Abstract
Introduction
No research has examined the impact of any physical-artistic-cognitive activity on foetal neurodevelopment. The aim of the study was to investigate the efficacy of a unique prenatal dance activity in pre- and postnatal cognitive and motor development as a complementary health care practice.
Methods
26 clinically uncomplicated primiparas and multiparas with singleton pregnancies and their later born children were examined in this prospective study at the University of Pécs, Hungary. The activity group participated in supervised, 60-min, twice-weekly, moderate-intensity prenatal dance classes for 19.56 ± 3.97 weeks, whereas the control group did not. We determined the developmental ages of their children with the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development in both groups at 5 weeks of age and in the activity group at 33 months of age.
Results
Prenatal dance activity did not cause any adverse outcomes. Infants in the activity group had significantly higher mean developmental ages than the control group regarding cognitive skills (P < 0.001), receptive (P < 0.001) and expressive communication (P = 0.007), fine (P < 0.001) and gross motor (P = 0.001). As toddlers their mean developmental ages were significantly higher than their mean calendar age regarding cognitive skills (P = 0.001), receptive (P = 0.001) and expressive communication (P = 0.001), fine (P = 0.002) and gross motor (P = 0.001).
Conclusions
Our results confirm the safe implementation of this prenatal dance activity and the more advanced cognitive and motor development of children in the activity group as infants compared to the control group and as toddlers compared to the norm. These results offer a novel approach to dance in pre- and postnatal clinical practice.
Introduction
Dancing is not one of the prenatal lifestyle pieces of advice usually recommended to enhance intrauterine brain development, although it may have the potential to provide an excellent basis for cognitive and motor development already in the uterus and to support it even after birth. Although studies have shown positive foetal effects of separate physical, musical, and cognitive activities during pregnancy, no research has examined the impact of prenatal dance on foetal neurodevelopment. In our study we aim to demonstrate that dance deserves more attention as a possible complementary therapy in prenatal care and may provide the basis for postnatal development, as it offers all benefits of the combination of physical exercise, cognitive and musical activity.
In utero conditions bear crucial implications for long-term health status [1], therefore serious attention needs to be paid to maternal and environmental influences during pregnancy. Neurodevelopment is exposed to these factors from the third week of gestation [2]. In the complex process of intrauterine brain development characterized by high plasticity, in addition to internal effects, external ones such as environmental stimuli affecting the human foetus may also cause alterations in either a positive or a negative way [3–5].
Researchers have confirmed the beneficial impact of prenatal physical activity (PA) on foetal health, brain development, and later cognitive performance [6–10]. One physiological reason for the positive influence may be an increase in blood volume and placental function, suggesting the greater transport of oxygen and nutriments to the brains of physically active participants' foetuses [11].
Nevertheless, PA levels are still low worldwide. Although this is a leading risk factor for non-communicable diseases and can have a negative effect on mental health and quality of life [12, 13] most people still lead sedentary lifestyles during pregnancy [14]. National guidelines have been prepared to define the intensity, regularity, and duration, as well as the recommended and contraindicated forms of prenatal exercises [15–17] to encourage those who are expecting to choose from a range of safe, appropriate, and well-tolerated activities.
We propose dance to be investigated separately, since in addition to physical, it also bears cognitive, social, emotional, and artistic qualities. Dance is a complex sequence of steps and movements of a person, a couple, or a group that harmonizes different parts of the body in a structured, choreographed, or improvised manner, with or without sound, mostly rhythmically [18].
In recent years, the number of investigations examining the health-preserving and healing effects of dance and its application for various clinical groups has also been increasing [19–27], but its applicability in several areas, such as prenatal care, is still under-researched, especially for foetal outcomes.
From a foetal neurological perspective, the clinical rationale for prenatal dance activity (PDA) may be multifactorial and exert extra external stimuli on the foetus. In addition to physical effects, they are exposed to more music - which may contribute to healthy [28] or better [29] neurodevelopment -, they may feel the rhythmic, synchronized movements performed to it, and may benefit from their mother's cognitive activities - which may affect foetal blood flow [30] - and positive maternal feelings.
