Authors:
Cathy Kwok Department of Psychology, Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Search for other papers by Cathy Kwok in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jessica R. Grisham School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Kensington, NSW, Australia

Search for other papers by Jessica R. Grisham in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Melissa M. Norberg Department of Psychology, Centre for Emotional Health, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Search for other papers by Melissa M. Norberg in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Open access

Background and aims

People who hoard form intense attachments to their possessions and save items for sentimental and instrumental reasons. Feeling socially excluded may encourage these individuals to anthropomorphize objects (i.e., perceive them as human-like) to fulfill unmet belonging needs, which may increase the sentimental and instrumental values of objects, and then lead to stronger object attachment.

Methods

We randomly assigned 331 participants with excessive acquisition tendencies to be excluded, included, or overincluded in an online ball-tossing game before presenting them with five objects that had human characteristics. Participants then completed measures assessing anthropomorphism, sentimental and instrumental values, and object attachment.

Results

Inconsistent with this study hypothesis, socially excluded participants did not rate unowned objects as more human-like than the included or overincluded participants; however, stronger anthropomorphism predicted greater instrumental and sentimental values, which then predicted greater object attachment.

Discussion and conclusions

Sentimental and instrumental values may explain how stronger anthropomorphism may lead to greater object attachment. Learning that leads to anthropomorphism may help us better understand object attachment.

Abstract

Background and aims

People who hoard form intense attachments to their possessions and save items for sentimental and instrumental reasons. Feeling socially excluded may encourage these individuals to anthropomorphize objects (i.e., perceive them as human-like) to fulfill unmet belonging needs, which may increase the sentimental and instrumental values of objects, and then lead to stronger object attachment.

Methods

We randomly assigned 331 participants with excessive acquisition tendencies to be excluded, included, or overincluded in an online ball-tossing game before presenting them with five objects that had human characteristics. Participants then completed measures assessing anthropomorphism, sentimental and instrumental values, and object attachment.

Results

Inconsistent with this study hypothesis, socially excluded participants did not rate unowned objects as more human-like than the included or overincluded participants; however, stronger anthropomorphism predicted greater instrumental and sentimental values, which then predicted greater object attachment.

Discussion and conclusions

Sentimental and instrumental values may explain how stronger anthropomorphism may lead to greater object attachment. Learning that leads to anthropomorphism may help us better understand object attachment.

Introduction

Hoarding disorder (HD) is characterized by difficulty discarding possessions regardless of their actual value, which then results in excessive clutter that leads to significant distress and impairment for individuals who live within and nearby the affected home (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Although more than two thirds of individuals with HD excessively acquire items, almost all research into hoarding has focused on discarding difficulties (Frost, Rosenfield, Steketee, & Tolin, 2013; Frost, Tolin, Steketee, Fitch, & Selbo-Bruns, 2009; Timpano et al., 2011). The limited research into excessive acquisition has shown that individuals with acquiring difficulties experience more severe hoarding problems and social impairment in comparison to non-acquirers (Frost et al., 2013; Norberg, Crone, Kwok, & Grisham, 2018; Timpano et al., 2011). These findings suggest that examining factors associated with excessive acquisition may be important in improving existing treatment for individuals with HD who also have acquiring difficulties.

The cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding posits that individuals who hoard demonstrate excessive object attachment to their possessions and save them due to elevated perceptions of an item’s sentimental and/or instrumental values (Frost & Hartl, 1996). According to Frost and Steketee (2008), sentimental value refers to the symbolic meaning of an item and its ability to provide comfort and support, due to its association with an important person or event. Instrumental value, on the other hand, refers to judgments about the potential use, function, or need for a possession (Frost & Steketee, 2008). Although the model specifies that individuals who hoard acquire and like objects due to their greatly perceived sentimental and instrumental values, it does not specify the process that leads to these extreme judgment values. Nor do compulsive buying theories, which focus on a preoccupation with buying and lack of impulse control (O’Guinn & Faber, 1989; Ridgway, Kukar-Kinney, & Monroe, 2008).

Examining belonging and self-esteem needs may help improve our understanding of why individuals with HD assign elevated judgment values to objects. Social psychologists posit that individuals have an innate need to maintain interpersonal relationships to achieve a sense of belongingness and to maintain positive self-evaluations, and thus, social exclusion motivates people to seek out positive social contact (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Leary & Baumeister, 2000; Leary, Tambor, Terdal, & Downs, 1995; Park & Maner, 2009). In support of this, social psychological research has shown that socially excluded individuals report a diminished sense of belonging, control, and self-esteem, and express greater interest in turning to others to renew a sense of social connection in comparison to non-excluded individuals (Maner, DeWall, Baumeister, & Schaller, 2007; Williams & Zadro, 2001).

As individuals with HD often suffer from interpersonal problems and are socially isolated, their ability to gain guidance or support from loved ones when distressed may be limited (Grisham, Steketee, & Frost, 2008; Medard & Kellett, 2014; Nordsletten, de la Cruz, Billotti, & Mataix-Cols, 2013). As a result, individuals who hoard may form a strong attachment to objects due to recurrent unmet esteem and belonging needs (Fromm, 1947). This pattern of turning to objects to fulfill interpersonal needs begins in childhood when parents are unavailable, as children form an emotional bond to human-like objects as a parental replacement (Gjersoe, Hall, & Hood, 2015). In adulthood, research using non-clinical samples has shown that socially excluded individuals shop to fulfill unmet interpersonal needs (Lastovicka & Sirianni, 2011) and prefer products that represent their affiliation to a group (Mead, Baumeister, Stillman, Rawn, & Vohs, 2011). Moreover, research on social inclusion has also shown that undergraduates who are instructed to write about a time they felt supported assigned less value to owned and novel items in comparison to those who wrote about a positive restaurant experience (Clark et al., 2011). As many individuals with HD are socially isolated, deriving a sense of security in objects may help compensate for unmet social needs.

One explanation for the ability of objects to substitute for human connections is anthropomorphism. Anthropomorphism involves attributing human-like qualities to objects, such as free will or agency (Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2007). Research has found that undergraduate students attributed more human-like personality traits onto inanimate objects after being primed to recall a time they felt lonely compared to undergraduates who were not primed with loneliness (Bartz, Tchalova, & Fenerci, 2016; Eyssel & Reich, 2013). Chronically lonely individuals have also been shown to assign more human-like traits to inanimate objects than less lonely individuals (Epley, Akalis, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2008). As research using non-clinical and subclinical hoarding populations has shown that greater anthropomorphic tendencies are positively associated with greater acquisition tendencies (Burgess, Graves, & Frost, 2018; Timpano & Shaw, 2013) and greater overall hoarding severity (Neave, Jackson, Saxton, & Hönekopp, 2015; Neave, Tyson, McInnes, & Hamilton, 2016), it is possible that anthropomorphism may increase the value of objects for those with HD. However, this has not yet been studied in the context of social exclusion.

Exploring general and situational-dependent determinants that influence anthropomorphism may provide a better understanding of how anthropomorphism may impact object value. In regard to general determinants, social and consumer psychological researchers have shown that people are more likely to anthropomorphize objects that share physical or schematic similarities to humans (e.g., Aggarwal & McGill, 2007) and that they are likely to evaluate humanized brands more positively than non-humanized brands (e.g., Delbaere, McQuarrie, & Phillips, 2011). Epley et al.’s (2007) three-factor theory of anthropomorphism highlights the possible role of situational-dependent determinants. Their sociality motivation tenet proposes that social disconnection instills an unmet need for belonging, which prompts anthropomorphism, which then provides an individual with a sense of human connection, whereas their effectance motivation tenet proposes that human-like objects provide a mechanism for establishing control and understanding the world (e.g., My car didn’t start because it wasn’t in the mood to deal with traffic today). Consequently, socially disconnected individuals should be motivated to anthropomorphize objects around them to attain comfort and to establish control (Bartz et al., 2016; MacInnis & Folkes, 2017; Waytz et al., 2010), which should then increase the sentimental and instrumental values of objects. Prior research has provided some support for Epley et al.’s theoretical tenets. First, self-report studies have shown that greater anxious interpersonal attachment (chronic fear of abandonment) is associated with greater acquisition tendencies (Norberg et al., 2018) and hoarding severity (Medard & Kellett, 2014). Research also has shown that acute social exclusion leads to greater anthropomorphism (Epley et al., 2008), and stronger anthropomorphic tendencies are associated with greater object attachment and acquiring difficulties (Burgess et al., 2018; Keefer, 2016; Norberg et al., 2018; Timpano & Shaw, 2013). However, prior research has not experimentally examined if social disconnection leads to greater object attachment through anthropomorphism’s effects on instrumental and sentimental values.

