoid the term “mystical-type experiences” and refer instead to “subjective effects of psychedelics,” which are considered necessary for a lasting therapeutic effect.
The community observers—those individuals who had documented the participants' attitude and behavioral changes in their life worlds in the previously referenced studies—are also absent from the design of the study by Davis et al. (2021). Apparently, the research ambition to engage with the social and ecological life contexts from which the patients come and to which they return has diminished; the treatment model has become somewhat more individual-centered. A terminological change completes the overall impression of the shift in discourse: While participants in Griffiths et al. (2016) were “encouraged” to lie on the couch and wear sleep masks and headphones, in Davis et al. (2021) they are “instructed” to do so—indicating that their freedom to deviate from these instructions has somewhat diminished.
In their conclusion, Davis et al. (2021) emphasize the rapid and sustained antidepressant effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy, which are documented in both the participants' self-ratings and the professionals' assessments (again using the Hamilton Scale), and are far superior to those of conventional depression treatments. They then take it a step further in expanding the scope of psilocybin-assisted therapy: In view of positive treatment outcomes documented in various studies for other disorders, they postulate a transdiagnostic mechanism of action for psilocybin, ultimately suggesting an expansion of its application from depression to other psychiatric diagnoses (thereby focusing primarily on alcohol and nicotine dependence, but ultimately with a much broader claim, especially targeting eating disorders). Additional highly lucrative areas of application come into view.
Zooming out
What initially had appeared to be a move by a “Shambhala warrior” (Trungpa, 1984) in order to infiltrate psychedelics into pharmacology and through it into the healthcare system has apparently produced counter-effects (as the Luther Bible says, “he who enters danger, perishes in it”). This maneuver has led to the increasing neutralization, spiritual depotentialization, therapeutic containment, individualization, and standardization of psychedelics. Psychedelics have, in this process, and more or less inadvertently, been prepared for medicalization and an adoption into the prevailing healthcare system. The mystical experiences have adapted to the new scientific host culture, first in the form of quantifiable responses to questionnaires, and have then ultimately disappeared into the supplement.
Standardization is a direct result of the pharmacological research logic. This logic, for one, forces researchers to precisely determine the amount of the substance administered. One cannot write in a research report that a subject consumed 20 mushrooms of the genus Psilocybe semilanceata, but must provide measurements, in milligrams per kilogram of body weight, of a precisely defined ‘pure substance’. These pure substances (which, notably, suggest that other forms are impure) are produced in laboratories, shifting control over them from naturalist collectors to pharmacologists, and ultimately to the pharmaceutical industry, and to patent attorneys. Not to be overlooked in this shift is the change at the substance level itself: Unlike pure substances, both wild and cultivated mushrooms contain a spectrum of components, thus a chemical ‘timbre’ whose entourage effects are still largely unexplored.
Related to this, and unlike the use in traditional communities or the practices of the 1960s and the psychedelic underground, the results of today's psychedelic research studies are obtained in clinical settings from carefully isolated individuals who experience psilocybin within a hospital atmosphere—a classic institution of disciplinary power (Foucault, 1973). Traditional healers, in contrast, emphasize that their practice with psychedelic plants and mushrooms cannot be captured in such ‘sterile’ hospital scenarios. They point out the loss of the community-building and spiritual dimensions of the psychedelic experience in such settings, where the impulses for social and ecological engagement and communal renewal inherent in the psychedelic experience cannot unfold. In the clinic, psychedelics encounter epistemological structures that steer them away from a “psychedelic sociality” (Roseman, Preller, Fotiou, & Winkelman, 2022) and toward a resilience, creativity, and flexibility technology that aligns isolated individuals with the imperatives of the prevailing neoliberal zeitgeist, i.e., individualization and responsibilization.