To the best of our knowledge, ours is the first scientific research to examine the safety of dance during pregnancy and its effects on foetal and postnatal cognitive and motor development.
The objectives of our study were to investigate the safety and to assess the efficacy of a moderate-intensity prenatal dance method in foetal and postnatal development. Our hypothesis was that the activity has no negative effect on gestational, birth, or neonatal outcomes. We also hypothesized that children of dancing participants would perform better in developmental tests as infants compared to the control group and as toddlers compared to the norm, assuming also the long-term effects of PDA.
Materials and methods
Study design
The research was designed to assess the potential influence of a special, newly developed, regularly performed PDA on foetal and postnatal cognitive and motor development without causing any adverse gestational, foetal, birth, or neonatal outcomes. We used the STROBE statement to write our research article.
Setting
We conducted our research at the Department of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Clinical Centre, University of Pécs, Hungary.
Participants
We recruited clinically uncomplicated primiparas and multiparas for our research. We defined the following inclusion criteria: age over 18 years, singleton pregnancy, 16th to 30th week of gestation, no mental disorders, no contraindication to PA. Exclusion criteria were non-compliance with the above conditions, lack of written informed consent, non-regular participation in the activity, engaging in any sports, occurrence of any foetal or maternal disorders before or during the research, or moving out of town, while in the case of children, preterm birth and any known abnormalities.
Volunteers were allocated into an activity (AG) and control group (CG) using convenience sampling. All AG subjects needed to undertake regular attendance at the sessions until the 36th week of pregnancy. All participants had received verbal and written information about the purpose and procedures of the study before any data collection or the activity was initiated. Members of the AG consulted their physicians about the activity and provided written informed consent along with CG members. Gestational age was determined based on menstrual history and ultrasound examination.
Variables
The primary outcome of the study was the safe implementation of the intervention during pregnancy without any adverse effects. The secondary outcome was the efficacy of the intervention. AG participants were exposed to a 20-week-long dance activity, CG were not. Any other physical and artistic activity or heterogenous groups could have been potential confounders or effect modifiers regarding the outcomes.
Ethical statement
The research was approved by the University of Pécs Clinical Centre Regional Committee for the Research Ethics, Hungary (April 21, 2017), No: PTE/6618. Data were treated anonymously. During the research, any identifying information was removed from the medical records and each participant was given a unique code. This was also linked to the appropriate infant and toddler tests. The study met institutional, national, and international guidelines for protection of human subjects concerning their safety and privacy. The research study was performed in accordance with the principles stated in the Declaration of Helsinki.
Procedures
The AG regularly attended a supervised group dance course, with the mean attendance of 19.56 ± 3.97 weeks, at the earliest from the 16th week of gestation. Members of the CG did not take part in the activities. We measured foetal heart rate (FHR) and umbilical artery impedance indices – resistive index (RI) and pulsatility index (PI) – before and during the sessions of the AG once a week and tested the infants' developmental progress at 5 weeks of age in both groups and at 33 months of age in the AG.
The PDA examined in this study was developed for this research by the lead author of this article who is a trained dance educator. The sessions were held by two dance teachers and supervised by four obstetrician gynaecologists. It is a unique dance method that combines various dance styles specifically selected for this population, including a wide range of movements, musical styles, and rhythms, as well as improvisation. It was a 60-min, twice-weekly, moderate-intensity dance activity, requiring a moderate amount of effort and noticeably accelerating the heart rate, without causing rapid breathing, as defined by the WHO [31]. We monitored the participants' heart rate and applied the talk test (the ability to converse with some slight effort during the exercises). Each session began with resting heart rate and blood pressure measurements for the participants. The active part of the lessons consisted of warm-ups with sitting and standing exercises, stretching, learning and performing steps and choreographies of the dance styles. Participants were encouraged to apply the learned movements on their own and improvise. Cool-down terminated the classes with a low-intensity free dance when we asked participants to imagine themselves as if they were dancing with their babies in their arms. Once a week, the classes were held at the clinic and once in the university dance studio.