The present study

This study used an experimental manipulation of social exclusion to test the hypothesis that individuals are more likely to anthropomorphize unowned items after being socially rejected, as opposed to being included, and that anthropomorphizing objects would lead to stronger sentimental and instrumental values, which would lead to greater object attachment. We incorporated an overinclusion condition to provide a direct recommendation on how treatment for HD might be tailored to reduce anthropomorphism and ultimately excessive object attachment. We examined individuals with acquiring problems and unowned items to fill a rather large gap in the literature.

Methods

Participants and procedure

Individuals were recruited for a study on “why people like gifts” using flyers disseminated on and around campus, online, and during talks on HD from October 2016 to August 2017. These individuals completed the Saving Inventory – Revised (SI-R; Frost, Steketee, & Grisham, 2004) online to determine their eligibility for the study. Participants were eligible if they scored above the clinical cut-off on the excessive acquisition subscale of the SI-R (9+; Frost et al., 2004). Three hundred and sixty-one participants above the age of 18 years eventually completed this study. The majority of these participants scored above other SI-R clinical cut-offs: 62.9% exceeded the discarding difficulties clinical cut-off (SI-R ≥ 14), 32.6% exceeded the excessive clutter clinical cut-off (SI-R ≥ 17), and 55.9% exceeded the SI-R total score clinical cut-off (SI-R ≥ 41; Frost & Hristova, 2011; Frost et al., 2004). After providing informed consent, participants were randomly assigned to be excluded, included, or overincluded in a ball-throwing task (Cyberball; Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000). Immediately after the task, participants completed the Need Threat Questionnaire (Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, 2004), which measured their sense of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence.

Following this, participants were presented with five potential “gifts” individually and in a randomized order (adapted from Mead et al., 2011). As people are more likely to anthropomorphize objects with physical human-like characteristics (Aggarwal & McGill, 2007), we replicated previous studies’ procedures using items with distinct human-like features (e.g., eyes and mouth; Bartz et al., 2016; Epley et al., 2008). These items included an M&M’s on A Couch M&M Dispenser, a clay plant pot with a painted face, a tea man infuser, a wine holder in the form of a waiter, and robot salt/pepper shakers (Appendix A). We used unowned items as we aimed to examine what factors underlie object acquisition. After viewing each object, participants completed questionnaires assessing its attributes and likeability. Participants completed a manipulation check after viewing all gifts to identify if they were aware of the purpose of the study before being debriefed. All participants were reimbursed with course credit or cash for participation in the study.

Social exclusion manipulation

Cyberball is a validated virtual ball-throwing task widely used to study social exclusion (Williams et al., 2000). As with previous Cyberball studies (e.g., Zadro et al., 2004), participants were informed that the Cyberball task was a mental visualization task to prepare them for the experiment. During the Cyberball task (version 4; https://cyberball.wikispaces.com/), participants imagined themselves playing a ball-tossing game with two confederates named Sarah and Matthew, whom the participant was led to believe were participants from another university. This fallacy was strengthened by the experimenter making a staged phone call to ensure participants at the other university were ready to begin (Zadro, Boland, & Richardson, 2006). Each player was depicted as an animated digital avatar within the task. Participants were instructed to throw the ball to one of the other players after receiving it. Following procedures used in previous studies (e.g., Van Beest & Williams, 2006; Zadro et al., 2006), Cyberball was truncated to 30 throws to avoid fatigue. Participants who were randomly assigned to the overinclusion condition received the ball half of the time (roughly 15 times), participants who were randomly assigned to the inclusion condition received the ball one third of the time (roughly 10 times), and participants who were in the exclusion condition received the ball twice before being excluded thereafter (e.g., Stillman et al., 2009).

Measures

Hoarding behaviors

The SI-R (Frost et al., 2004) is a self-reported measure that assesses excessive acquisition, clutter, and difficulty discarding. Participants responded to 23 items on a scale ranging from 0 (none/not at all) to 4 (almost all/extreme). The SI-R has demonstrated good reliability for all three subscales in non-clinical and clinical samples (Moulding, Nedeljkovic, Kyrios, Osborne, & Mogan, 2017; Norberg, Keyan, & Grisham, 2015). The discarding and clutter subscales demonstrated good reliability in this study (α = .82 and .89, respectively), but the acquisition subscale demonstrated poor reliability (α = .55) likely due to the restricted range of scores on this subscale (Cortina, 1993).

Social exclusion manipulation check

Participants were asked to complete the Need Threat Questionnaire (Zadro et al., 2004) and the manipulation check question, “To what extent were you included by other participants during the game?” Reponses could range from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much so) and “excluded” participants had to score from 1 to 3, “included” participants had to score over 4, and “overincluded” participants had to score over 7 for their data to be included in study analyses. The 12 items from the Need Threat Questionnaire (Zadro et al., 2004) required participants to assess their levels of four fundamental psychological needs (belonging, control, meaningful existence, and self-esteem) on 9-point scales, from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much). Items for each need were averaged to obtain subscale scores and higher scores referred to less belonging, less control, lower self-esteem, and less meaningfulness. In this study, items for belongingness (α = .79), control (α = .82), self-esteem (α = .83), and meaningful existence (α = .72) demonstrated adequate reliability.

Anthropomorphism

Participants rated the degree to which the inanimate objects possessed human-like characteristics using the Anthropomorphic Mental State ratings scale (AMS; Epley et al., 2008). Using five items, the AMS (Epley et al., 2008) assessed the extent to which each potential “gift” had a mind of its own, intentions, free will, consciousness, and emotions, on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much). Responses for each object were individually summed. These object totals were then averaged to derive a total AMS score, with higher scores demonstrating stronger anthropomorphism. The AMS has demonstrated adequate internal consistency (Epley et al., 2008) and in the current sample, it demonstrated good internal consistency for each individual gift (ranged from α = .89–.90).

Object value

To assess sentimental and instrumental values, we used nine items derived from Mead et al.’s (2011) study on social exclusion and consumer behavior, and Richins’ (1994) study on object value. Two authors categorized these items into two categories separately using the definitions of sentimental and instrumental values provided by Frost and Steketee (2008). The two authors resolved discrepancies through discussion. The final sentimental value subscale consisted of six items addressing the emotional meaning of an object and the association between the object to a particular social connection (“it would provide comfort and emotional security,” “it would remind me of my relationship with a particular person,” “it would express what is unique about me, different from others,” “it would remind me of my family or a group of people I belong to,” “it would remind me of particular events or places,” and “it would be a record of my personal history”) using a 7-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much so). In the current sample, interitem correlations were medium to large for each individual gift (correlation coefficients ranged from r = .33–.65) (Hemphill, 2003). The sentimental subscale demonstrated good internal consistency for each individual gift (ranged from α = .83–.85). The final instrumental value subscale consisted of three items addressing general perceptions of the potential utility or function of an object (“it would make me socially desirable, it would impress others,” “it would be practically useful,” “it would provide an opportunity to improve my life”) on the same 7-point scale. In the current sample, interitem correlations were small to medium for each individual gift (correlation coefficients ranged from r = .27–.48). The instrumental subscale demonstrated poor to adequate internal consistency for each gift (ranged from α = .60–.70). This low Cronbach’s α coefficient is possibly due to the small number of items on the instrumental value subscale (Cortina, 1993) or the variability in instrumental value items on the scale.

Object attachment

Participants were asked three questions to assess object attachment: “How much do you like the object?,” “How much do you desire to own the object?,” and “How much would you like to receive the object as a gift?” These questions were created specifically for this study and were scored on a 7-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much so) and then averaged. The questions demonstrated excellent reliability for each individual gift (ranged from α = .92–.93).

Demographics

Participants were asked to complete a brief demographic questionnaire indicating their gender, age, ethnicity, whether they were currently studying at university, and the amount of tertiary education they had completed.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 24.0 (IBM Cooperation, New York, USA). Before testing study hypotheses, we checked the data for statistical assumptions and usability. We then examined the effect of our social exclusion manipulation on variables of interest before running correlational analyses to examine bivariate relationships between our variables of interest.