How strongly monetization interests have already grasped the psychedelic realm (with long-term proponents of psychedelic therapies sometimes taking these interests into their own hands and professionalizing at an impressive pace) and how easily countercultural remnants can be combined with fresh entrepreneurial spirit became particularly evident at the recent Psychedelic Science Conference (PS2023) in Denver in June 2023. With its reported 12,000(!) participants, it strengthened the impression of an emerging psychedelic industrial complex that is merely awaiting the final green light in the form of substance approvals by the FDA (U.S. Food and Drug Administration) or the EMA (European Medicines Agency).
This is by no means to suggest that such developments are in line with Griffiths' intentions. Indeed, the trend of psychedelic research moving toward clinical studies has also been noted by Griffiths himself (Lawrence et al., 2021). The focus of the present study was on a particular trajectory of his research group, albeit the one with the strongest scientific impact and probably the most direct relevance for monetization. This trajectory, however, by no means represents all ambitions pursued with psychedelics within this group. Rather, the broad range of ambitions, which assemble at the Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research at Johns Hopkins University, has so far ensured the cohesion of the psychedelic as a “boundary object” (Star & Griesemer, 1989), i.e., an object which is part of multiple social and scientific worlds and onto which, hence, discourses from various societal and scientific fields converge, and which is stabilized precisely through this convergence, this discursive tug-of-war.
That Griffiths and his circle have not lost interest in mystical-type experiences shows itself in recent articles (e.g., Barrett & Griffiths, 2018; Griffiths et al., 2018). It appears, though, that these interests are increasingly diverging from clinical studies. This is evidenced by the dynamics of an initiative that has garnered significant public interest in magazines and newspapers like Washington Post or Guardian: In 2015, a study was launched at Johns Hopkins University in collaboration with the NYU Langone Medical Center to investigate the effects of psilocybin-induced experiences on 24 “professional leaders in religion” from various denominations. Though the study was registered as early as 2015 at the NIH for the treatment of religious or spiritual issues (sic!), results have not yet been published in a scientific format. Outside of science, the study has spawned interesting effects, though: Two of its participants have started initiatives to renew their (Jewish and Christian) faith traditions through psychedelic inspirations (shefaflow.org; ligare.org; McCarthy & Priest, 2024).
Strategies for generating and intensifying religious or liminal experiences through psychedelics, as they express themselves in such religious renewal initiatives, do not fit well into the clinical settings of mainstream contemporary psychedelic research. In such settings a tension arises for psychedelic therapy between an enhancement and dramatization of the experience, necessary as it is for a realignment of self- and world-relations and characteristic as it is of rites of passage, and the inherent tendency of the clinic to avoid risk and dramatization or at least make it manageable and reproducible. This tension between enhancing the experience while simultaneously standardizing it also documents itself in the meticulous disclosure of musical playlists that are used in psychedelic research studies — including in one of Griffith's research group (Davis et al., 2021).
In a similar vein, the charisma of the therapist does not fit into clinical scenarios. While almost all Eastern and indigenous traditions emphasize the need for spiritually experienced and charismatic process facilitators to ensure that the uncovering of access to psychic energy or insights does not lead to confusion, clinical studies have so far placed little emphasis on such qualities of the facilitators, not even on their previous psychedelic experience; often, they are described with the not very respectable term ‘trip-sitter’. Just as with the taming of animal magnetism, tendencies towards de-dramatization and standardization are characteristic for the current transformation of the psychedelic.
The most fundamental challenge to its coherence, though, seems to come from the introduction of chemical variants that have effects on neurotransmitter balance and neuroplasticity comparable to those of psychedelics but induce no or significantly fewer alterations in consciousness (Cameron & Olson, 2022; Cameron et al., 2021). These non-hallucinogenic psychoplastogens promise similar antidepressant, anxiolytic, or addiction-reducing effects as their psychedelic counterparts, but without inducing or inviting mystical or particular meaningful experiences and their potential pitfalls. In this case, one could safely forget the subjective, even mystical experience, or what the subject has to report about it. The fuss about set and setting, preparation, and integration of the psychedelic experience would become unnecessary, as would laborious, hours-long ‘trip-sitting’. A path of research and therapeutic practice that began with experience would then only understand experience as a negligible epiphenomenon. It would make little sense to still call these substances ‘psychedelic’; with them, we would be moving towards a purely pharmaceutical psychotechnology.