Data sources
During the first meeting, participants completed a sociodemographic questionnaire. Information on childbirth and the neonatal data were collected from medical reports during the postpartum visit. To analyse infant and toddler cognitive and motor development, we used the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley-III). It is an individually administered instrument assessing developmental functioning from the age of one month. It evaluates cognitive, communication (receptive and expressive), and motor (fine and gross) skills.
The lead author conducted the testing with the involvement of the parents and the assessment with a psychologist. First the calendar age of infants was determined in days, no adjustment to prematurity was needed. In the follow-up research age was given in months. Scales consisted of tasks that children had to perform in different positionings during testing, with or without the involvement of certain objects. We calculated total raw scores for the scales from which we determined scaled and composite scores and then computed developmental age.
Throughout the research all mandatory laboratory health and safety procedures were complied with in the course of conducting the activity by performing ultrasound examinations and cardiac monitoring, and controlling the intensity and proper execution of movements, hydration, convenient room temperature, and clothing. No adverse event occurred throughout the research.
Bias
We paid close attention to address potential sources of bias. Medical records and tests were assessed by the lead author, four obstetrician gynaecologists, and a psychologist.
Study size
We determined sample size from similar previous research on prenatal PA and interventional studies. Due to the nature and length of the activity, the use of a larger sample was neither our goal nor feasible. Prenatal physical activity is low worldwide, therefore activating pregnant volunteers is a great challenge. At the supervised activity we examined due attention had to be paid to all participants, therefore the number had to be limited. It also required several months of regular participation, which could not be accepted by everyone. It was followed by an infant testing five weeks after birth and a toddler testing at almost 3 years of age, which also narrowed the number of participants, especially in a city with less than 150,000 residents. We targeted 25 participants and were satisfied to experience a larger number.
Statistical methods
We applied the IBM SPSS Statistics 25.0 and 27.0 for Mac (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Based on the results of the normality tests (Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests), we conducted a Mann-Whitney U-test, paired t tests and non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon) to detect statistical differences. In our study, quantitative variables are described with mean ± standard deviation (SD). Qualitative variables are presented with the number (n) and percentage (%) of participants. Statistical significance was accepted at P < 0.050.
Results
Recruitment for the research lasted from 21 August, 2017 to 31 August, 2019. The 35 volunteers were allocated into an AG (n = 20) and a CG (n = 15). Four of them were excluded from the AG and five from the CG based on the exclusion criteria, as shown in Fig. 1. Therefore, the final sample consisted of 26 participants: 16 in the AG and 10 in the CG, all of whom took part in the infant testing, whereas 13 AG members participated in the toddler testing. The first meeting took place on 4 October, 2017 and the activity lasted from 11 October, 2017 to 28 June, 2019. Infant testing was conducted from 14 March, 2018 to 13 January, 2020, and toddler testing took place from 15 January to 15 March, 2021.
Research profile–The diagram shows the research with detailed information on the excluded participants
Citation: Physiology International 109, 4; 10.1556/2060.2022.00121
Table 1 presents sociodemographic and anthropometric measures of the participants in which the two populations appeared homogeneous. The only statistically significant difference was detected in the 36th week BMI of the groups. No significant difference was found in gestational, birth, or neonatal measures between the AG and CG, as shown in Table 2.