In our final model, we used Model 4 of the PROCESS v 2.16 macro (Hayes, 2013) to test a parallel mediation model between X and Y through M1 and M2. PROCESS utilizes bias-corrected bootstrapping to access the indirect and direct effects of the variables by examining approximations of the estimated product of coefficients in the indirect paths (Hayes, 2013). To determine the significance of the indirect effect, 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were used, with confidence intervals resampled 10,000 times for each analysis (Hayes, 2013; Preacher & Hayes, 2008). To improve our confidence in the proposed model, we also ran models that reversed our mediators with our dependent variable (Sheets & Braver, 1999).

Ethics

The study procedures were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The institutional review board at Macquarie University approved the study. All participants were informed about the study and provided informed consent prior to initiating the study. They were also provided with an opportunity to reconsent for the use of their data in research when debriefed about the use of deception at the end of the experimental session.

Results

Social exclusion manipulation check

Of the 361 participants who completed the study, five Cyberball “excluded” participants were excluded from the study, because they did not report low feelings of inclusion after Cyberball. Five “included” and six “overincluded” participants were also excluded for not feeling included enough. A further 14 participants were excluded as they reported being fully aware of the aims of the study before being debriefed. Cut-off criteria were derived from mean manipulation check scores reported in previous studies (e.g., Williams et al., 2000; Zadro et al., 2004). Descriptive characteristics and group comparisons on variables of interest for the final sample (N = 331) are presented in Table 1. Of the 331 participants, 107 participants were randomly allocated to the exclusion condition, 111 to the inclusion condition, and 113 to the overinclusion condition.

Table 1.

Means, standard deviations, count, and percentages for demographic characteristics and manipulation check

All (N = 331) Exclusion (n = 107) Inclusion (n = 111) Overinclusion (n = 113) Statistics p value
Sex (% female) 256 (77.3%) 76 (71.0%) 92 (82.9%) 88 (77.9%) χ2(2) = 4.40 .111
Age (years) 22.88 (7.75) 22.48 (7.73) 22.39 (6.61) 23.75 (8.74) F(2, 328) = 1.09 .339
Ethnicity χ2(8) = 5.20 .736
 Caucasian 104 (31.4%) 37 (34.6%) 31 (27.9%) 36 (31.9%)
 European 35 (10.6%) 11 (10.3%) 8 (7.2%) 16 (14.2%)
 Middle Eastern 19 (5.7%) 5 (4.7%) 8 (7.2%) 6 (5.3%)
 Asian 143 (43.2%) 44 (41.1%) 54 (48.6%) 45 (39.8%)
 Other 30 (9.1%) 10 (9.3%) 10 (9.0%) 10 (8.8%)
Completed tertiary education (years) 2.07 (1.26) 1.96 (1.24) 2.05 (1.28) 2.18 (1.26) F(2, 327) = 0.80 .448
SI-R Total 43.42 (12.17) 43.22 (13.01) 43.71 (11.66) 43.33 (11.95) F(2, 328) = 0.05 .951
 Acquisition 14.78 (3.40) 14.67 (3.42) 14.78 (3.44) 15.07 (3.35) F(2, 328) = 0.86 .422
 Discarding 15.08 (4.84) 14.89 (5.03) 15.04 (4.82) 15.31 (4.72) F(2, 328) = 0.22 .805
 Clutter 13.59 (6.82) 13.86 (7.80) 13.94 (6.25) 12.98 (6.37) F(2, 328) = 0.67 .510
Manipulation checka 2.41 (0.82)a 6.67 (1.48)b 8.43 (0.95)c F(2, 328) = 832.10 <.001**
Need threat
 Belonging 7.38 (1.22)a 3.54 (1.32)b 2.76 (1.34)c F(2, 328) = 397.58 <.001**
 Control 7.30 (1.09)a 3.47 (1.50)b 2.58 (1.45)c F(2, 328) = 369.90 <.001**
 Self-esteem 5.93 (1.72)a 2.99 (1.55)b 2.40 (1.13)c F(2, 328) = 177.21 <.001**
 Meaningful existence 6.70 (1.55)a 3.18 (1.28)b 2.61 (1.41)c F(2, 328) = 266.78 <.001**
Anthropomorphism 12.19 (6.47) 12.01 (6.23) 13.12 (7.00) 11.45 (6.06) F(2, 328) = 1.94 .146

Note. Parameter estimates in each row that share superscripts do not differ significantly. χ2: chi-square, F: F-statistic.

The manipulation check score represents an average of two items on a 9-point scale (ranging from 1 = not at all to 9 = very much so): “To what extent were you included…” and “Rejected – Accepted” (bipolar scale). A higher score refers to greater inclusion.

As shown in Table 1, the groups differed in their four need threats and perceptions of inclusion after Cyberball, p < .001. Contrast analyses demonstrated that excluded participants reported feeling less included than included participants, t(328) = −28.03, p < .001, and that included participants felt less included than overincluded participants, t(328) = 11.77, p < .001. As for threats to the four fundamental psychological needs, excluded participants reported lower feelings of belongingness, t(328) = 21.87, p < .001; having less control, t(328) = 20.76, p < .001; less meaningful existence, t(328) = 18.38, p < .001; and lower self-esteem, t(328) = 14.64, p < .001 than included participants, whereas included participants reported lower feelings of belongingness, t(328) = −4.56, p < .001; having less control, t(328) = −4.91, p < .001; less meaningful existence, t(328) = −3.00, p = .003; and lower self-esteem, t(328) = −2.98, p = .003, than overincluded participants.

Social exclusion analyses

Before testing our hypothesized model, we ran preliminary tests to assess for statistically significant relationships between the study variables. Although all objects in this study had human-like qualities, people who compulsively acquire are known to view each object as infinitely unique (Kyrios, Frost, & Steketee, 2004). Thus, we first checked scores for anthropomorphism, object attachment, and value to understand, if the objects were perceived differently. Although the items differed slightly in anthropomorphism, sentimental and instrumental values, and object attachment (Appendix B), a mixed model analysis of variance revealed that anthropomorphism, sentimental value, and instrumental value scores for each individual gift did not differ as a function of our social exclusion manipulation (all ps > .05). Thus, we averaged the scores for each individual gift to create mean anthropomorphism, sentimental value, instrumental value, and object attachment scores. Our social exclusion manipulation did not affect anthropomorphism, F(2, 328) = 1.94, p = .146; instrumental value, F(2, 328) = 2.64, p = .073; or object attachment, F(2, 328) = 1.45, p = .237; however, it affected sentimental value, such that overincluded and included participants reported greater sentimental attachment to the objects than excluded participants, F(1, 328) =6.28, p = .013. As the magnitude of the effect was small, ηp2 = 0.02, the social exclusion variable was dropped from further analyses.

Although the social exclusion manipulation did not meaningfully affect the value of objects or participants’ attachment to the objects, we conducted an unplanned multiple regression model to determine if threats to the four psychological needs were predictive of anthropomorphism. Threats to belongingness, control, and meaningful existence were not associated with greater anthropomorphism (all p’s ≥ .331); however, greater threat to one’s self-esteem was associated with greater anthropomorphism, b = 0.14, SE = 0.055, β = 0.134, t(329) = 2.46, p = .014.

Pearson’s correlation coefficients were then calculated for the whole sample to identify the size of bivariate relationships between anthropomorphism, instrumental value, sentimental value, and object attachment for the overall sample. Our main variables of interest did not differ depending on gender. In regard to ethnicity, Caucasian participants rated each individual item as less human-like than Asian participants, F(1, 326) = 13.66, p < .001. We did not complete further analyses by gender or ethnicity. As we identified positive bivariate relationships between our variables of interest (Table 2), we proceeded with running a parallel multiple mediation model (Hayes, 2013). Parallel multiple mediation tested the hypothesis that sentimental and instrumental value may contribute to object attachment.

Table 2.

Count, percentages, and zero-order correlations for measures of interest

Mean (SD) Anthropomorphism Instrumental Sentimental Object attachment
1. Anthropomorphism 12.19 (6.47)
2. Instrumental 3.26 (0.94) .410**
3. Sentimental 3.05 (1.13) .441** .656**
4. Object attachment 4.52 (1.04) .201** .609** .455**

Note. Pearson’s correlations are noted in the table. SD: standard deviation of the mean.

p < .01.

