The paper reviews the role of Noam Chomsky in the conceptual changes in modern psychology that are described by many as the cognitive revolution. Several aspects of the work of Chomsky are identified as key elements in the changes regarding the human mind, and the determinants of human nature. The mentalism of Chomsky resulted in the general spread of theory theories about human development, where the human mind is interpreted as a theory-using open, creative system. The peculiarities of sequential behavioral organization and later the sequential interface issues as well as the concentration on (syntactic) pure form were important inspirations for several general theories of human cognition. Chomsky, with his differentiation between competence and performance, opened the road along with David Marr to multilayered computational theories of the mind. While the innatist commitments of Chomsky regarding human development created many fruitful controversies during half a century, they also tied the underdetermined nature of human language with the philosophical issues of freedom. Language with its innate recursive system is a basic factor of human freedom. Freedom is connected to a rational image of humanity.
Anderson, John R. and Gordon H. Bower. 1973. Human associative memory. Washington, DC: V. H. Winston & Sons.
Baars, Bernard J. 1986. The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York: Guilford Press.
Bever, Tom G. 1970. The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In R. Hayes (ed.) Cognition and language development. New York: Wiley. 279–362.
Blumenthal, Arthur . 1970. Psychology and language: A historical introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Wiley.
Broadbent, Donald E . 1958. Perception and communication. London: Pergamon.
Brown, Roger . 1958. Words and things. New York: Free Press.
Brown, Roger . 1970. Psycholinguistics: Selected papers. New York: Free Press.
Brown, Roger . 1973. A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, Jerome S. 1974. From communication to language. A psychological perspective. Cognition 3. 225–287.
Bruner, Jerome S . 1983. In search of mind: Essays in autobiography. New York: Harper and Row.
Bruner, Jerome S . 1997. Will cognitive revolutions ever stop? In D. M. Johnson and C. E. Erneling (eds.) The future of the cognitive revolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 279–292.
Burt, Cyril . 1961. Intelligence and social mobility. British Journal of Statistical Psychology 14. 3–24.
Carnap, Rudolf . 1934/1937. Logische Syntax der Sprache. English translation: The Logical Syntax of Language. New York: Harcourt.
Chomsky, Noam . 1956. Three models for the description of language. IRE Transactions on Information Theory 2. 113–124.
Chomsky, Noam . 1957. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.
Chomsky, Noam . 1959. A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal behavior. Language 35. 26–58.
Chomsky, Noam . 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, Noam . 1966. Cartesian linguistics. New York: Harcourt.
Chomsky, Noam . 1967. Recent contributions to the theory of innate ideas. Synthese 17. 2–11.
Chomsky, Noam . 1968. Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.
Chomsky, Noam . 1972a. Problems of knowledge and freedom: The Russell lectures. New York: Vintage Books.
Chomsky, Noam . 1972b. Psychology and ideology. Cognition 1. 11–46.
Chomsky, Noam . 1978. Language development, human intelligence, and social organization. In W. Feinberg (ed.) Equality and social policy. Champaing, IL: University of Illinois Press. 120–143.
Chomsky, Noam . 1980. Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, Noam . 1986. Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger.
Chomsky, Noam . 1988. Language and problems of knowledge: The Nicaraguan lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chomsky, Noam . 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36. 1–22.
Chomsky, Noam . 2015. What kind of creatures are we? New York: Columbia University Press.
Chomsky, Noam and John McGilvray. 2012. The science of language: Interviews with Noam Chomsky. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark, Andy . 1989. Microcognitio. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
Clark, Eve V. and Herbert H. Clark. 1977. Psychology and language: An introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Cohen, David . 2004. Psychologists on psychology. London: Hodder and Stroughton.
Cohen-Cole, John . 2005. The reflexivity of cognitive science: the scientist as model of human nature. History of the Human Sciences 18. 107–139.
Csibra, Gergely and György Gergely. 2007. ‘Obsessed with goals’: Fuctions and mechanism of teleological interpretation of actions in humans. Acta Psychologica 124. 60–78.
Csibra, Gergely and György Gergely. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13. 148–153.
Cutler, Anne (ed.). 2005. Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones. Mah-wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Elman, Jeff, Elizabeth Bates, Mark Johnson, Annette Karmiloff-Smith, Domenico Parisi and Kim Plunkett. 1996. Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fillenbaum, Samuel . 1973. Syntactic factors in memory? The Hague: Mouton.