Baseline characteristics of the intention-to-treat population: sociodemographic and anthropometric measures
Activity group (n = 16) | Control group (n = 10) | P value | |
Age (years) | 30.38 (6.46) | 31.50 (5.42) | 0.596 NS |
Marital status | 0.937 NS | ||
| 3 (18.75%) | 2 (20%) | |
| 13 (81.25%) | 8 (80%) | |
Type of residence | 0.508 NS | ||
| 14 (87.50%) | 10 (100%) | |
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
Education | 0.576 NS | ||
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
| 3 (18.75%) | 1 (10%) | |
| 12 (75.00%) | 9 (90%) | |
Being employed | 16 (100%) | 10 (100%) | |
Having prepared for pregnancy | 0.846 NS | ||
| 2 (12.50%) | 1 (10%) | |
| 14 (87.50%) | 9 (90%) | |
Number of children | 0.636 NS | ||
| 11 (68.75%) | 8 (80%) | |
| 3 (18.75%) | 0 (0%) | |
| 1 (6.25%) | 2 (20%) | |
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
Family income (HUF) | 0.056 NS | ||
Below 100,000 | 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
100,000–250,000 | 3 (18.75%) | 0 (0%) | |
250,000–400,000 | 9 (56.25%) | 4 (40%) | |
400,000–550,000 | 1 (6.25%) | 5 (50%) | |
550,000–700,000 | 0 (0%) | 1 (10%) | |
Over 700,000 | 2 (12.50%) | 0 (0%) | |
No smoking or alcohol consumption | 16 (100%) | 10 (100%) | |
Living in a healthy environment | 0.420 NS | ||
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
| 15 (93.75%) | 10 (100%) | |
Vitamin-rich, balanced diet | 0.342 NS | ||
| 4 (25.00%) | 4 (40%) | |
| 10 (62.50%) | 5 (50%) | |
| 0 (0%) | 1 (10%) | |
| 2 (12.50%) | 0 (0%) | |
BMI (kg/m2) | |||
| 23.81 (4.12) | 21.38 (2.11) | 0.126 NS |
| 28.94 (4.13) | 26.07 (2.53) | 0.045 * |
Gestational weight gain (kg) | 14.25 (3.30) | 13.90 (2.88) | 0.615 NS |
Data are n (%) or mean (±SD). HUF = Hungarian currency. BMI = body mass index.
Notes: NS = not statistically significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Baseline characteristics of the intention-to-treat population: gestational, birth, and neonatal measures
Activity group (n = 16) | Control group (n = 10) | P value | |
Preeclampsia | 1.000 NS | ||
| 16 (100%) | 10 (100%) | |
| 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | |
Placental insufficiency | 0.254 NS | ||
| 14 (87.50%) | 10 (100%) | |
| 2 (12.50%) | 0 (0%) | |
Length of pregnancy (weeks) | 39.50 (0.97) | 39.40 (1.17) | 0.779 NS |
Weakening of foetal heart sound | 0.773 NS | ||
| 12 (75%) | 8 (80%) | |
| 4 (25%) | 2 (20%) | |
Oxygen deficiency | 0.429 NS | ||
| 15 (93.75%) | 10 (100%) | |
| 1 (6.25%) | 0 (0%) | |
Complications during labour or childbirth | 0.492 NS | ||
| 9 (56.3%) | 7 (70%) | |
| 7 (43.8%) | 3 (30%) | |
Weakening of contractions | 0.849 NS | ||
| 14 (87.50%) | 9 (90%) | |
| 2 (12.50%) | 1 (10%) | |
Prolonged labour | 0.555 NS | ||
| 13 (81.25%) | 9 (90%) | |
| 3 (18.25%) | 1 (10%) | |
Surgical delivery | 0.325 NS | ||
| 8 (50%) | 3 (30%) | |
| 8 (50%) | 7 (70%) | |
New-born's birth weight (g) | 3,419.38 (348.38) | 3,494.00 (512.12) | 0.792 NS |
New-born's birth length (cm) | 50.56 (1.86) | 50.50 (2.72) | 0.667 NS |
Head circumference (cm) | 36.58 (1.45) | 37.45 (1.80) | 0.294 NS |
Chest circumference (cm) | 37.50 (0.71) | 37.00 (1.73) | 0.157 NS |
Apgar scores | |||
| 8.94 (0.25) | 8.80 (0.42) | 0.295 NS |
| 9.94 (0.25) | 9.90 (0.32) | 0.732 NS |
Days spent in hospital after birth | 3.88 (1.78) | 3.90 (0.57) | 0.237 NS |
Calendar age of infants at testing (days) | 39.40 (1.51) | 37.88 (2.78) | 0.182 NS |
Data are n (%) or mean (±SD).