Parallel mediation analyses

Figure 1 presents the results from the parallel mediation tests using ordinary least squares path analysis (Model 4; Hayes, 2013) to examine the relationship between anthropomorphism and object attachment through instrumental and sentimental values. As shown in Figure 1, stronger anthropomorphism predicted greater instrumental value, t(329) =7.81, p < .001, 95% CI [0.04, 0.07] and greater sentimental value, t(329) = 8.70, p < .001, 95% CI [0.06, 0.09]. Greater instrumental value, t(327) = 10.34, p < .001, 95% CI [0.51, 0.74] and greater sentimental value, t(327) = 2.09, p = .037, 95% CI [0.01, 0.22] predicted greater object attachment. A bias-corrected bootstrap confidence interval for the indirect effect based on 10,000 bootstrap intervals was entirely above zero for instrumental value, 95% CI [0.03, 0.05] and sentimental value, 95% CI [0.00, 0.02]. The strength of the indirect effects suggested that the indirect effect via instrumental value was greater than the indirect effect via sentimental value, 95% CI [0.01, 0.05]. There was no evidence of a direct effect (c’) between anthropomorphism and liking, 95% CI [−0.03, 0.00], though the total effect (c) was statistically significant, 95% CI [0.02, 0.05], which provides evidence for full mediation. In our reversed models with instrumental and sentimental values as the outcome variable, both models demonstrated partial mediation (direct effect/s: c’ = 0.22, SE = 0.01, 95% CI [0.01, 0.03]; c’ = 0.04, SE = 0.01, 95% CI [0.02, 0.05], respectively), increasing our confidence for the proposed model.


            Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Multiple (parallel) mediation model of anthropomorphism (X) to object attachment (Y) through instrumental (M1) and sentimental value (M2). Unstandardized coefficients and standard errors of the mean (SE) are displayed in the figure. Standardized coefficients are displayed in parentheses next to the unstandardized coefficients

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 4; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.98

Discussion

This study aimed to test whether acute social exclusion increased object attachment via anthropomorphism’s effects on an object’s perceived instrumental and sentimental values. When including, excluding, or overincluding participants in an online ball-tossing game, we did not find a relationship between social exclusion and anthropomorphism. However, consistent with our hypotheses, greater instrumental and sentimental values mediated the relationship between anthropomorphism and greater liking of unowned objects.

Overall, this study adds to the body of research that inconsistently links social exclusion and loneliness with anthropomorphism (Bartz et al., 2016; Claus & Warlop, 2011; Epley et al., 2008; Feng, 2016). There are a number of reasons why this study may have been unable to replicate previous findings that social exclusion leads to stronger anthropomorphic tendencies (Bartz et al., 2016; Epley et al., 2008). First, in regard to anthropomorphism, we showed participants five “gifts” that they were not allowed to keep. Anthropomorphizing and seeking comfort from an object that cannot be owned may be dangerous as it could lead to further social exclusion (Cacioppo, Norris, Decety, Monteleone, & Nusbaum, 2009; Feng, 2016; Park & Maner, 2009). Thus, socially excluded individuals may be more likely to anthropomorphize and seek comfort from objects that can be owned. Research on HD highlights the importance of ownership as individuals with HD only appear to have difficulty making decisions about personal items (Grisham, Norberg, Williams, Certoma, & Kadib, 2010; Tolin et al., 2012; Wincze, Steketee, & Frost, 2007). Second, the AMS (Epley et al., 2008) items are abstract and philosophical (e.g., “free will” and “consciousness”), which require complex deductive reasoning skills to answer (Neave et al., 2015). Dispositional measures have been revised in recent years to reduce this abstract nature (e.g., the Graves Anthropomorphic Task Scale, Burgess, et al., 2018; Anthropomorphism Questionnaire, Neave et al., 2015), but this has yet to be done for situational measures. Based on measures from the dispositional literature, items such as “this object has a friendly personality” may be easier for individuals to comprehend and answer (Burgess et al., 2018; Neave et al., 2015).

Our social exclusion manipulation may also explain why we did not find a relationship between social exclusion and anthropomorphism. First, we studied acute social exclusion, whereas other studies have focused on chronic loneliness (Bartz et al., 2016; Epley et al., 2008). While both loneliness and social exclusion lead to deficits in belongingness (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), these two forms of low belonging may not motivate the same behavior (Stillman et al., 2009). In addition, this study found that greater threats to one’s self-esteem, but not threats to belongingness, led to greater anthropomorphism. This suggests that examining threats to self-esteem may help us better understand what leads to anthropomorphism, and ultimately object attachment (Park & Maner, 2009). Second, attachment theory predicts that social exclusion only results in negative consequences when it is done by significant others (Bowlby, 1969; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2016). As participants in this study were rejected by “strangers,” our social exclusion manipulation may not have been sufficient to induce anthropomorphism. Manipulations that require participants to recall a time in which they were excluded or unsupported by a close friend or loved one (e.g., Keefer, Landau, Rothschild, & Sullivan, 2012; Mourey, Olson, & Yoon, 2017) may be more likely to induce anthropomorphism. Thus, future studies should allow participants to take home study items, use a measure of chronic loneliness, an anxious attachment priming manipulation, and a more concrete, contextual measure of anthropomorphism to clarify the relationship between social exclusion and anthropomorphism.

Our hypothesis that stronger anthropomorphism would predict greater sentimental and instrumental values, which would in turn lead to greater object attachment was supported. This finding is in line with theoretical models (Epley et al., 2007; Frost & Hartl, 1996), and with prior research documenting a link between anthropomorphism and comfort and security (Keefer, 2016; Neave et al., 2016). While the role of sentimental value in hoarding has been well-acknowledged within the literature (e.g., Frost & Hartl, 1996; Steketee, Frost, & Kyrios, 2003), a growing amount of research has demonstrated that individuals with HD acquire and save potentially useful items to avoid guilt associated with wasting useful objects (Frost, Steketee, Tolin, Sinopoli, & Ruby, 2015; Nordsletten et al., 2013). This suggests that examining an object’s perceived instrumental value is as important as understanding how individuals with HD might derive comfort from an anthropomorphized item. However, before being able to effectively integrate Epley et al.’s (2007) tenets to Frost and Hartl’s (1996) cognitive-behavioral model of HD, we need to better understand what mechanisms acutely lead to anthropomorphism. This should involve examining how threats to one’s self-esteem may motivate individuals to anthropomorphize and acquire new items. Consumer and social psychologists have posited that people purchase consumer goods that represent their own values in order to enhance their self-esteem (Arndt, Solomon, Kasser, & Sheldon, 2004; Richins, 1994). These goods may in turn form part of one’s extended self and reflect one’s personal identity (Belk, 1988). While difficulties with self-concept appear to be related to hoarding and compulsive buying (Frost, Kyrios, McCarthy, & Matthews, 2007), few studies have experimentally examined how threats to one’s self-esteem or self-concept may motivate individuals to seek social connection and attachment in objects. Exploring this may help us better understand what mechanisms lead to anthropomorphism and object attachment in HD.

Although cultural influences on anthropomorphism were not part of our main hypotheses, our finding that Asian participants had higher average anthropomorphism scores than Caucasian participants contributes to the limited research on culture and anthropomorphism. In the three-factor theory of anthropomorphism, Epley et al. (2007) posited that people from collectivist cultures may anthropomorphize more than people from individualistic cultures due to the value placed on social affiliation in collectivist cultures (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Similar to this study, Wang (2017) found that Asian undergraduates anthropomorphized their smartphones more than Caucasian participants, but found that cultural orientation, one’s personal affiliation toward collectivism and individualism, did not affect anthropomorphism. This suggests that the relationship between culture and anthropomorphism is complex and perhaps cannot be reduced to a simple dichotomy between collectivism and individualism, particularly in predominantly westernized samples (Wang, 2017).