Where are we now?
Obviously, we are currently witnessing an array of new possibilities, alliances and figurations, of discursive and practical transductions and crossovers, through which the psychedelic is being redefined—and possibly even dissolved. The complex dynamics in which the different derivatives and offshoots of the psychedelic will engage, the economic, social, and political forces they will align with, and the psychedelic futures that will emerge from these interactions, seem more open than ever before: Will it be possible to disseminate psychedelics more widely without them being completely absorbed by the (epistemo)logic of the health care system? Will the legacy of the countercultural psychedelia of the 1960s be abandoned as its last witnesses disappear? Are we—as Aldous Huxley envisioned—witnessing the emergence of a spirituality informed by psychedelic experiences, connected to and compatible with science, a re-connection of humanity with the world? Or, on the contrary, will the technical-clinical and the spiritual-experiential diverge, with the pure technique of trip-free psychoplastogens being used for clinical applications, microdosing to enhance productivity and creativity in the workplace, while the ‘traditional’ psychedelic practice with its emphasis of life-changing peak-events, may find a place in neo-religious movements or high-end therapeutic niches that preserve the luxury of personal meaning-making through psychedelic experiences? These appear to be some of the questions and stakes that will likely continue to be relevant for some time, as controversies of such magnitude tend not to end with one side winning everything and the other(s) completely disappear.
However, the vast majority of scientific research on psychedelics and psychedelic-assisted therapy is currently conducted within a natural science framework. While it is undeniable that this clinically and increasingly biomedically oriented research is strategically important for the legalization of psychedelics, we should not overlook that this approach tends to medicalize their use and implicitly aligns it with the very power relations and exploitation structures that contribute to the pervasive unease in our current cultural situation (Hauskeller & Schwarz, 2023; Letcher, 2008). This makes it all the more crucial to keep in mind that scientific engagement with psychedelics and psychedelic therapy does not exhaust itself in creating clinical evidence (which, as any evidence, is an evidence for someone), but also encompasses critical social science perspectives, as expressed in the present study.
Conflict of interest
The author received no funding for this research and has no competing interests or financial relationships to disclose.
Acknowledgement
Open access funding provided by University of Vienna.
References
Barrett, F. S., & Griffiths, R. R. (2018). Classic hallucinogens and mystical experiences: Phenomenology and neural correlates. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 36, 393–430.
Bernheim, H. (1889). Die Suggestion und ihre Heilwirkung. Translated and with a foreword by Sigmund Freud. Leipzig und Wien: Deuticke.
Cameron, L. P., & Olson, D. E. (2022). The evolution of the psychedelic revolution. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(1), 413–414.
Cameron, L. P., Tombari, R. J., Lu, J., Pell, A. J., Hurley, Z. Q., Ehinger, Y., … Olson, D. E. (2021). A non-hallucinogenic psychedelic analogue with therapeutic potential. Nature, 589(7842), 474–479.
Davis, A. K., Barrett, F. S., May, D. G., Cosimano, M. P., Sepeda, N. D., Johnson, M. W., … Griffiths, R. R. (2021). Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 78(5), 481. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285.
Ellenberger, H. F. (1970). The discovery of the unconscious. The history and evolution of dynamic psychiatry. New York: Basic Books.
Foucault, M. (1973). The birth of the clinic: An archaeology of medical perception. London: Tavistock Publications.
Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Carducci, M. A., Umbricht, A., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., … Klinedinst, M. A. (2016). Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 1181–1197. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675513.
Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Richards, W. A., Richards, B. D., Jesse, R., MacLean, K. A., … Klinedinst, M. A. (2018). Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 32(1), 49–69.
Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W. A., McCann, U., & Jesse, R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology, 187(3), 268–283. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5.
Gründer, G., Brand, M., Mertens, L. J., Jungaberle, H., Kärtner, L., Scharf, D. J., … Wolff, M. (2024). Treatment with psychedelics is psychotherapy: Beyond reductionism. Lancet Psychiatry, 11(3), 231–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(23)00363-2.
Grzegorczyk, K. (2022). “Trust, Let Go, Be Open” – eine Dispositivanalyse der Psilocybin-assistierten Psychotherapie als Flexibilitätstechnologie. Master thesis. University of Vienna. u:theses | Detailansicht (63939) (univie.ac.at).
Hahn, M., & Schüttpelz, E. (2009). Trancemedien und neue Medien um 1900. Ein anderer Blick auf die Moderne. Bielefeld: transcript.
Hauskeller, C., & Schwarz, C. G. (2023). Critical doses: Nurturing diversity in psychedelic studies. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 48(5), 697–711. https://doi.org/10.1080/03080188.2023.2266974.
Lawrence, D. W., Sharma, B., Griffiths, R. R., & Carhart-Harris, R. (2021). Trends in the top-cited articles on classic psychedelics. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 53(4), 283–298.
Letcher, A. (2008). Mad thoughts on mushrooms: Discourse and power in the study of psychedelic consciousness. Anthropology of Consciousness, 18(2), 74–98.
McCarthy, B., & Priest, H. (2024). Psychedelic Christianity: From evangelical hippies and Roman Catholic intellectuals in the sixties to clergy in a Johns Hopkins clinical trial. Journal of Psychedelic Studies, 8(2), 143–152. https://doi.org/10.1556/2054.2024.00356.
Mithoefer, M. (2013). A manual for MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder. Santa Cruz, CA: Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies.
Pollan, M. (2018). How to change your mind. What the new science of psychedelics teaches us about consciousness, dying, addiction, depression, and transcendence. New York: Penguin Press.
Roseman, L., Preller, K. H., Fotiou, E., & Winkelman, M. J. (2022). Editorial Psychedelic sociality: Pharmacological and extrapharmacological perspectives. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2022.979764.
Schmidbauer, W. (2012). Die Geschichte der Psychotherapie: Von der Magie zur Wissenschaft. München: Herbig.
Schuster, C. R. (2006). Commentary on: Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance by Griffiths et al. Psychopharmacology, 187, 289–290.
Schüttpelz, E. (2012). Mediumismus und moderne Medien. Die Prüfung des europäischen Medienbegriffs. Deutsche Vierteljahrsschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Geistesgeschichte, 86(1), 121–144.
Sessa, B. (2017). The psychedelic renaissance. London: Muswell Hill Press.
Slunecko, T. (in press). Das Psychedelische - ein Grenzobjekt zwischen utopisch-religiösen, wissenschaftlich-therapeutischen und ökonomischen Zugriffen. In B. Bösel, & J. Weber (Eds.), Die Macht des Psychotechnischen. Zur Erschließung eines transdisziplinären Forschungsfeldes. Frankfurt: Campus.
Slunecko, T., & Benetka, G. (2023). Geschichte und Paradigmen der Psychologie und Psychotherapie. Wien: facultas UTB.
Star, S. L., & Griesemer, J. R. (1989). Institutional ecology, ‘translations’ and boundary objects: Amateurs and professionals in Berkeley's museum of vertebrate zoology, 1907–39. Social Studies of Science, 19(3), 387–420.
Trungpa, C. (1984). Shambhala: The sacred path of the warrior. Boston & London: Shambhala.
Yaden, D. B., & Griffiths, R. R. (2020). The subjective effects of psychedelics are necessary for their enduring therapeutic effects. ACS Pharmacology & Translational Science, 4(2), 568–572. https://doi.org/10.1021/acsptsci.0c00194.