Notes: NS = not statistically significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
As a primary outcome, we demonstrated the safety of the activity. Our own measurements during the sessions and medical reports on birth and neonatal variables did not show any maternal, foetal, or neonatal disorders among the participants in the AG. Our measurements show that FHR, RI, and PI increased during activity (Table 3). A significant difference was observed between mean FHR values at rest and during activity. The differences between the impedance indices were not significant.
Foetal mean resting and in-activity measures before and during the sessions of the activity
Resting measures (n = 16) | In-activity measures (n = 16) | P value | |
Impedance indices | |||
| 0.66 (0.06) | 0.67 (0.06) | 0.534 NS |
| 1.05 (0.17) | 1.06 (0.18) | 0.776 NS |
Foetal heart rate (beats per minute) | 141.14 (6.32) | 146.13 (6.14) | 0.010 * |
Data are mean (±SD).
Notes: NS = not statistically significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
As a secondary outcome, examining the efficacy of the activity, we detected a significant difference between the AG and CG infants in the cognitive, receptive and expressive communication, fine and gross motor subtests of the Bayley-III. Children in the AG performed better than those in the CG on all scales. The developmental ages were 2.5–1.6 times and 1.1–0.7 times their calendar ages, respectively. The mean calendar age of the infants was 37.88 ± 2.78 days in the AG and 39.40 ± 1.51 in the CG at the time of testing. We determined mean developmental ages in days based on scaled and composite scores (Fig. 2).
Mean Bayley-III infant test results representing developmental ages by scales compared with calendar age in the activity (n = 16) and the control (n = 10) groups
Notes: NS = not statistically significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.
Citation: Physiology International 109, 4; 10.1556/2060.2022.00121
In the follow-up research Bayley-III was applied in the AG when the children's mean calendar age was 33.46 ± 2.03 months. Mean developmental ages were significantly higher than their calendar age on all subscales. Children in all areas of development were 9 to 7 months before their age, as shown in Fig. 3.
Mean Bayley-III toddler test results representing developmental ages by scales compared with calendar age in the activity (n = 13) group. NS = not statistically significant, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001
Citation: Physiology International 109, 4; 10.1556/2060.2022.00121
Discussion
Our measurements and medical records showed that the supervised, regular, moderate-intensity dance activity was safe and well-tolerated in pregnancy and did not cause adverse gestational, foetal, birth, or neonatal outcomes.
We observed the growth in RI and PI and the significant increase in FHR values in a safe interval during activity, suggesting enhanced but not abnormal foetal movement and circulation. This finding supports the assumption that foetuses become more active due to the PDA.
We found that AG infants performed significantly better than CG infants on all examined scales of the Bayley-III. As toddlers mean developmental ages of the AG children were also significantly higher than their mean calendar age on all subtests. These findings suggest a more advanced cognitive and motor development in the AG children and a long-term impact of prenatal dance. Therefore, results lend credence to all our hypotheses.
The safe applicability of this PDA lies in the thorough and careful design of the method, the application of guidelines for prenatal PA, strict surveillance, and measurements. In terms of foetal development, complex effects may be assumed. We found that as participants became more active both physically and cognitively during the sessions, the foetal measures also reflected the same. Therefore, circulation and umbilical cord flow improved, contributing to an increase in the supply of nutrients and oxygen to the foetal brain. Their parent's dance involved their auditory, somatosensory, and vestibular systems simultaneously, since participants not only performed movements or listened to music separately but combined the two in synchrony, using varying rhythms, rocking, gliding, swaying, reversing, and turning steps while enjoying themselves. This may have caused the foetal brain to coordinate the impulses of the environment and the body, although we did not have the possibility to quantitatively measure these factors.
None of the participants engaged in any sporting activities during pregnancy, and we found no significant difference in their lifestyle, dietary, or socioeconomic factors. Parental, birth, and neonatal homogenous baseline characteristics of the two groups suggest that the children developed under similar conditions in the uterus, except for PDA as an environmental difference. Therefore, we assume that the differences can be interpreted specifically as a consequence of the dance activity.