Limitations

The results of this study should be interpreted considering its limitations. First, in regard to measurement issues, although we followed Frost and Steketee’s (2008) definitions for sentimental and instrumental values when categorizing items on our object value measure, our measure has not been validated. Our instrumental value subscale demonstrated α ratings of low reliability (α = .60–.70), and although this could be explained by the limited number of items on the subscale (Cortina, 1993), it is also possible that our instrumental value subscale was too broad or multidimensional (e.g., a scale that measures practical utility, functional ability, etc.; Schmitt, 1996). For example, items such as “…it would be practically useful” and “…it would impress others” may not target the same type of utilitarian value. As individuals with HD may anthropomorphize their possessions due to a strong sense of responsibility to protect and control their items (Frost, Hartl, Christian, & Williams, 1995), it may be beneficial to include items that address the functional ability of an item in establishing control before using exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses to examine the psychometric properties of this measure. Second, in this study, the SI-R acquisition subscale had poor internal consistency (α = .57). Although this could imply that the subscale is multidimensional (Schmitt, 1996), others have criticized the accuracy of Cronbach’s α in capturing the internal consistency of a measure (Sijtsma, 2009). In order to address this, future studies may choose to utilize structural equation modeling to better assess correlations of latent variables underlying acquisition in hoarding.

Third, although we chose to look at items with human-like physical characteristics, because they are more likely to be anthropomorphized than objects without these features (Aggarwal & McGill, 2007; Epley et al., 2007), we may not have needed to do this, because individuals with HD assign human-like characteristics to possessions regardless of their resemblance to humans (Frost & Hartl, 1996). As people with HD save clothes, books, and letters more frequently than novelty items (Mogan, Kyrios, Schweitzer, Yap, & Moulding, 2012), future studies should utilize such objects. Finally, many participants in this study were university students with SI-R acquisition scores in the subclinical to clinical range. As we did not include a diagnostic interview, we do not know how many participants met diagnostic criteria for HD. Thus, our findings may not be representative of a clinical hoarding population. Thus, caution should be made when drawing direct inferences of our findings to individuals with HD.

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, this study is the first experimental investigation that tests the tenets of Epley’s theory of anthropomorphism and Frost and Hartl’s (1996) cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding concurrently. Although we did not find a significant relationship between acute social exclusion and anthropomorphism, the study found cross-sectional empirical evidence that individuals with acquiring problems may like unowned objects they perceive as human-like as these items seem sentimental and useful. In order to obtain data necessary for expanding Frost and Hartl’s (1996) cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding, future research should include an anxious attachment priming manipulation, psychometrically validated object attachment and anthropomorphism measures, and allow participants to keep the objects examined in order to mimic real-life to a greater degree.

Authors’ contribution

MMN and JRG are responsible for the study concept and design and edited all drafts. CK wrote the first draft of the manuscript and conducted the statistical analyses. All authors contributed to the final version and approved of its submission.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Shanika Chandra and Sophie Peek for their efforts in data collection.

References

  • Aggarwal, P. , & McGill, A. L. (2007). Is that car smiling at me? Schema congruity as a basis for evaluating anthropomorphized products. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(4), 468479. doi:10.1086/518544

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Arndt, J. , Solomon, S. , Kasser, T. , & Sheldon, K. M. (2004). The urge to splurge: A terror management account of materialism and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3), 198212. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1403_2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bartz, J. A. , Tchalova, K. , & Fenerci, C. (2016). Reminders of social connection can attenuate anthropomorphism: A replication and extension of Epley, Akalis, Waytz, and Cacioppo (2008). Psychological Science, 27(12), 16441650. doi:10.1177/0956797616668510

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baumeister, R. F. , & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 139168. doi:10.1086/209154

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Version 1. New York, NY: Basic Books.

  • Burgess, A. M. , Graves, L. M. , & Frost, R. O. (2018). My possessions need me: Anthropomorphism and hoarding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(3), 340348. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12441

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Norris, C. J. , Decety, J. , Monteleone, G. , & Nusbaum, H. (2009). In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 8392. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, M. S. , Greenberg, A. , Hill, E. , Lemay, E. P. , Clark-Polner, E. , & Roosth, D. (2011). Heightened interpersonal security diminishes the monetary value of possessions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 359364. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Claus, B. , & Warlop, L. (2011). Once bitten, twice shy: Differences in social efficacy affect the perceived efficacy of anthropomorphizable products. In D. W. Dahl, G. V. Johar, & S. M. J. van Osselaer (Eds.), NA – Advances in consumer research (Vol. 38). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research. Retrieved from https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/16093/volumes/v38/NA-38

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is a coefficient alpha? An exanimation of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 98104. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delbaere, M. , McQuarrie, E. F. , & Phillips, B. J. (2011). Personification in advertising. Journal of Advertising, 40(1), 121130. doi:10.2753/JOA0091-3367400108

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Epley, N. , Akalis, S. , Waytz, A. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction: Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19(2), 114120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02056.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Epley, N. , Waytz, A. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114(4), 864886. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.864

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eyssel, F. & Reich, N. (2013). Loneliness makes the heart grow fonder (of robots) – On the effects of loneliness on psychological anthropomorphism. In ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (pp. 121122). Tokyo, Japan: IEEE.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Feng, W. (2016). When lonely people encounter anthropomorphic products. Social Behavior and Personality, 44(10), 16491660. doi:10.2224/sbp.2016.44.10.1649

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself: An inquiry into the psychology of ethics (Vol. 57). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

  • Frost, R. O. , & Hartl, T. L. (1996). A cognitive-behavioral model of compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(4), 341350. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00071-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Hartl, T. L. , Christian, R. , & Williams, N. (1995). The value of possessions in compulsive hoarding: Patterns of use and attachment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 897902. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00043-W

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , & Hristova, V. (2011). Assessment of hoarding. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(5), 456466. doi:10.1002/jclp.20790

  • Frost, R. O. , Kyrios, M. , McCarthy, K. D. , & Matthews, Y. (2007). Self-ambivalence and attachment to possessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21(3), 232242. doi:10.1891/088983907781494582

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Rosenfield, E. , Steketee, G. , & Tolin, D. F. (2013). An examination of excessive acquisition in hoarding disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 2(3), 338345. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.06.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , & Steketee, G. (2008). Compulsive hoarding. In J. S. Abramowitz, D. McKay, & S. Taylor (Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Subtypes and spectrum conditions (pp. 7693). New York, NY: Elsevier.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , & Grisham, J. (2004). Measurement of compulsive hoarding: Saving Inventory – Revised. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 11631182. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.07.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , Tolin, D. F. , Sinopoli, N. , & Ruby, D. (2015). Motives for acquiring and saving in hoarding disorder, OCD, and community controls. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 4, 5459. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.12.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Tolin, D. F. , Steketee, G. , Fitch, K. E. , & Selbo-Bruns, A. (2009). Excessive acquisition in hoarding. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 632639. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gjersoe, N. L. , Hall, E. L. , & Hood, B. (2015). Children attribute mental lives to toys when they are emotionally attached to them. Cognitive Development, 34, 2838. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.12.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grisham, J. R. , Norberg, M. M. , Williams, A. D. , Certoma, S. P. , & Kadib, R. (2010). Categorization and cognitive deficits in compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(9), 866872. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grisham, J. R. , Steketee, G. , & Frost, R. (2008). Interpersonal problems and emotional intelligence in compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety, 25(9), E63E71. doi:10.1002/da.20327

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitude of correlation coefficients. American Psychologist, 58(1), 7879. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.78

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keefer, L. A. (2016). Is there anybody out there? Trait anthropomorphism predicts the psychological benefits of a favorite belonging. Journal of Individual Differences, 37(4), 231238. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000210

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keefer, L. A. , Landau, M. J. , Rothschild, Z. K. , & Sullivan, D. (2012). Attachment to objects as compensation for close others’ perceived unreliability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 912917. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kyrios, M. , Frost, R. O. , & Steketee, G. (2004). Cognitions in compulsive buying and acquisition. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(2), 241258. doi:10.1023/B:COTR.0000021543.62799.32

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lastovicka, J. L. , & Sirianni, N. J. (2011). Truly, madly, deeply: Consumers in the throes of material possession love. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(2), 323342. doi:10.1086/658338

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, M. R. , & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 162. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, M. R. , Tambor, E. S. , Terdal, S. K. , & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 518530. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.518

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacInnis, D. J. , & Folkes, V. S. (2017). Humanizing brands: When brands seem to be like me, part of me, and in a relationship with me. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(3), 355374. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2016.12.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maner, J. K. , DeWall, C. N. , Baumeister, R. F. , & Schaller, M. (2007). Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the porcupine problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 4255. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.42