Flores d'Arcais , Giovanni B. 1974. Is there a memory for sentences? Acta Psychologica 38. 33–58.
Fodor, Jerry A . 1975. The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Fodor, Jerry A. 1981. Representations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, Jerry A . 1983. The modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, Jerry A . 1985. Fodor's guide to mental representation: The intelligent auntie's vademecum. Mind 94. 76–100.
Fodor, Jerry A . 2008. LOT2: The language of thought revisited. Oxford: Clarendon.
Forgács, Bálint . 2010. Könyvismertetés [Book review]: Jerry A. Fodor: LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited, 2008. Magyar Pszichológiai Szemle 65. 555–563.
Forster, Kenneth I. and Ilmar Olbrei. 1973. Semantic heuristics and syntactic analysis. Cognition 2. 319–347.
Friederici, Angela D . 2002. Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6. 78–84.
Gardner, Howard . 1985. The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.
Gentner, Daniel . 2010. Psychology in cognitive science: 1978–2038. Topics in Cognitive Science 2. 328–344.
Gervain, Judit . 2014. Developmental science and the nature–nurture issue: The case of language. In Cs. Pléh, G. Csibra and P. Richerson (eds.) Naturalistic approaches to culture. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó. 209–221.
Green, C. David . 1996. Where did the word “cognitive” come from anyway? Canadian Psychology 37. 31–39.
Green, Judith . 1973. Psycholinguistics: Chomsky and psychology. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Greenwood, John D . 1999. Understanding the “cognitive revolution” in psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 35. 1–22.
Hauser, Marc D., Noam Chomsky and W. Tecumseh Fitch. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298. 1569–1579.
Hebb, D. O . 1949. The organization of behavior; a neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.
Herrnstein, Richard J . 1973. I.Q. in the meritocracy. Boston, MA: Little Brown.
Hilgard, Ernest R . 1980. The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 16. 107–114.
Hintikka, Jako . 1991. Overcoming “overcoming metaphysics through logical analysis of language” through logical analysis of language. Dialectica 45. 203–218.
Hinzen, Wolfram . 2017. Universal grammar and philosophy of mind. In A. Ledgeway and I. G. Roberts (eds.) Cambridge handbook of historical syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 37–60.
Hubel, David H. and Thorton N. Wiesel. 1959. Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology 148. 574–591.
Jackendoff, Ray . 1992. Languages of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Jakobson, Roman . 1941. Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Uppsala: Almquist & Wiksell. (In English: Child Language, Aphasia and Phonological Universals, The Hague: Mouton, 1968.)
Jensen, Arthur . 1969. How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review 39. 1–123.
Johnson-Laird, Philip N. and Rosemary Stevenson. 1970. Memory for syntax. Nature 227. 412.
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette . 1992. Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Katz, Jerrold J . 1964. Mentalism in linguistics. Language 40. 124–137.
Katz, Jerrold J. and Paul Postal. 1964. An integrated theory of linguistic descriptions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kenesei, István . 2013. The role of creativity in the cognitive turn in linguistics. International Review of Pragmatics 5. 270–291.
Kuhn, Thomas . 1962. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kuhn, Thomas . 1970. The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd, enl. ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lachman, Robert, James L. Lachman and Robert C. Butterfield. 1979. Cognitive psychology and information processing: An introduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lashley, K . 1951. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffres (ed.) Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. New York: Wiley. 112–131.
Lenneberg, Eric H. (ed.). 1967. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.
Leslie, Allan M . 1987. Pretense and representation: Origins of a “theory of mind”. Psychological Review 94. 412–426.
Lorenz, Konrad . 1941/1962. Kant's doctrine of the a priori in the light of contemporary biology. In L. von Bertalanffy and A. Rapoport (eds.) General systems: Yearbook of the Society for General Systems Research. Vol. VII. New York: Society for General Systems Research. 23–35.
Mandler, George . 2002a. Interesting times. An encounter with the 20th century. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mandler, George . 2002b. Origins of the cognitive revolution. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 38. 339–353.
Marr, David . 1982. Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.
Marslen-Wilson, William D. and Lorainne Komisarjevsky Tyler. 1980. The temporal structure of spoken language understanding. Cognition 8. 1–71.