As prenatal dance is an under-researched part of the scientific field, we could only compare our findings with partially overlapping studies. Examining dance as a PA, our findings are consistent with results of research that have discovered the safe applicability of sports and various moderate physical exercises during pregnancy [15, 32–35]. Systematic reviews on the impact of PA on foetal brain development are rare. Those investigating this found that expectant maternal exercise altered the offspring's brain and behaviour and that PA habits appear to have beneficial influences on pre- and postnatal brain development [6–10, 36].
Our study bears several strengths. First, we examined an area that has not yet been researched. The activity applied is a unique, novel, and innovative initiative. Second, the activity was not self-monitored but supervised, ensuring regularity, the same intensity and duration, standardized measures, and identical content for each AG member. Expert supervision was also essential for proper execution of movements, hydration, appropriate clothing, venue, and group dynamics. Third, there were no statistically significant differences between the two populations in terms of sociodemographic and anthropometric measures, birth and neonatal data, and difference was found only in the 36th week BMI values. Fourth, we analysed medical reports, resting and in-activity ultrasound measurements, and validated tests to detect safety, as well as infant and toddler development. Fifth, almost 3 years after birth, we conducted a follow-up research to investigate the long-term effects of the activity. Sixth, the same person led the testing with all participants to ensure standardization of the experimental protocol.
We acknowledge that our study bears some limitations. First, we could not carry out our research involving a physical activity control group. Therefore, we can conclude only that dancing is similarly beneficial to other types of physical activities. Although music, rhythm, and emotions experienced during dancing may offer additional benefits, this has not been proven. Second, we could not follow the CG and compared the results of toddlers of the AG to the general population of their age. Third, the number of participants was low. However, the remarkable effect of the dancing activity demonstrated by the significant difference in developmental ages between AG and CG infants, and between AG toddlers and general population may offer new, potentially useful information in this under-researched field.
Future investigations are necessary to strengthen and expand upon evidence-based research on the connection between PDA and foetal-postnatal cognitive and motor development. In addition to questionnaires and developmental scales, the use of safe neuroimaging technology to examine the brains of foetuses, infants, and toddlers may yield more impressive results. Increasing the number of participants volunteering for a randomized controlled trial would be useful in prospective studies. Assessing possible differences in the impact of dance and sports or dance and music during pregnancy on pre- and postnatal cognitive and motor development may also offer research potential.
Our findings suggest that PDA has a significant influence on the developing foetal brain and long-term effects on cognitive and motor development in infants and toddlers. Interpreting the results and benefits without any harms can help make prenatal dance a safe and joyful, novel, forward-looking, and innovative initiative. By encouraging childbearing parents to take part in such a joyous activity as dancing, a new mode of prenatal intervention could be introduced in clinical practice to increase the number of physically active expectant people for the benefit of their children's development. Our findings may also support future studies to concentrate on prenatal dance, while also identifying the mechanisms behind its effects.
Our mission is to inform parents about our achievements and encourage them to choose this activity as they can have a significant impact on the quality of future life of their children already in the uterus.
Author contributions
Beatrix Bánkyné Perjés: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft, Project administration. Gábor Mátrai, Bernadett Nagy: Investigation, Data curation. Daniella Erdei: Data curation. Alexandra Makai: Formal analysis. Viktória Prémusz: Writing – reviewing and editing. Kálmán András Kovács: Validation. József Bódis: Validation, Supervision.
Funding
This work was supported by RRF-2.3.1-21-2022-00012 “National Laboratory on Human Reproduction”. The funding source had no involvement in study design, in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the article for publication.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Acknowledgement
Authors wish to thank all participants for contributing to our study at such a significant time of their life.
List of abbreviations
AG | Activity Group |
Bayley-III | Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development |
BMI | Body Mass Index |
CG | Control Group |
FHR | Foetal Heart Rate |
PA | Physical Activity |
PDA | Prenatal Dance Activity |
PI | Pulsatility Index |
RI | Resistive Index |
References
- 1.↑
Barker DJ. The fetal and infant origins of adult disease. BMJ 1990; 301(6761): 1111–1111. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.301.6761.1111.