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Markus, H. R. , & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mead, N. L. , Baumeister, R. F. , Stillman, T. F. , Rawn, C. D. , & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Social exclusion causes people to spend and consume strategically in the service of affiliation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 902919. doi:10.1086/656667

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Medard, E. , & Kellett, S. (2014). The role of adult attachment and social support in hoarding disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42(5), 629633. doi:10.1017/S1352465813000659

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M. , & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mogan, C. , Kyrios, M. , Schweitzer, I. , Yap, K. , & Moulding, R. (2012). Phenomenology of hoarding – What is hoarded by individuals with hoarding disorder? Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 1(4), 306311. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.08.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moulding, R. , Nedeljkovic, M. , Kyrios, M. , Osborne, D. , & Mogan, C. (2017). Short- term cognitive-behavioral group treatment for hoarding disorder: A naturalistic treatment outcome study. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(1), 235244. doi:10.1002/cpp.2001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mourey, J. A. , Olson, J. G. , & Yoon, C. (2017). Products as pals: Engaging with anthropomorphic products mitigates the effects of social exclusion. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(2), 414431. doi:10.1093/jcr/ucx038

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neave, N. , Jackson, R. , Saxton, T. , & Hönekopp, J. (2015). The influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 214219. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.041

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neave, N. , Tyson, H. , McInnes, L. , & Hamilton, C. (2016). The role of attachment style and anthropomorphism in predicting hoarding behaviors in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 3337. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.067

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norberg, M. M. , Crone, C. , Kwok, C. , & Grisham, J. R. (2018). Anxious attachment and excessive acquisition: The mediating roles of anthropomorphism and distress tolerance. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(1), 171180. doi:10.1556/2006.7.2018.08

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norberg, M. M. , Keyan, D. , & Grisham, J. R. (2015). Mood influences the relationship between distress intolerance and discarding. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 7782. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.06.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nordsletten, A. E. , de la Cruz, L. F. , Billotti, D. , & Mataix-Cols, D. (2013). Finders keepers: The features differentiating hoarding disorder from normative collecting. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(3), 229237. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.07.063

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • O’Guinn, T. C. , & Faber, R. J. (1989). Compulsive buying: A phenomenological exploration. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 147157. doi:10.1086/209204

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park, L. E. , & Maner, J. K. (2009). Does self-threat promote social connection? The role of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(1), 203217. doi:10.1037/a0013933

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Preacher, K. J. , & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Richins, M. L. (1994). Valuing things: The public and private meanings of possessions. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(3), 504521. doi:10.1086/209414

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ridgway, N. M. , Kukar-Kinney, M. , & Monroe, K. B. (2008). An expanded conceptualization and a new measure of compulsive buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(4), 622639. doi:10.1086/591108

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological Assessment, 8(4), 350353. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350

  • Sheets, V. L. , & Braver, S. L. (1999). Organizational status and perceived sexual harassment: Detecting the mediators of a null effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(9), 11591171. doi:10.1177/01461672992512009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107120. doi:10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steketee, G. , Frost, R. O. , & Kyrios, M. (2003). Cognitive aspects of compulsive hoarding. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(4), 463479. doi:10.1023/A:1025428631552

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stillman, T. F. , Baumeister, R. F. , Lambert, N. M. , Crescioni, A. W. , DeWall, C. N. , & Fincham, F. D. (2009). Alone and without purpose: Life loses meaning following social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 686694. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Timpano, K. R. , Exner, C. , Glaesmer, H. , Rief, W. , Keshaviah, A. , Brähler, E. , & Wilhelm, S. (2011). The epidemiology of the proposed DSM-5 hoarding disorder: Exploration of the acquisition specifier, associated features, and distress. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72(6), 780786. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06380

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Timpano, K. R. , & Shaw, A. M. (2013). Conferring humanness: The role of anthropomorphism in hoarding. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 383388. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tolin, D. F. , Stevens, M. C. , Villavicencio, A. L. , Norberg, M. M. , Calhoun, V. D. , Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , Rauch, S. L. , & Pearlson, G. D. (2012). Neural mechanisms of decision making in hoarding disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(8), 832841. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1980

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Beest, I. , & Williams, K. D. (2006). When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 918928. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.918

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, W. (2017). Smartphones as social actors? Social dispositional factors in assessing anthropomorphism. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 334344. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.022.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Waytz, A. , Morewedge, C. K. , Epley, N. , Monteleone, G. , Gao, J. H. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Making sense by making sentient: Effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 410435. doi:10.1037/a0020240

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, K. D. , Cheung, C. K. , & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748762. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, K. D. , & Zadro, L. (2001). Ostracism: On being ignored, excluded and rejected. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 2153). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wincze, J. P. , Steketee, G. , & Frost, R. O. (2000). Categorization in compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 6372. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zadro, L. , Boland, C. , & Richardson, R. (2006). How long does it last? The persistence of the effects of ostracism in the socially anxious. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 692697. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zadro, L. , Williams, K. D. , & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(4), 560567. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Appendix A: PICTURES OF THE FIVE “GIFTS” USED IN THE STUDY

Citation: Journal of Behavioral Addictions 7, 4; 10.1556/2006.7.2018.98

Appendix B: PRELIMINARY STATISTICS FOR EACH “GIFT”

Table B1.

Means, standard deviations, count and percentages for each gift

Anthropomorphism Instrumental value Sentimental value Object attachment
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
M&Ms dispenser 11.12 (7.13)c 2.97 (1.37)a 3.01 (1.43)a 4.48 (1.78)a
Plant pot with face 14.63 (8.43)a 2.92 (1.37)a 3.51 (1.48)b 4.77 (1.65)b
Salt and pepper shakers 12.25 (7.21)b 3.25 (1.30)b 2.74 (1.39)c 4.41 (1.76)a
Wine bottle holder 11.25 (6.98)c 3.23 (1.26)b 2.80 (1.41)c 3.98 (1.72)c
Tea man infuser 11.71 (7.49)c 3.91 (1.35)c 3.20 (1.48)d 4.97 (1.59)b

Note. Parameter estimates in each column that share subscripts (i.e., a, b, and c) do not differ significantly. M: mean; SD: standard deviation of the mean. Repeated measures ANOVA for anthropomorphism: F(4, 1320) = 39.11, p < .001; instrumental value: F(4, 1320) = 46.45, p < .001, η2p = 0.12, for sentimentality: F(4, 1320) = 32.81, p < .001, η2p = 0.09, and for object attachment: F(4, 1320) = 20.70, p < .001, η2p = 0.06.

  • Aggarwal, P. , & McGill, A. L. (2007). Is that car smiling at me? Schema congruity as a basis for evaluating anthropomorphized products. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(4), 468479. doi:10.1086/518544

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Arndt, J. , Solomon, S. , Kasser, T. , & Sheldon, K. M. (2004). The urge to splurge: A terror management account of materialism and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(3), 198212. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1403_2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bartz, J. A. , Tchalova, K. , & Fenerci, C. (2016). Reminders of social connection can attenuate anthropomorphism: A replication and extension of Epley, Akalis, Waytz, and Cacioppo (2008). Psychological Science, 27(12), 16441650. doi:10.1177/0956797616668510

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baumeister, R. F. , & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(2), 139168. doi:10.1086/209154

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Version 1. New York, NY: Basic Books.