McLafferty, Claire L . 2006. Examining unproven assumptions of Galton's nature–nurture paradigm. American Psychologist 61. 177–178.
Mehler, Jacques . 1969. Psycholinguistique et grammaire générative. Langages 4. 1–13.
Miller, George A . 1962. Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist 17. 748–762.
Miller, George A . 1990. The place of language in a scientific psychology. Psychological Science 1. 7–14.
Miller, George A . 2003. The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7. 141–144.
Miller, George A. and Noam Chomsky. 1963. Finitary models of language users. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush and E. Galanter (eds.) Handbook of mathematical psychology, vol. 2. New York: Wiley. 419–491.
Miller, George A., Eugene Galanter and Karl H. Pribram. 1960. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.
Newell, Alan, Herbert A. Simon and James J. Shaw. 1958. Elements of a theory of problem solving. Psychological Review 65. 151–166.
O'Donohue, William, Karl E. Ferguson and Adam E. Naugle. 2003. The structure of the cognitive revolution: An examination from the philosophy of science. The Behavior Analyst 26. 85–110.
Olson, Gary A., Lester Faigley and Noam Chomsky. 1991. Language, politics, and composition. Journal of Advanced Composition 11. 1–35.
Palermo, David . 1971. Is a scientific revolution taking place in psychology? Science Studies 1. 135–155.
Pinker, Steven . 1991. Rules of language. Science 253. 530–535.
Pinker, Steven . 1999. Words and rules. New York: Basic Books.
Pinker, Steven . 2002. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Penguin.
Pléh, Csaba . 1976. Ponjatie kreativnosti v psichologi i v sovremennom jazikoznanii. Annales Universitatis Budapestiensis Sectio Paedagogica et Psychologica 5. 90–95.
Pléh, Csaba . 2003. Narrativity in text construction and self construction. Neohelicon 30. 187–205.
Pléh, Csaba . 2008. History and theories of the mind. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó.
Pléh, Csaba . 2009. Chomsky és a pszichológia [Chomsky and psychology]. Magyar Tudomány 170. 1075–1081.
Pléh, Csaba . 2018. Narrative psychology as cultural psychology. In G. Jovanović, L. Allolio-Näcke and C. Ratner (eds.) The challenges of cultural psychology: Historical legacies and future responsibilities. London: Routledge. 237–249.
Pléh, Csaba . 2019. Chomsky és a pszichológia: a nyelv mentális realitása és az egyéni különbségek [Chomsky and psychology: The mental reality of language and individual differences]. In I. Kenesei (ed.) Nyelv, biológia, szabadság. A 90 éves Chomsky jelentősége a tudományban és azon túl [Language, biology, freedom. The importance of the 90-year-old Chomsky in science and beyond]. Budapest: Gondolat. 55–83.
Pléh, Csaba and Lilia Gurova. 2013. Existing and would-be accounts of the history of cognitive science: An introduction. In Cs. Pléh, L. Gurova and R. László (eds.) New perspectives on the history of cognitive science. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó. 1–34.
Postal, Paul . 1964. Constituent structure: A study of contemporary models of syntactic description. The Hague: Mouton.
Quine, Willard van Orman . 1969. Ontological relativity and other essays. New York: Columbia University Press.
Rumelhart, David E . 1975. Notes on a schema for stories. In D. G. Bobrow and A. N. Collins (eds.) Representation and understanding. New York: Academic Press. 211–236.
Rumelhart, David E., James L. McClelland and the PDP Research Group (eds.). 1986. Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructures of cognition. Vol. 1: Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Ryle, Gilbert . 1949. The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.
Sachs, Jacqueline Strunk . 1967. Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse. Perception and Psychophysics 2. 437–442.
Skinner, Burhus F . 1963. Operant behavior. American Psychologist 18. 503–515.
Spelke, Elisabeth S. 2000. Core knowledge. American Psychologist 55. 1233–1243.
Tolman, Edward C. 1932. Purposive behavior in animals and me. New York: Century.
Tolman, Edward C . 1948. Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55. 189–208.
Tomasello, Michael . 2003. Constructing a language. A language based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ullman, Michael Thomas . 2001. A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model. Nature Review Neuroscience 2. 717–726.
Weimer, Walter B. and David S. Palermo. 1973. Paradigms and normal science in psychology. Social Studies of Science 3. 211–244.