- 2.↑
Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev 2010; 20(4): 327–348. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4.
- 3.↑
Rice D, Barone S, Jr. Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous system: Evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect 2000; 108(Suppl 3): 511–533. https://doi.org/10.1289/ehp.00108s3511.
- 4.
Gottlieb G. Conceptions of prenatal development: Behavioral embryology. Psychol Rev 1976; 83(3): 215–234. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.3.215.
- 5.
Chaudhury S, Sharma V, Kumar V, Nag TC, Wadhwa S. Activity-dependent synaptic plasticity modulates the critical phase of brain development. Brain Dev 2016; 38(4): 355–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2015.10.008.
- 6.↑
Clapp JF, III. Morphometric and neurodevelopmental outcome at age five years of the offspring of women who continued to exercise regularly throughout pregnancy. J Pediatr 1996; 129(6): 856–863. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3476(96)70029-X.
- 7.
Esteban-Cornejo I, Martinez-Gomez D, Tejero-Gonzalez CM, Izquierdo-Gomez R, Carbonell-Baeza A, Castro-Piñero J, et al. Maternal physical activity before and during the prenatal period and the offspring’s academic performance in youth. The UP&DOWN study. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2015; 29(9): 1414–1420. https://doi.org/10.3109/14767058.2015.1049525.
- 8.
Labonte-Lemoyne E, Curnier D, Ellemberg D. Exercise during pregnancy enhances cerebral maturation in the newborn: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2016; 39(4): 347–354. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1227427.
- 9.
Clapp JF, Simonian S, Lopez B, Appleby-Wineberg S, Harcar-Sevcik R. The one-year morphometric and neurodevelopmental outcome of the offspring of women who continued to exercise regularly throughout pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 1998; 178(3): 594–599. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-9378(98)70444-2.
- 10.
Jukic AMZ, Lawlor DA, Juhl M, Owe KM, Lewis B, Liu J, et al. Physical activity during pregnancy and language development in the offspring. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2013; 27(3): 283–293. https://doi.org/10.1111/ppe.12046.
- 12.↑
Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: A pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 1.9 million participants. Lancet Glob Health 2018; 6(10): e1077–e1086. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30357-7.
- 13.↑
Ács P, Stocker M, Füge K, Paár D, Oláh A, Kovács A. Economic and public health benefits: The result of increased regular physical activity. Eur J Integr Med 2016; 8(Suppl 2): 8–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eujim.2016.11.003.
- 14.↑
Downs DS, Chasan-Taber L, Evenson KR, Leiferman J, Yeo S. Physical activity and pregnancy: Past and present evidence and future recommendations. Res Q Exerc Sport 2012; 83: 485–502. https://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2012.10599138.
- 15.↑
The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. ACOG committee opinion number 804. Committee on obstetric practice, physical activity and exercise during pregnancy and the postpartum period [Internet]. The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists; 2020 [updated 2022 April 30]. Available from: https://www.acog.org/clinical/clinical-guidance/committee-opinion/articles/2020/04/physical-activity-and-exercise-during-pregnancy-and-the-postpartum-period.
- 16.
Wolfe L, Davies G. Canadian guidelines for exercise in pregnancy. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2003; 46(2): 488–495. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003081-200306000-00027.
- 17.
gov.uk. UK Chief medical officers' physical activity guidelines [Internet]. UK Government; 2019 [updated 2022 April 30]. Available from: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/832868/uk-chief-medical-officers-physical-activity-guidelines.pdf.
- 18.↑
Bläsing B, Calvo-Merino B, Cross ES, Jola C, Honisch J, Stevens CJ. Neurocognitive control in dance perception and performance. Acta Psychol 2012; 139(2): 300–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.12.005.