  • Burgess, A. M. , Graves, L. M. , & Frost, R. O. (2018). My possessions need me: Anthropomorphism and hoarding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(3), 340348. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12441

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Norris, C. J. , Decety, J. , Monteleone, G. , & Nusbaum, H. (2009). In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(1), 8392. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, M. S. , Greenberg, A. , Hill, E. , Lemay, E. P. , Clark-Polner, E. , & Roosth, D. (2011). Heightened interpersonal security diminishes the monetary value of possessions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 359364. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.08.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Claus, B. , & Warlop, L. (2011). Once bitten, twice shy: Differences in social efficacy affect the perceived efficacy of anthropomorphizable products. In D. W. Dahl, G. V. Johar, & S. M. J. van Osselaer (Eds.), NA – Advances in consumer research (Vol. 38). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research. Retrieved from https://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/16093/volumes/v38/NA-38

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cortina, J. M. (1993). What is a coefficient alpha? An exanimation of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 98104. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Delbaere, M. , McQuarrie, E. F. , & Phillips, B. J. (2011). Personification in advertising. Journal of Advertising, 40(1), 121130. doi:10.2753/JOA0091-3367400108

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Epley, N. , Akalis, S. , Waytz, A. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Creating social connection through inferential reproduction: Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and greyhounds. Psychological Science, 19(2), 114120. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02056.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Epley, N. , Waytz, A. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114(4), 864886. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.864

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eyssel, F. & Reich, N. (2013). Loneliness makes the heart grow fonder (of robots) – On the effects of loneliness on psychological anthropomorphism. In ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (pp. 121122). Tokyo, Japan: IEEE.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Feng, W. (2016). When lonely people encounter anthropomorphic products. Social Behavior and Personality, 44(10), 16491660. doi:10.2224/sbp.2016.44.10.1649

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fromm, E. (1947). Man for himself: An inquiry into the psychology of ethics (Vol. 57). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

  • Frost, R. O. , & Hartl, T. L. (1996). A cognitive-behavioral model of compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(4), 341350. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00071-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Hartl, T. L. , Christian, R. , & Williams, N. (1995). The value of possessions in compulsive hoarding: Patterns of use and attachment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 897902. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(95)00043-W

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , & Hristova, V. (2011). Assessment of hoarding. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(5), 456466. doi:10.1002/jclp.20790

  • Frost, R. O. , Kyrios, M. , McCarthy, K. D. , & Matthews, Y. (2007). Self-ambivalence and attachment to possessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21(3), 232242. doi:10.1891/088983907781494582

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Rosenfield, E. , Steketee, G. , & Tolin, D. F. (2013). An examination of excessive acquisition in hoarding disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 2(3), 338345. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.06.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , & Steketee, G. (2008). Compulsive hoarding. In J. S. Abramowitz, D. McKay, & S. Taylor (Eds.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Subtypes and spectrum conditions (pp. 7693). New York, NY: Elsevier.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , & Grisham, J. (2004). Measurement of compulsive hoarding: Saving Inventory – Revised. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 11631182. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.07.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , Tolin, D. F. , Sinopoli, N. , & Ruby, D. (2015). Motives for acquiring and saving in hoarding disorder, OCD, and community controls. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 4, 5459. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2014.12.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Frost, R. O. , Tolin, D. F. , Steketee, G. , Fitch, K. E. , & Selbo-Bruns, A. (2009). Excessive acquisition in hoarding. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 632639. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gjersoe, N. L. , Hall, E. L. , & Hood, B. (2015). Children attribute mental lives to toys when they are emotionally attached to them. Cognitive Development, 34, 2838. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2014.12.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grisham, J. R. , Norberg, M. M. , Williams, A. D. , Certoma, S. P. , & Kadib, R. (2010). Categorization and cognitive deficits in compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(9), 866872. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grisham, J. R. , Steketee, G. , & Frost, R. (2008). Interpersonal problems and emotional intelligence in compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety, 25(9), E63E71. doi:10.1002/da.20327

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitude of correlation coefficients. American Psychologist, 58(1), 7879. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.78

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keefer, L. A. (2016). Is there anybody out there? Trait anthropomorphism predicts the psychological benefits of a favorite belonging. Journal of Individual Differences, 37(4), 231238. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000210

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Keefer, L. A. , Landau, M. J. , Rothschild, Z. K. , & Sullivan, D. (2012). Attachment to objects as compensation for close others’ perceived unreliability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 912917. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kyrios, M. , Frost, R. O. , & Steketee, G. (2004). Cognitions in compulsive buying and acquisition. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(2), 241258. doi:10.1023/B:COTR.0000021543.62799.32

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lastovicka, J. L. , & Sirianni, N. J. (2011). Truly, madly, deeply: Consumers in the throes of material possession love. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(2), 323342. doi:10.1086/658338

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, M. R. , & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 162. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Leary, M. R. , Tambor, E. S. , Terdal, S. K. , & Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(3), 518530. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.518

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MacInnis, D. J. , & Folkes, V. S. (2017). Humanizing brands: When brands seem to be like me, part of me, and in a relationship with me. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(3), 355374. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2016.12.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maner, J. K. , DeWall, C. N. , Baumeister, R. F. , & Schaller, M. (2007). Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the porcupine problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(1), 4255. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.42

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Markus, H. R. , & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mead, N. L. , Baumeister, R. F. , Stillman, T. F. , Rawn, C. D. , & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Social exclusion causes people to spend and consume strategically in the service of affiliation. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 902919. doi:10.1086/656667

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Medard, E. , & Kellett, S. (2014). The role of adult attachment and social support in hoarding disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42(5), 629633. doi:10.1017/S1352465813000659

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mikulincer, M. , & Shaver, P. R. (2016). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mogan, C. , Kyrios, M. , Schweitzer, I. , Yap, K. , & Moulding, R. (2012). Phenomenology of hoarding – What is hoarded by individuals with hoarding disorder? Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 1(4), 306311. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.08.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Moulding, R. , Nedeljkovic, M. , Kyrios, M. , Osborne, D. , & Mogan, C. (2017). Short- term cognitive-behavioral group treatment for hoarding disorder: A naturalistic treatment outcome study. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(1), 235244. doi:10.1002/cpp.2001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mourey, J. A. , Olson, J. G. , & Yoon, C. (2017). Products as pals: Engaging with anthropomorphic products mitigates the effects of social exclusion. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(2), 414431. doi:10.1093/jcr/ucx038

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neave, N. , Jackson, R. , Saxton, T. , & Hönekopp, J. (2015). The influence of anthropomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 214219. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.041

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Neave, N. , Tyson, H. , McInnes, L. , & Hamilton, C. (2016). The role of attachment style and anthropomorphism in predicting hoarding behaviors in a non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 3337. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.067

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norberg, M. M. , Crone, C. , Kwok, C. , & Grisham, J. R. (2018). Anxious attachment and excessive acquisition: The mediating roles of anthropomorphism and distress tolerance. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(1), 171180. doi:10.1556/2006.7.2018.08

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Norberg, M. M. , Keyan, D. , & Grisham, J. R. (2015). Mood influences the relationship between distress intolerance and discarding. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 6, 7782. doi:10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.06.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nordsletten, A. E. , de la Cruz, L. F. , Billotti, D. , & Mataix-Cols, D. (2013). Finders keepers: The features differentiating hoarding disorder from normative collecting. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(3), 229237. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.07.063

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • O’Guinn, T. C. , & Faber, R. J. (1989). Compulsive buying: A phenomenological exploration. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 147157. doi:10.1086/209204

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Park, L. E. , & Maner, J. K. (2009). Does self-threat promote social connection? The role of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(1), 203217. doi:10.1037/a0013933

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Preacher, K. J. , & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Richins, M. L. (1994). Valuing things: The public and private meanings of possessions. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(3), 504521. doi:10.1086/209414

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ridgway, N. M. , Kukar-Kinney, M. , & Monroe, K. B. (2008). An expanded conceptualization and a new measure of compulsive buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(4), 622639. doi:10.1086/591108

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmitt, N. (1996). Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychological Assessment, 8(4), 350353. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.350

  • Sheets, V. L. , & Braver, S. L. (1999). Organizational status and perceived sexual harassment: Detecting the mediators of a null effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(9), 11591171. doi:10.1177/01461672992512009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sijtsma, K. (2009). On the use, the misuse, and the very limited usefulness of Cronbach’s alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 107120. doi:10.1007/s11336-008-9101-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Steketee, G. , Frost, R. O. , & Kyrios, M. (2003). Cognitive aspects of compulsive hoarding. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(4), 463479. doi:10.1023/A:1025428631552

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stillman, T. F. , Baumeister, R. F. , Lambert, N. M. , Crescioni, A. W. , DeWall, C. N. , & Fincham, F. D. (2009). Alone and without purpose: Life loses meaning following social exclusion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), 686694. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Timpano, K. R. , Exner, C. , Glaesmer, H. , Rief, W. , Keshaviah, A. , Brähler, E. , & Wilhelm, S. (2011). The epidemiology of the proposed DSM-5 hoarding disorder: Exploration of the acquisition specifier, associated features, and distress. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72(6), 780786. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06380