- 19.↑
Nigmatullina Y, Hellyer PJ, Nachev P, Sharp DJ, Seemungal BM. The neuroanatomical correlates of training-related perceptuoreflex uncoupling in dancers. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25(2): 554–562. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht266.
- 20.
Meier J, Topka MS, Hänggi J. Differences in cortical representation and structural connectivity of hands and feet between professional handball players and ballet dancers. Neural Plast 2016; 2016: e6817397. https://doi.org/10.1155/2016/6817397.
- 21.
Karpati FJ, Giacosa C, Foster NEV, Penhune VB, Hyde KL. Dance and the brain: a review. Ann NY Acad Sci 2015; 1337: 140–146. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12632.
- 22.
Koch S, Kunz T, Lykou S, Cruz R. Effects of dance movement therapy and dance on health-related psychological outcomes: a meta-analysis. Arts Psychother 2014; 41(1): 46–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2013.10.004.
- 23.
Hackney ME, Earhart GM. Effects of dance on movement control in Parkinson’s disease: A comparison of Argentine tango and American ballroom. J Rehabil Med 2009; 41(6): 475–481. https://doi.org/10.2340/16501977-0362.
- 24.
Ballesteros S, Kraft E, Santana S, Tziraki C. Maintaining older brain functionality: A targeted review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2015; 55: 453–477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.008.
- 25.
Lifshitz-Vahav H, Shnitzer S, Mashal N. Participation in recreation and cognitive activities as a predictor of cognitive performance of adults with/without Down syndrome. Aging Ment Health 2016; 20(9): 955–964. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1047322.
- 26.
Loo LWM, Nishibun K, Welsh L. Using a cultural dance program to increase sustainable physical activity for breast cancer survivors - a pilot study. Complement Ther Med 2019; 47: 102197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2019.102197.
- 27.
DeJesus BM, Oliveira RC, de Carvalho FO, de Jesus Mari J, Arida RM, Teixeira-Machado L. Dance promotes positive benefits for negative symptoms in autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A systematic review. Complement Ther Med 2020; 49: 102299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102299.
- 28.↑
McMahon E, Wintermark P, Lahav A. Auditory brain development in premature infants: the importance of early experience. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2012; 1252: 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06445.x.
- 29.↑
Arya R, Chansoria M, Konanki R, Tiwari DK. Maternal music exposure during pregnancy influences neonatal behaviour: An open-label randomized controlled trial. Int J Pediatr 2012; 2012: 901812. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/901812.
- 30.↑
Hassidov D, Asher UA, Ben-Ami M, Keselman L, Sabri R, Haddad S. The effect of maternal mathematical activities on the fetal brain. Open J Obstet Gynecol 2018; 8: 826–835. https://doi.org/10.4236/ojog.2018.810086.
- 31.↑
World Health Organization Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean. Promoting physical activity to prevent and control noncommunicable diseases [Internet]. World Health Organization Regional Office for the Eastern Mediterranean; 2022 [updated 2022 April 30]. Available from: http://www.emro.who.int/noncommunicable-diseases/publications/questions-and-answers-on-promoting-physical-activity-to-prevent-and-control-noncommunicable-diseases.html.
- 32.↑
Lokey EA, Tran ZV, Wells CL, Myers BC, Tran AC. Effects of physical exercise on pregnancy outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1991; 23(11): 1234–1239.
- 33.
Leet T, Flick L. Effect of exercise on birth-weight. Clin Obstet Gynecol 2003; 46(2): 423–431. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003081-200306000-00021.
- 34.
Wolfe LA, Weissgerber TL. Clinical physiology of exercise in pregnancy: A literature review. J Obstet Gynaecol Can 2003; 25(6): 473–483. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1701-2163(16)30309-7.
- 35.
Morris SN, Johnson NR. Exercise during pregnancy: a critical appraisal of the literature. J Reprod Med 2005; 50(3): 181–188.
- 36.↑
Gomes da Silva S, Arida RM. Physical activity and brain development. Expert Rev Neurother 2015; 15(9): 1041–1051. https://doi.org/10.1586/14737175.2015.1077115.