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Timpano, K. R. , & Shaw, A. M. (2013). Conferring humanness: The role of anthropomorphism in hoarding. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 383388. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tolin, D. F. , Stevens, M. C. , Villavicencio, A. L. , Norberg, M. M. , Calhoun, V. D. , Frost, R. O. , Steketee, G. , Rauch, S. L. , & Pearlson, G. D. (2012). Neural mechanisms of decision making in hoarding disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69(8), 832841. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1980

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Van Beest, I. , & Williams, K. D. (2006). When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 918928. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.918

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wang, W. (2017). Smartphones as social actors? Social dispositional factors in assessing anthropomorphism. Computers in Human Behavior, 68, 334344. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.022.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Waytz, A. , Morewedge, C. K. , Epley, N. , Monteleone, G. , Gao, J. H. , & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Making sense by making sentient: Effectance motivation increases anthropomorphism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 410435. doi:10.1037/a0020240

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, K. D. , Cheung, C. K. , & Choi, W. (2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748762. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, K. D. , & Zadro, L. (2001). Ostracism: On being ignored, excluded and rejected. In M. R. Leary (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 2153). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wincze, J. P. , Steketee, G. , & Frost, R. O. (2000). Categorization in compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 6372. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zadro, L. , Boland, C. , & Richardson, R. (2006). How long does it last? The persistence of the effects of ostracism in the socially anxious. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(5), 692697. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Zadro, L. , Williams, K. D. , & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(4), 560567. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collapse
  • Expand
The author instructions are available in PDF.
Please, download the file from HERE

 

Dr. Zsolt Demetrovics
Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University
Address: Izabella u. 46. H-1064 Budapest, Hungary
Phone: +36-1-461-2681
E-mail: jba@ppk.elte.hu

Indexing and Abstracting Services:

  • Web of Science [Science Citation Index Expanded (also known as SciSearch®)
  • Journal Citation Reports/Science Edition
  • Social Sciences Citation Index®
  • Journal Citation Reports/ Social Sciences Edition
  • Current Contents®/Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • EBSCO
  • GoogleScholar
  • PsycINFO
  • PubMed Central
  • SCOPUS
  • Medline
  • CABI
  • CABELLS Journalytics

2023  
Web of Science  
Journal Impact Factor 6.6
Rank by Impact Factor Q1 (Psychiatry)
Journal Citation Indicator 1.59
Scopus  
CiteScore 12.3
CiteScore rank Q1 (Clinical Psychology)
SNIP 1.604
Scimago  
SJR index 2.188
SJR Q rank Q1

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Publication Model Gold Open Access
Submission Fee none
Article Processing Charge 990 EUR/article for articles submitted after 30 April 2023 (850 EUR for articles submitted prior to this date)
Regional discounts on country of the funding agency World Bank Lower-middle-income economies: 50%
World Bank Low-income economies: 100%
Further Discounts Corresponding authors, affiliated to an EISZ member institution subscribing to the journal package of Akadémiai Kiadó: 100%.
Subscription Information Gold Open Access

Journal of Behavioral Addictions
Language English
Size A4
Year of
Foundation
2011
Volumes
per Year
1
Issues
per Year
4
Founder Eötvös Loránd Tudományegyetem
Founder's
Address
H-1053 Budapest, Hungary Egyetem tér 1-3.
Publisher Akadémiai Kiadó
Publisher's
Address
H-1117 Budapest, Hungary 1516 Budapest, PO Box 245.
Responsible
Publisher
Chief Executive Officer, Akadémiai Kiadó
ISSN 2062-5871 (Print)
ISSN 2063-5303 (Online)

Senior editors

Editor(s)-in-Chief: Zsolt DEMETROVICS

Assistant Editor(s): Csilla ÁGOSTON

Associate Editors

  • Stephanie ANTONS (Universitat Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Joel BILLIEUX (University of Lausanne, Switzerland)
  • Beáta BŐTHE (University of Montreal, Canada)
  • Matthias BRAND (University of Duisburg-Essen, Germany)
  • Ruth J. van HOLST (Amsterdam UMC, The Netherlands)
  • Daniel KING (Flinders University, Australia)
  • Gyöngyi KÖKÖNYEI (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Ludwig KRAUS (IFT Institute for Therapy Research, Germany)
  • Marc N. POTENZA (Yale University, USA)
  • Hans-Jurgen RUMPF (University of Lübeck, Germany)

Editorial Board

  • Sophia ACHAB (Faculty of Medicine, University of Geneva, Switzerland)
  • Alex BALDACCHINO (St Andrews University, United Kingdom)
  • Judit BALÁZS (ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Maria BELLRINGER (Auckland University of Technology, Auckland, New Zealand)
  • Henrietta BOWDEN-JONES (Imperial College, United Kingdom)
  • Damien BREVERS (University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg)
  • Wim VAN DEN BRINK (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Julius BURKAUSKAS (Lithuanian University of Health Sciences, Lithuania)
  • Gerhard BÜHRINGER (Technische Universität Dresden, Germany)
  • Silvia CASALE (University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
  • Luke CLARK (University of British Columbia, Vancouver, B.C., Canada)
  • Jeffrey L. DEREVENSKY (McGill University, Canada)
  • Geert DOM (University of Antwerp, Belgium)
  • Nicki DOWLING (Deakin University, Geelong, Australia)
  • Hamed EKHTIARI (University of Minnesota, United States)
  • Jon ELHAI (University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio, USA)
  • Ana ESTEVEZ (University of Deusto, Spain)
  • Fernando FERNANDEZ-ARANDA (Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Naomi FINEBERG (University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom)
  • Sally GAINSBURY (The University of Sydney, Camperdown, NSW, Australia)
  • Belle GAVRIEL-FRIED (The Bob Shapell School of Social Work, Tel Aviv University, Israel)
  • Biljana GJONESKA (Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Republic of North Macedonia)
  • Marie GRALL-BRONNEC (University Hospital of Nantes, France)
  • Jon E. GRANT (University of Minnesota, USA)
  • Mark GRIFFITHS (Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom)
  • Joshua GRUBBS (University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA)
  • Anneke GOUDRIAAN (University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
  • Susumu HIGUCHI (National Hospital Organization Kurihama Medical and Addiction Center, Japan)
  • David HODGINS (University of Calgary, Canada)
  • Eric HOLLANDER (Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA)
  • Zsolt HORVÁTH (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Susana JIMÉNEZ-MURCIA (Clinical Psychology Unit, Bellvitge University Hospital, Barcelona, Spain)
  • Yasser KHAZAAL (Geneva University Hospital, Switzerland)
  • Orsolya KIRÁLY (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Chih-Hung KO (Faculty of Medicine, College of Medicine, Kaohsiung Medical University, Taiwan)
  • Shane KRAUS (University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA)
  • Hae Kook LEE (The Catholic University of Korea, Republic of Korea)
  • Bernadette KUN (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Katerina LUKAVSKA (Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic)
  • Giovanni MARTINOTTI (‘Gabriele d’Annunzio’ University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy)
  • Gemma MESTRE-BACH (Universidad Internacional de la Rioja, La Rioja, Spain)
  • Astrid MÜLLER (Hannover Medical School, Germany)
  • Daniel Thor OLASON (University of Iceland, Iceland)
  • Ståle PALLESEN (University of Bergen, Norway)
  • Afarin RAHIMI-MOVAGHAR (Teheran University of Medical Sciences, Iran)
  • József RÁCZ (Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary)
  • Michael SCHAUB (University of Zurich, Switzerland)
  • Marcantanio M. SPADA (London South Bank University, United Kingdom)
  • Daniel SPRITZER (Study Group on Technological Addictions, Brazil)
  • Dan J. STEIN (University of Cape Town, South Africa)
  • Sherry H. STEWART (Dalhousie University, Canada)
  • Attila SZABÓ (Eötvös Loránd University, Hungary)
  • Hermano TAVARES (Instituto de Psiquiatria do Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil)
  • Alexander E. VOISKOUNSKY (Moscow State University, Russia)
  • Aviv M. WEINSTEIN (Ariel University, Israel)
  • Anise WU (University of Macau, Macao, China)

 

Monthly Content Usage

Abstract Views Full Text Views PDF Downloads
Mar 2024 0 97 84
Apr 2024 0 141 124
May 2024 0 121 84
Jun 2024 0 104 33
Jul 2024 0 78 37
Aug 2024 0 62 36
Sep 2024 0 18 28